Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2012

01-11-2012 | Original Article

Representation of grasp postures and anticipatory motor planning in children

Auteurs: Tino Stöckel, Charmayne M. L. Hughes, Thomas Schack

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In this study, we investigated anticipatory motor planning and the development of cognitive representation of grasp postures in children aged 7, 8, and 9 years. Overall, 9-year-old children were more likely to plan their movements to end in comfortable postures, and have distinct representational structures of certain grasp postures, compared to the 7- and 8-year old children. Additionally, the sensitivity toward comfortable end-states (end-state comfort) was related to the mental representation of certain grasp postures. Children with grasp comfort related and functionally well-structured representations were more likely to have satisfied end-state comfort in both the simple and the advanced planning condition. In contrast, end-state comfort satisfaction for the advanced planning condition was much lower for children whose cognitive representations were not structured by grasp comfort. The results of the present study support the notion that cognitive action representation plays an important role in the planning and control of grasp postures.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Based on previous research demonstrating that grasping behaviour in the bar transport task is relatively consistent between trials (e.g., Thibaut and Toussaint, 2010; Weigelt and Schack, 2010) and that behaviour should be similar irrespective of initial bar orientation (black end oriented to the right, black end oriented to the left), we used two trials per condition to test for end-state comfort sensitivity.
 
2
See a detailed description of the SDA-M in the electronic supplementary material.
 
3
Although the majority of children clustered the pictures with ambiguous grasps (pictures 2, 4, 6) with the comfortable grasps (pictures 1, 3, 5), they were not used in the classification of group affiliation.
 
