Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2-3/2004

01-04-2004 | Original Article

Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects

Auteur: Wilfried Kunde

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2-3/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Theories assuming an effect-based coding of action predict that motor responses become activated by the perception of the responses’ sensory effects. In accordance with this prediction it was found that responding to a visual target is faster and more accurate when the target is briefly preceded by the visual effect of the required response. Most importantly, this effect-induced response priming was independent of prime perceptibility and it occurred even when the prime was not consciously discriminable. Beyond ruling out alternative interpretations of earlier induction studies in terms of deliberate response biases, this suggests that effect codes evoke their associated motor patterns in a highly automatic manner not affording conscious mediation. The results accord with a functional dissociation between the consciousness-mediated implementation and the consciousness-independent realization of action goals.
Voetnoten
1
Counterbalancing the R-E mapping eliminates potential influences of coding preferences in orthogonal S-R ensembles (e.g., “right” being more closely related to “up” than to “left”, Weeks & Proctor, 1991). Still, a left response that consistently produces, say, an up-pointing arrow might be recoded as being “up” (or vice versa, the up-pointing arrow might be recoded as being “left”). This, however, is not only true for arrows but for any contingent R-E relationship (e.g., a high tone might be recoded as “left” when produced by a left key press or vice versa, Elsner & Hommel, 2001). Note that this is not an alternative account of the present priming effects as much as a rephrasing of the proposed ideo-motor account: Responses and effects become linked with each other bi-directionally, whereby they acquire each others’ properties.
 
2
The procedure by Hautus (1995) for the correction of extreme values in calculating d’ was applied.
 
3
The prime discrimination data of one participant were lost, so this mean is based on 15 participants. The response priming data of this participant were well within the range of the other subjects, and excluding this participant from analysis did not alter the overall data pattern in any significant respect.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beckers, T., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2002). Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: The case of associative affective Simon effect. Psychological Research, 66, 166–173.CrossRefPubMed Beckers, T., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2002). Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: The case of associative affective Simon effect. Psychological Research, 66, 166–173.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240. Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Humphrey, G. K. (1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67, 181–207.PubMed Goodale, M. A., & Humphrey, G. K. (1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67, 181–207.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Timesharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Timesharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 52–57.PubMed
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (2003). On doing two things at once. III. Confirmation of perfect timesharing when simultaneous tasks are ideomotor compatible. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 859–868. Greenwald, A. G. (2003). On doing two things at once. III. Confirmation of perfect timesharing when simultaneous tasks are ideomotor compatible. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 859–868.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 70–76.PubMed Greenwald, A. G., & Shulman, H. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 70–76.PubMed
go back to reference Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73. Harleß, E. (1861). Der Apparat des Willens. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 50–73.
go back to reference Hautus, M. J. (1995). Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d’. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 27, 46–51. Hautus, M. J. (1995). Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d’. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 27, 46–51.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 59, 176–186.PubMed Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 59, 176–186.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 869–937. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 869–937.
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.PubMed Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.PubMed
go back to reference James, W. (1981/1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. James, W. (1981/1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 387–403. Keele, S. W. (1968). Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 387–403.
go back to reference Klapp, S. T., Porter-Graham, K. A., & Hoijfield, A. R. (1991). The relation of perception and motor action: Ideomotor compatibility and interference in divided attention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 155–162. Klapp, S. T., Porter-Graham, K. A., & Hoijfield, A. R. (1991). The relation of perception and motor action: Ideomotor compatibility and interference in divided attention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 155–162.
go back to reference Koch, I., & Kunde, W. (2002). Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory and Cognition, 30, 1297–1303. Koch, I., & Kunde, W. (2002). Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory and Cognition, 30, 1297–1303.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394. Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 387–394.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2003a). Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198–205. Kunde, W. (2003a). Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198–205.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2003b). Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psychological Research, 67, 153–159.PubMed Kunde, W. (2003b). Temporal response-effect compatibility. Psychological Research, 67, 153–159.PubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.CrossRefPubMed Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lien, M-C., Proctor, R. W., & Allen, P. A. (2002). Ideomotor compatibility in the psychological refractory period effect: Twenty-eight years of oversimplification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 396–409. Lien, M-C., Proctor, R. W., & Allen, P. A. (2002). Ideomotor compatibility in the psychological refractory period effect: Twenty-eight years of oversimplification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 396–409.
go back to reference Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder die Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung. Lotze, R. H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder die Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
go back to reference Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.
go back to reference Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In Umilta, C., Moscovitch, M. (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing. Attention and performance series (pp. 123–150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In Umilta, C., Moscovitch, M. (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing. Attention and performance series (pp. 123–150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Öhmann, A., & Soares, J. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69–82. Öhmann, A., & Soares, J. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69–82.
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461.PubMed Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461.PubMed
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260. Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: A study of imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1746–1759.CrossRefPubMed Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: A study of imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1746–1759.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Invariant time-course of priming with and without awareness. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.). Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Invariant time-course of priming with and without awareness. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger, & H. Müller (Eds.). Psychophysics beyond sensation: Laws and invariants of human cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1991). Salient-features coding in the translation between orthogonal stimulus and response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 355–366. Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1991). Salient-features coding in the translation between orthogonal stimulus and response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 355–366.
go back to reference Zießler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2002). Effect anticipation and action planning. In Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Zießler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2002). Effect anticipation and action planning. In Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects
Auteur
Wilfried Kunde
Publicatiedatum
01-04-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2-3/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0147-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2-3/2004

Psychological Research 2-3/2004 Naar de uitgave