Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2017

01-08-2016 | Original Article

Route planning with transportation network maps: an eye-tracking study

Auteurs: Elise Grison, Valérie Gyselinck, Jean-Marie Burkhardt, Jan Malte Wiener

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Planning routes using transportation network maps is a common task that has received little attention in the literature. Here, we present a novel eye-tracking paradigm to investigate psychological processes and mechanisms involved in such a route planning. In the experiment, participants were first presented with an origin and destination pair before we presented them with fictitious public transportation maps. Their task was to find the connecting route that required the minimum number of transfers. Based on participants’ gaze behaviour, each trial was split into two phases: (1) the search for origin and destination phase, i.e., the initial phase of the trial until participants gazed at both origin and destination at least once and (2) the route planning and selection phase. Comparisons of other eye-tracking measures between these phases and the time to complete them, which depended on the complexity of the planning task, suggest that these two phases are indeed distinct and supported by different cognitive processes. For example, participants spent more time attending the centre of the map during the initial search phase, before directing their attention to connecting stations, where transitions between lines were possible. Our results provide novel insights into the psychological processes involved in route planning from maps. The findings are discussed in relation to the current theories of route planning.
Literatuur
go back to reference Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276–292.CrossRefPubMed Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beck, M. R., Trenchard, M., van Lamsweerde, A., Goldstein, R. R., & Lohrenz, M. (2012). Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1411–1415. doi:10.1177/1071181312561400.CrossRef Beck, M. R., Trenchard, M., van Lamsweerde, A., Goldstein, R. R., & Lohrenz, M. (2012). Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1411–1415. doi:10.​1177/​1071181312561400​.CrossRef
go back to reference Benshoof, J. A. (1970). Characteristics of drivers’ route selection behavior. Traffic Engineering and Control, 11, 604–606. Benshoof, J. A. (1970). Characteristics of drivers’ route selection behavior. Traffic Engineering and Control, 11, 604–606.
go back to reference Bovy, P. H. L., & Stern, E. (1990). Route choice: Wayfinding in transport networks. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 27, 338–339. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-033-4. Bovy, P. H. L., & Stern, E. (1990). Route choice: Wayfinding in transport networks. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 27, 338–339. doi:10.​1007/​978-94-009-033-4.
go back to reference Brunyé, T. T., Andonova, E., Meneghetti, C., Noordzij, M. L., Pazzaglia, F., Wienemann, R., … Taylor, H. A. (2012a). Planning routes around the world: International evidence for southern route preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(4), 297–304. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.05.003.CrossRef Brunyé, T. T., Andonova, E., Meneghetti, C., Noordzij, M. L., Pazzaglia, F., Wienemann, R., … Taylor, H. A. (2012a). Planning routes around the world: International evidence for southern route preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(4), 297–304. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jenvp.​2012.​05.​003.CrossRef
go back to reference Brunyé, T. T., Gagnon, S. A., Waller, D., Hodgson, E., Tower-Richardi, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2012b). Up north and down south: Implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1880–1894. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.663393.CrossRefPubMed Brunyé, T. T., Gagnon, S. A., Waller, D., Hodgson, E., Tower-Richardi, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2012b). Up north and down south: Implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1880–1894. doi:10.​1080/​17470218.​2012.​663393.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brunyé, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 772–787. doi:10.1002/acp.1508.CrossRef Brunyé, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 772–787. doi:10.​1002/​acp.​1508.CrossRef
go back to reference Casakin, H., Barkowsky, T., Klippel, A., & Freksa, C. (2000). Schematic maps as way finding aids. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 54–71). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Casakin, H., Barkowsky, T., Klippel, A., & Freksa, C. (2000). Schematic maps as way finding aids. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 54–71). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference de Condappa, O., & Wiener, J. (2016). Human place and response learning: Navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior. Psychological Research, 80(1), 82–93.CrossRefPubMed de Condappa, O., & Wiener, J. (2016). Human place and response learning: Navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior. Psychological Research, 80(1), 82–93.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Freksa, C. (1999). Spatial aspects of task-specific wayfinding maps: A representation-theoretic perspective. In J. S. Gero & B. Tversky (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning in design (pp. 15–32). Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney. Freksa, C. (1999). Spatial aspects of task-specific wayfinding maps: A representation-theoretic perspective. In J. S. Gero & B. Tversky (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning in design (pp. 15–32). Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney.
go back to reference Gärling, T., Säisä, J., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986). The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the large-scale environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6(4), 261–280. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(86)80001-9.CrossRef Gärling, T., Säisä, J., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986). The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the large-scale environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6(4), 261–280. doi:10.​1016/​S0272-4944(86)80001-9.CrossRef
