Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2012

01-07-2012 | Original Article

Representing others’ actions: the role of expertise in the aging mind

Auteurs: Nadine Diersch, Emily S. Cross, Waltraud Stadler, Simone Schütz-Bosbach, Martina Rieger

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A large body of evidence suggests that action execution and action observation share a common representational domain. To date, little is known about age-related changes in these action representations that are assumed to support various abilities such as the prediction of observed actions. The purpose of the present study was to investigate (a) how age affects the ability to predict the time course of observed actions; and (b) whether and to what extent sensorimotor expertise attenuates age-related declines in prediction performance. In a first experiment, older adults predicted the time course of familiar everyday actions less precisely than younger adults. In a second experiment, younger and older figure skating experts as well as age-matched novices were asked to predict the time course of figure skating elements and simple movement exercises. Both young age and sensorimotor expertise had a positive influence on prediction performance of figure skating elements. The expertise-related benefit did not show a transfer to movement exercises. Together, the results suggest a specific decline of action representations in the aging mind. However, extensive sensorimotor experience seems to enable experts to represent actions from their domain of expertise more precisely even in older age.
Literatuur
go back to reference Abernethy, B., & Zawi, K. (2007). Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39(5), 353–367.PubMedCrossRef Abernethy, B., & Zawi, K. (2007). Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39(5), 353–367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 1109–1116.PubMedCrossRef Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 1109–1116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1993). The Spot-the-Word test: A robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 55–65.PubMedCrossRef Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1993). The Spot-the-Word test: A robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 55–65.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507.PubMedCrossRef Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bennett, P. J., Sekuler, R., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification. Vision Research, 47(6), 799–809.PubMedCrossRef Bennett, P. J., Sekuler, R., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification. Vision Research, 47(6), 799–809.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.PubMedCrossRef Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905–1910.PubMedCrossRef Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905–1910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1148–1167.PubMedCrossRef Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1148–1167.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Celnik, P., Stefan, K., Hummel, F., Duque, J., Classen, J., & Cohen, L. G. (2006). Encoding a motor memory in the older adult by action observation. Neuroimage, 29(2), 677–684.PubMedCrossRef Celnik, P., Stefan, K., Hummel, F., Duque, J., Classen, J., & Cohen, L. G. (2006). Encoding a motor memory in the older adult by action observation. Neuroimage, 29(2), 677–684.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Colcombe, S. J., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125–130.PubMedCrossRef Colcombe, S. J., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cotman, C. W., & Berchtold, N. C. (2002). Exercise: A behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences, 25(6), 295–301.PubMedCrossRef Cotman, C. W., & Berchtold, N. C. (2002). Exercise: A behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences, 25(6), 295–301.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1257–1267.PubMedCrossRef Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1257–1267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Parkinson, J., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2011). The influence of visual training on predicting complex action sequences. Human Brain Mapping, doi:10.1002/hbm.21450. Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Parkinson, J., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2011). The influence of visual training on predicting complex action sequences. Human Brain Mapping, doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​21450.