4
We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out this possibility.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Adalbjornsson, C. F., Fischman, M. G., & Rudisill, M. E. (2008). The end-state comfort effect in young children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79, 36–41.CrossRefPubMed Adalbjornsson, C. F., Fischman, M. G., & Rudisill, M. E. (2008). The end-state comfort effect in young children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79, 36–41.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Blaesing, B., Schack, T., & Brugger, P. (2009). The functional architecture of the human body: Assessing body representation by sorting body parts and activities. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 119–129.CrossRef Blaesing, B., Schack, T., & Brugger, P. (2009). The functional architecture of the human body: Assessing body representation by sorting body parts and activities. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 119–129.CrossRef
go back to reference Braun, S. M., Beurskens, A. J. H. M., Schack, T., Marcellis, R. G., Oti, K. C., Schols, J. M., et al. (2007). Is it possible to use the SDA-M to investigate representations of motor actions in stroke patients? Clinical Rehabilitation, 21, 822–832.CrossRefPubMed Braun, S. M., Beurskens, A. J. H. M., Schack, T., Marcellis, R. G., Oti, K. C., Schols, J. M., et al. (2007). Is it possible to use the SDA-M to investigate representations of motor actions in stroke patients? Clinical Rehabilitation, 21, 822–832.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Claxton, L. J., Keen, R., & McCarty, M. E. (2003). Evidence of motor planning in infant reaching behavior. Psychological Science, 14, 354–356.CrossRefPubMed Claxton, L. J., Keen, R., & McCarty, M. E. (2003). Evidence of motor planning in infant reaching behavior. Psychological Science, 14, 354–356.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage, 6, 231–236.CrossRefPubMed Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage, 6, 231–236.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.CrossRefPubMed Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Herbort, O., & Butz, M. V. (2011). Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 371–382.CrossRefPubMed Herbort, O., & Butz, M. V. (2011). Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 371–382.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., & Franz, E. A. (2008). Goal-related planning constraints in bimanual grasping and placing of objects. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 541–550.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M. L., & Franz, E. A. (2008). Goal-related planning constraints in bimanual grasping and placing of objects. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 541–550.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M. L., Haddad, J. M., Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Ryu, J. H. (2011a). Physically coupling two objects in a bimanual task alters kinematics but not end-state comfort. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 219–229.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M. L., Haddad, J. M., Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., & Ryu, J. H. (2011a). Physically coupling two objects in a bimanual task alters kinematics but not end-state comfort. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 219–229.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Johnson-Frey, S. H., McCarty, M., & Keen, R. (2004). Reaching beyond spatial perception: effects of intended future actions on visually-guided prehension. Visual Cognition, 11, 371–399.CrossRef Johnson-Frey, S. H., McCarty, M., & Keen, R. (2004). Reaching beyond spatial perception: effects of intended future actions on visually-guided prehension. Visual Cognition, 11, 371–399.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J. P., Stolze, H., Jöhnk, K., Boczek-Funcke, A., & Illert, M. (1998). Development of prehension movements in children: A kinematic study. Experimental Brian Research, 122, 424–432.CrossRef Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J. P., Stolze, H., Jöhnk, K., Boczek-Funcke, A., & Illert, M. (1998). Development of prehension movements in children: A kinematic study. Experimental Brian Research, 122, 424–432.CrossRef
go back to reference Manoel, E. J., & Moreira, C. R. P. (2005). Planning manipulative hand movements: Do young children show the end-state comfort effect? Journal of Human Movement Studies, 49, 93–114. Manoel, E. J., & Moreira, C. R. P. (2005). Planning manipulative hand movements: Do young children show the end-state comfort effect? Journal of Human Movement Studies, 49, 93–114.
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). Assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). Assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rand, W. M. (1971). Objective criteria for the evaluation of clustering methods. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 66, 846–580.CrossRef Rand, W. M. (1971). Objective criteria for the evaluation of clustering methods. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 66, 846–580.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J. D., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J. D., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schack, T. (in press). A method for measuring mental representation. handbook of measurement in sport. In G. Tenenbaum, R.C. Eklund, & A. Kamata (Eds.), Handbook of measurement in sport and exercise psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Schack, T. (in press). A method for measuring mental representation. handbook of measurement in sport. In G. Tenenbaum, R.C. Eklund, & A. Kamata (Eds.), Handbook of measurement in sport and exercise psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. (2006). Representation of motor skills in human long-term memory. Neuroscience Letters, 391, 77–81.CrossRefPubMed Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. (2006). Representation of motor skills in human long-term memory. Neuroscience Letters, 391, 77–81.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schack, T., & Ritter, H. (2009). The cognitive nature of action-functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science and robotics. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 231–252.CrossRefPubMed Schack, T., & Ritter, H. (2009). The cognitive nature of action-functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science and robotics. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 231–252.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Short, M. W., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1997). Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control: End-state comfort and point-of-change effects. Acta Psychologica, 96, 133–147.CrossRefPubMed Short, M. W., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1997). Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control: End-state comfort and point-of-change effects. Acta Psychologica, 96, 133–147.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thibaut, J. P., & Toussaint, L. (2010). Developing motor planning over ages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 116–129.CrossRefPubMed Thibaut, J. P., & Toussaint, L. (2010). Developing motor planning over ages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 116–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed
go back to reference Von Hofsten, C. (1993). Prospective control: A basic aspect of action development. Human Development, 36, 253–270.CrossRef Von Hofsten, C. (1993). Prospective control: A basic aspect of action development. Human Development, 36, 253–270.CrossRef
go back to reference von Hofsten, C., & Ronnqvist, L. (1988). Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 610–621. von Hofsten, C., & Ronnqvist, L. (1988). Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 610–621.
go back to reference Weigelt, M., Ahlmeyer, T., Lex, H., & Schack, T. (2011). The cognitive representation of a throwing technique in judo experts—Technological ways for individual skill diagnostics in high-performance sports. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 231–235.CrossRef Weigelt, M., Ahlmeyer, T., Lex, H., & Schack, T. (2011). The cognitive representation of a throwing technique in judo experts—Technological ways for individual skill diagnostics in high-performance sports. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 231–235.CrossRef
go back to reference Weigelt, M., Kunde, W., & Prinz, W. (2006). End-state comfort in bimanual object manipulation. Experimental Psychology, 53, 143–148.CrossRefPubMed Weigelt, M., Kunde, W., & Prinz, W. (2006). End-state comfort in bimanual object manipulation. Experimental Psychology, 53, 143–148.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Hülshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132, 68–79.CrossRefPubMed Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Hülshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132, 68–79.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weigelt, M., & Schack, T. (2010). The development of end-state comfort planning in preschool children. Experimental Psychology, 57, 476–482.CrossRefPubMed Weigelt, M., & Schack, T. (2010). The development of end-state comfort planning in preschool children. Experimental Psychology, 57, 476–482.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Representation of grasp postures and anticipatory motor planning in children
Auteurs
Tino Stöckel
Charmayne M. L. Hughes
Thomas Schack
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0387-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2012

Psychological Research 6/2012 Naar de uitgave