go back to reference Golledge, R. (1995). Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (COSIT’95). Lecture notes in computer science, no. 988 (pp. 207–222). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Golledge, R. (1995). Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (COSIT’95). Lecture notes in computer science, no. 988 (pp. 207–222). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human cognitive maps and wayfinding. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 1–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human cognitive maps and wayfinding. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 1–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
go back to reference Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A, Jr., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210. doi:10.1037/00961523.25.1.210. Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A, Jr., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210. doi:10.​1037/​00961523.​25.​1.​210.
go back to reference MacGregor, J. N., Chronicle, E. P., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). Convex hull or crossing avoidance? Solution heuristics in the traveling salesperson problem. Memory and Cognition, 32(2), 260–270. doi:10.3758/BF03196857.CrossRefPubMed MacGregor, J. N., Chronicle, E. P., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). Convex hull or crossing avoidance? Solution heuristics in the traveling salesperson problem. Memory and Cognition, 32(2), 260–270. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196857.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (1999). Spatial and contextual factors in human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Perception, 28(11), 1417–1427. doi:10.1068/p2863.CrossRefPubMed MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (1999). Spatial and contextual factors in human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Perception, 28(11), 1417–1427. doi:10.​1068/​p2863.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meilinger, T., Hölscher, C., Büchner, S. J., & Brösamle, M. (2007). How much information do you need? Schematic maps in wayfinding and self localisation. In T. Barkowsky (Ed.), Spatial cognition V—reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 381–400). Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-75666-8_22.CrossRef Meilinger, T., Hölscher, C., Büchner, S. J., & Brösamle, M. (2007). How much information do you need? Schematic maps in wayfinding and self localisation. In T. Barkowsky (Ed.), Spatial cognition V—reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 381–400). Berlin: Springer. doi:10.​1007/​978-3-540-75666-8_​22.CrossRef
go back to reference Raveau, S., Guo, Z., Muñoz, J. C., & Wilson, N. H. M. (2014). A behavioural comparison of route choice on metro networks: Time, transfers, crowding, topology and socio-demographics. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 66, 185–195. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2014.05.010. Raveau, S., Guo, Z., Muñoz, J. C., & Wilson, N. H. M. (2014). A behavioural comparison of route choice on metro networks: Time, transfers, crowding, topology and socio-demographics. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 66, 185–195. doi:10.​1016/​j.​tra.​2014.​05.​010.
go back to reference Roberts, M. J. (2008). Underground maps after Beck (2nd ed.). Harrow Weald: Capital Transport Publishing. Roberts, M. J. (2008). Underground maps after Beck (2nd ed.). Harrow Weald: Capital Transport Publishing.
go back to reference Roberts, M. J., Newton, E. J., Lagattolla, F. D., Hughes, S., & Hasler, M. C. (2013). Objective versus subjective measures of Paris Metro map usability: Investigating traditional octolinear versus all-curves schematics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 71, 363–386. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2012.09.004.CrossRef Roberts, M. J., Newton, E. J., Lagattolla, F. D., Hughes, S., & Hasler, M. C. (2013). Objective versus subjective measures of Paris Metro map usability: Investigating traditional octolinear versus all-curves schematics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 71, 363–386. doi:10.​1016/​j.​ijhcs.​2012.​09.​004.CrossRef
go back to reference Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(5), 840–845. doi:10.3758/BF03194109.CrossRefPubMed Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(5), 840–845. doi:10.​3758/​BF03194109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Orden, K. F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., & Jung, T. P. (2001). Eye activity correlates of workload during a visuospatial memory task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 43(1), 111–121.CrossRef Van Orden, K. F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., & Jung, T. P. (2001). Eye activity correlates of workload during a visuospatial memory task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 43(1), 111–121.CrossRef
go back to reference Vickers, D., Lee, M. D., Dry, M., & Hughes, P. (2003). The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Memory and Cognition, 31(7), 1094–1104. doi:10.3758/BF03196130.CrossRefPubMed Vickers, D., Lee, M. D., Dry, M., & Hughes, P. (2003). The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Memory and Cognition, 31(7), 1094–1104. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wiener, J. M., & Tenbrink, T. (2008). Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Künstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition, 1(08), 18–22. Wiener, J. M., & Tenbrink, T. (2008). Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Künstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition, 1(08), 18–22.
Metagegevens
Titel
Route planning with transportation network maps: an eye-tracking study
Auteurs
Elise Grison
Valérie Gyselinck
Jean-Marie Burkhardt
Jan Malte Wiener
Publicatiedatum
01-08-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0792-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2017

Psychological Research 5/2017 Naar de uitgave