go back to reference Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior–anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209.PubMedCrossRef Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior–anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Vignemont, F., & Haggard, P. (2008). Action observation and execution: What is shared? Social Neuroscience, 3(3), 421–433.PubMedCrossRef de Vignemont, F., & Haggard, P. (2008). Action observation and execution: What is shared? Social Neuroscience, 3(3), 421–433.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed. ed., pp. 1–54). New York: Psychology Press. Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed. ed., pp. 1–54). New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception—action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32(9), 1127–1139.PubMedCrossRef Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception—action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32(9), 1127–1139.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.PubMedCrossRef Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gescheider, G. A. (1997). The classical psychophysical methods. In G. A. Gescheider (Ed.), Psychophysics: The fundamentals (3rd ed. ed., pp. 45–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gescheider, G. A. (1997). The classical psychophysical methods. In G. A. Gescheider (Ed.), Psychophysics: The fundamentals (3rd ed. ed., pp. 45–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36(Supplement 2), T22–T32.PubMedCrossRef Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36(Supplement 2), T22–T32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grafton, S. T. (2009). Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 97–117.PubMedCrossRef Grafton, S. T. (2009). Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 97–117.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuninckx, S., Wenderoth, N., Debaere, F., Peeters, R., & Swinnen, S. P. (2005). Neural basis of aging: The penetration of cognition into action control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(29), 6787–6796.PubMedCrossRef Heuninckx, S., Wenderoth, N., Debaere, F., Peeters, R., & Swinnen, S. P. (2005). Neural basis of aging: The penetration of cognition into action control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(29), 6787–6796.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuninckx, S., Wenderoth, N., & Swinnen, S. P. (2008). Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: Recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(1), 91–99.PubMedCrossRef Heuninckx, S., Wenderoth, N., & Swinnen, S. P. (2008). Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: Recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(1), 91–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.PubMedCrossRef Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Muesseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(05), 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Muesseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(05), 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Horton, S., Baker, J., & Schorer, J. (2008). Expertise and aging: Maintaining skills through the lifespan. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 5(2), 89–96.CrossRef Horton, S., Baker, J., & Schorer, J. (2008). Expertise and aging: Maintaining skills through the lifespan. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 5(2), 89–96.CrossRef
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PloS Biology, 3(3), 529–535.CrossRef Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PloS Biology, 3(3), 529–535.CrossRef
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14(1), 103–109.CrossRef Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14(1), 103–109.CrossRef
go back to reference Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166.PubMedCrossRef Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., & Greenough, W. T. (2004). Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. Journals of Gerontology Series A—Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(9), 940–957.CrossRef Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., & Greenough, W. T. (2004). Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. Journals of Gerontology Series A—Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(9), 940–957.CrossRef
go back to reference Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 769–776.PubMedCrossRef Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 769–776.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., & Charness, N. (2006). Aging and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 723–742). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Krampe, R. T., & Charness, N. (2006). Aging and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 723–742). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(4), 331–359.CrossRef Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(4), 331–359.CrossRef
go back to reference Léonard, G., & Tremblay, F. (2007). Corticomotor facilitation associated with observation, imagery and imitation of hand actions: A comparative study in young and old adults. Experimental Brain Research, 177(2), 167–175.CrossRef Léonard, G., & Tremblay, F. (2007). Corticomotor facilitation associated with observation, imagery and imitation of hand actions: A comparative study in young and old adults. Experimental Brain Research, 177(2), 167–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Mann, D. T. Y., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Janelle, C. M. (2007). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29(4), 457–478.PubMed Mann, D. T. Y., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Janelle, C. M. (2007). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29(4), 457–478.PubMed
go back to reference Maryott, J., & Sekuler, R. (2009). Age-related changes in imitating sequences of observed movements. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 476–486.PubMedCrossRef Maryott, J., & Sekuler, R. (2009). Age-related changes in imitating sequences of observed movements. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 476–486.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., Tessitore, A., Hariri, A. R., Das, S., Callicott, J. H., et al. (2002). Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology, 58(4), 630–635.PubMedCrossRef Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., Tessitore, A., Hariri, A. R., Das, S., Callicott, J. H., et al. (2002). Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology, 58(4), 630–635.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McHorney, C. A., Ware, J. E., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247–263.PubMedCrossRef McHorney, C. A., Ware, J. E., & Raczek, A. E. (1993). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measuring physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care, 31(3), 247–263.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mulder, T., Hochstenbach, J. B. H., van Heuvelen, M. J. G., & den Otter, A. R. (2007). Motor imagery: The relation between age and imagery capacity. Human Movement Science, 26(2), 203–211.PubMedCrossRef Mulder, T., Hochstenbach, J. B. H., van Heuvelen, M. J. G., & den Otter, A. R. (2007). Motor imagery: The relation between age and imagery capacity. Human Movement Science, 26(2), 203–211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müller, S., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2006). How do world-class cricket batsmen anticipate a bowler’s intention? