Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2008

01-09-2008 | Original Article

Response inhibition under task switching: its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation

Auteurs: Michel D. Druey, Ronald Hübner

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Under task switch conditions, response repetitions usually produce benefits if the task also repeats, but costs if the task switches. So far, it is largely undecided how to account for these effects. In the present study, we provide additional evidence in favor of the account that each response is inhibited in order to prevent its accidental re-execution. To test this hypothesis, the risk of an accidental re-execution of a given response was manipulated by modulating the activation of the response in the previous task. In Experiment 1, this was done by means of congruent and incongruent stimuli. As expected, on task switch trials, the repetition costs were larger if a congruent rather than an incongruent stimulus occurred in the previous task. In Experiment 2, the same effect occurred for stimulus-response compatible versus incompatible stimuli in the previous task. In Experiment 3, both manipulations were applied together, which produced almost additive effects. Altogether, the results support the inhibition account for the response repetition effects under task switch conditions.
Voetnoten
1
Here and in the following, we use the term “trial” even if actually effects within a trial (i.e. from a first to a second task in a dual-task or sequence procedure) were considered.
 
2
For the response times, the effects of congruency and SR-compatibility were additive, as could have been expected from the PDP models mentioned in the introduction (Botvinick et al., 2001; Gilbert & Shallice, 2002; Houghton & Tipper, 1994). With respect to the error rates, however, this was not the case. The reason for this interaction might though simply reflect a floor effect: Given a congruent stimulus, errors are already so infrequent that even if the stimulus was also compatible, this could not further improve performance.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brown, J. W., Reynolds, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2007). A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 37–85.PubMedCrossRef Brown, J. W., Reynolds, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2007). A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 37–85.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Campbell, K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362.CrossRef Campbell, K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362.CrossRef
go back to reference De Jong, R. (2000). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell, J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT. De Jong, R. (2000). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell, J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT.
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.PubMedCrossRef Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.PubMedCrossRef Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Forstmann, B. U., Brass, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Methodological and empirical issues when dissociating cue-related from task-related processes in the explicit task-cuing procedure. Psychological Research, 71, 393–400.PubMedCrossRef Forstmann, B. U., Brass, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Methodological and empirical issues when dissociating cue-related from task-related processes in the explicit task-cuing procedure. Psychological Research, 71, 393–400.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilbert, S. J., & Shallice, T. (2002). Task switching: A PDP model. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 297–337.PubMedCrossRef Gilbert, S. J., & Shallice, T. (2002). Task switching: A PDP model. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 297–337.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–775.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–775.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach, T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic. Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach, T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic.
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2007). Multiple response codes play specific roles in response selection and inhibition under task shifting. Psychological Research. (in press). Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2007). Multiple response codes play specific roles in response selection and inhibition under task shifting. Psychological Research. (in press).
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362–372.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362–372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef Meiran, N. (2000a). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell, J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge: MIT. Meiran, N. (2000b). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell, J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 377–399). Cambridge: MIT.
go back to reference Meiran N. (2005). Task rule-congruency and Simon-like effects in switching between spatial tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1023–1041. Meiran N. (2005). Task rule-congruency and Simon-like effects in switching between spatial tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1023–1041.
go back to reference Meiran N., Chorev Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 211–253.PubMedCrossRef Meiran N., Chorev Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 211–253.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Notebaert W., & Soetens, E. (2003). The influence of irrelevant stimulus changes on stimulus and response repetition effects. Acta Psychologica, 112, 143–156.PubMedCrossRef Notebaert W., & Soetens, E. (2003). The influence of irrelevant stimulus changes on stimulus and response repetition effects. Acta Psychologica, 112, 143–156.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Notebaert W., Soetens E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task––evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological Research, 65, 170–184.PubMedCrossRef Notebaert W., Soetens E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task––evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological Research, 65, 170–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: 2. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33–48.CrossRef Pashler H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: 2. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (1997). A dual-route processing architecture for stimulus-response correspondence effects. In B. Hommel, W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 119–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R. (1997). A dual-route processing architecture for stimulus-response correspondence effects. In B. Hommel, W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 119–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, M. C. (1968). Repetition effect and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 435–439.PubMedCrossRef Smith, M. C. (1968). Repetition effect and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 435–439.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soetens, E. (1998). Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 547–568.CrossRef Soetens, E. (1998). Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 547–568.CrossRef
go back to reference Soetens, E., Melis, A., & Notebaert, W. (2004). Sequence learning and sequential effects. Psychological Research, 69, 124–137.PubMedCrossRef Soetens, E., Melis, A., & Notebaert, W. (2004). Sequence learning and sequential effects. Psychological Research, 69, 124–137.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2006). Response-based strengthening in task shifting: Evidence from shift effects produced by errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 517–534.PubMedCrossRef Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2006). Response-based strengthening in task shifting: Evidence from shift effects produced by errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 517–534.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schroeter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363.PubMedCrossRef Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schroeter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Seiss, E., & Leuthold, H. (2005). Executive control in the Simon task: A dual-task examination of response priming and its suppression. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 590–618.CrossRef Stürmer, B., Seiss, E., & Leuthold, H. (2005). Executive control in the Simon task: A dual-task examination of response priming and its suppression. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 590–618.CrossRef
go back to reference Wühr, P., & Ansorge, U. (2005). Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 705–731. Wühr, P., & Ansorge, U. (2005). Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 705–731.
Metagegevens
Titel
Response inhibition under task switching: its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation
Auteurs
Michel D. Druey
Ronald Hübner
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0127-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2008

Psychological Research 5/2008 Naar de uitgave