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2162–2186.PubMedCrossRef Müller, S., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2006). How do world-class cricket batsmen anticipate a bowler’s intention? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2162–2186.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nedelko, V., Hassa, T., Hamzei, F., Weiller, C., Binkofski, F., Schoenfeld, M. A. l, et al. (2010). Age-independent activation in areas of the mirror neuron system during action observation and action imagery. A fMRI study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28(6), 737–747.PubMed Nedelko, V., Hassa, T., Hamzei, F., Weiller, C., Binkofski, F., Schoenfeld, M. A. l, et al. (2010). Age-independent activation in areas of the mirror neuron system during action observation and action imagery. A fMRI study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28(6), 737–747.PubMed
go back to reference Norman, J. F., Payton, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging and the perception of biological motion. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 219–225.PubMedCrossRef Norman, J. F., Payton, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging and the perception of biological motion. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 219–225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRef Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320.PubMedCrossRef Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173–196.PubMedCrossRef Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173–196.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perrett, D. I., Xiao, D. K., Barraclough, N. E., Keysers, C., & Oram, M. W. (2009). Seeing the future: Natural image sequences produce “anticipatory” neuronal activity and bias perceptual report. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(11), 2081–2104.PubMedCrossRef Perrett, D. I., Xiao, D. K., Barraclough, N. E., Keysers, C., & Oram, M. W. (2009). Seeing the future: Natural image sequences produce “anticipatory” neuronal activity and bias perceptual report. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(11), 2081–2104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Personnier, P., Kubicki, A., Laroche, D., & Papaxanthis, C. (2010). Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging. Neuroscience Letters, 476(3), 146–149.PubMedCrossRef Personnier, P., Kubicki, A., Laroche, D., & Papaxanthis, C. (2010). Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging. Neuroscience Letters, 476(3), 146–149.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Personnier, P., Paizis, C., Ballay, Y., & Papaxanthis, C. (2008). Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: II. The influence of the gravito-inertial context on the duration of overt and covert arm movements. Behavioural Brain Research, 186(2), 273–283.PubMedCrossRef Personnier, P., Paizis, C., Ballay, Y., & Papaxanthis, C. (2008). Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: II. The influence of the gravito-inertial context on the duration of overt and covert arm movements. Behavioural Brain Research, 186(2), 273–283.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pilz, K. S., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (2010). Effects of aging on biological motion discrimination. Vision Research, 50(2), 211–219.PubMedCrossRef Pilz, K. S., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (2010). Effects of aging on biological motion discrimination. Vision Research, 50(2), 211–219.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W., & Rapinett, G. (2008). Filling the gap: Dynamic representation of occluded action. In F. Morganti, A. Carassa, & G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective on the study of interactions (pp. 223–236). Amsterdam: IOS Press. Prinz, W., & Rapinett, G. (2008). Filling the gap: Dynamic representation of occluded action. In F. Morganti, A. Carassa, & G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective on the study of interactions (pp. 223–236). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
go back to reference Raz, N., & Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 730–748.PubMedCrossRef Raz, N., & Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 730–748.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roudaia, E., Bennett, P. J., Sekuler, A. B., & Pilz, K. S. (2010). Spatiotemporal properties of apparent motion perception and aging. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 1–15.CrossRef Roudaia, E., Bennett, P. J., Sekuler, A. B., & Pilz, K. S. (2010). Spatiotemporal properties of apparent motion perception and aging. Journal of Vision, 10(14), 1–15.CrossRef
go back to reference Saimpont, A., Pozzo, T., & Papaxanthis, C. (2009). Aging affects the mental rotation of left and right hands. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6714.PubMedCrossRef Saimpont, A., Pozzo, T., & Papaxanthis, C. (2009). Aging affects the mental rotation of left and right hands. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6714.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “Use it or lose It” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 68–87.CrossRef Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “Use it or lose It” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 68–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Schippers, M. B., & Keysers, C. (2011). Mapping the flow of information within the putative mirror neuron system during gesture observation. Neuroimage, 57(1), 37–44.PubMedCrossRef Schippers, M. B., & Keysers, C. (2011). Mapping the flow of information within the putative mirror neuron system during gesture observation. Neuroimage, 57(1), 37–44.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schorer, J., & Baker, J. (2009). An exploratory study of aging and perceptual-motor expertise in handball goalkeepers. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef Schorer, J., & Baker, J. (2009). An exploratory study of aging and perceptual-motor expertise in handball goalkeepers. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Prospective coding in event representation. Cognitive Processing, 8(2), 93–102.PubMedCrossRef Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Prospective coding in event representation. Cognitive Processing, 8(2), 93–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(1), 170–175.PubMedCrossRef Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(1), 170–175.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seidler, R. D., Bangert, A. S., Anguera, J. A., & Quinn-Walsh, C. M. (2007). Motor Control. In J. E. Birren (Ed.), Encyclopedia of gerontology: Age, aging and the aged (2nd ed., Vol. 2 (pp. 228–235). New York: Elsevier. Seidler, R. D., Bangert, A. S., Anguera, J. A., & Quinn-Walsh, C. M. (2007). Motor Control. In J. E. Birren (Ed.), Encyclopedia of gerontology: Age, aging and the aged (2nd ed., Vol. 2 (pp. 228–235). New York: Elsevier.
go back to reference Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 721–733.PubMedCrossRef Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 721–733.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Skoura, X., Papaxanthis, C., Vinter, A., & Pozzo, T. (2005). Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: I. Age effects on the temporal features of overt and covert execution of actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 165(2), 229–239.PubMedCrossRef Skoura, X., Papaxanthis, C., Vinter, A., & Pozzo, T. (2005). Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: I. Age effects on the temporal features of overt and covert execution of actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 165(2), 229–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Skoura, X., Personnier, P., Vinter, A., Pozzo, T., & Papaxanthis, C. (2008). Decline in motor prediction in elderly subjects: Right versus left arm differences in mentally simulated motor actions. Cortex, 44(9), 1271–1278.PubMedCrossRef Skoura, X., Personnier, P., Vinter, A., Pozzo, T., & Papaxanthis, C. (2008). Decline in motor prediction in elderly subjects: Right versus left arm differences in mentally simulated motor actions. Cortex, 44(9), 1271–1278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Snowden, R. J., & Kavanagh, E. (2006). Motion perception in the ageing visual system: Minimum motion, motion coherence, and speed discrimination thresholds. Perception, 35(1), 9–24.PubMedCrossRef Snowden, R. J., & Kavanagh, E. (2006). Motion perception in the ageing visual system: Minimum motion, motion coherence, and speed discrimination thresholds. Perception, 35(1), 9–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sparenberg, P., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting others’ actions: Evidence for a constant time delay in action simulation. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0321-z. Sparenberg, P., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting others’ actions: Evidence for a constant time delay in action simulation. Psychological Research. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-011-0321-z.
go back to reference Stadler, W., Schubotz, R. I., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., Graf, M., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement. Human Brain Mapping, 32(5), 677–687.PubMedCrossRef Stadler, W., Schubotz, R. I., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., Graf, M., & Prinz, W. (2011). Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement. Human Brain Mapping, 32(5), 677–687.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Urgesi, C., Maieron, M., Avenanti, A., Tidoni, E., Fabbro, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2511–2521.PubMedCrossRef Urgesi, C., Maieron, M., Avenanti, A., Tidoni, E., Fabbro, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2511–2521.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Urgesi, C., Savonitto, M. M., Fabbro, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2011). Long- and short-term plastic modeling of action prediction abilities in volleyball. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0383-y. Urgesi, C., Savonitto, M. M., Fabbro, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2011). Long- and short-term plastic modeling of action prediction abilities in volleyball. Psychological Research. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-011-0383-y.​
go back to reference Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. Neuroimage, 48(3), 564–584.PubMedCrossRef Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. Neuroimage, 48(3), 564–584.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., Bayliss, M. S., McHorney, C. A., Rogers, W. H., & Raczek, A. E. (1995). Comparison of methods for the scoring and statistical analysis of SF-36 health profile and summary measures: Summary of results from the medical outcomes study. Medical Care, 33(4), AS264–AS279.PubMed Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., Bayliss, M. S., McHorney, C. A., Rogers, W. H., & Raczek, A. E. (1995). Comparison of methods for the scoring and statistical analysis of SF-36 health profile and summary measures: Summary of results from the medical outcomes study. Medical Care, 33(4), AS264–AS279.PubMed
go back to reference Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.PubMedCrossRef Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473–483.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593–602.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593–602.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11(18), 729–732.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11(18), 729–732.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11(7–8), 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11(7–8), 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Representing others’ actions: the role of expertise in the aging mind
Auteurs
Nadine Diersch
Emily S. Cross
Waltraud Stadler
Simone Schütz-Bosbach
Martina Rieger
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0404-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2012

Psychological Research 4/2012 Naar de uitgave