Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2007

01-01-2007 | Original Article

Repetition costs in word identification: evaluating a stimulus–response integration account

Auteurs: Bruce Milliken, Juan Lupianez

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Stimulus repetition facilitates performance in many experimental contexts. However, an episodic approach to interpreting repetition effects suggests that repetition effects should depend on how stimuli are encoded. In Experiments 1–3, stimulus repetition in a word identification task led to a cost rather than a benefit in performance, but only when the prime was presented for a relatively long duration. One account of these results is that long prime durations allow integration between stimulus and response codes to occur, which in turn can interfere with responding to a following identical target. In Experiment 4, a stimulus intensity based episodic specificity effect that was insensitive to the proportion of repeated trials supported this stimulus–response integration account.
Voetnoten
1
Although the fact that no errors occurred on identity match trials might raise a concern that repetition costs in RT are caused by a speed–accuracy trade-off, we think this is unlikely to be the case. In particular, note that errors were most frequent for the shortest ISIs, and we noted that almost all of the errors occurred when participants named the prime word rather than the target word, perhaps anticipating an identity match trial. If these prime perseveration ‘errors’ occurred on identity match trials, they would have been indistinguishable from correct responses and therefore not coded as errors. As such, the tendency for participants to make a very occasional error on mismatch trials but not on match trials does not undermine the RT results on which we have based our theoretical discussion.
 
2
We thank Mike Wendt for pointing out this issue.
 
3
Francis and Milliken (2003) also report a prime duration based change in repetition effect, but in a task requiring line length judgments. Longer duration primes produced larger repetition costs, as in the present study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allport, D. A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus–response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Allport, D. A., & Wylie, G. (2000). Task switching, stimulus–response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Bowers, J. S. (2000). In defense of abstractionist theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 83–99. Bowers, J. S. (2000). In defense of abstractionist theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 83–99.
go back to reference Brooks, L. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. E. Roschm, & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brooks, L. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. E. Roschm, & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Dennis, I., & Schmidt, K. (2003). Associative processes in repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 532–538.PubMedCrossRef Dennis, I., & Schmidt, K. (2003). Associative processes in repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 532–538.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Deschepper, B., & Triesman, A. (1996). Visual memory for novel shapes: Implicit coding without attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 27–47.PubMedCrossRef Deschepper, B., & Triesman, A. (1996). Visual memory for novel shapes: Implicit coding without attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 27–47.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fox, E., & deFockert, J. (2001). Inhibitory effects of repeating color and shape: Inhibition of return or repetition blindness? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 798–812.CrossRefPubMed Fox, E., & deFockert, J. (2001). Inhibitory effects of repeating color and shape: Inhibition of return or repetition blindness? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 798–812.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Francis, L., & Milliken, B. (2003). Inhibition of return for the length of a line? Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1208–1221. Francis, L., & Milliken, B. (2003). Inhibition of return for the length of a line? Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1208–1221.
go back to reference Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2005). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action). Hommel, B. (2005). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action).
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11, 483–521.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11, 483–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1 – 17.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1 – 17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jacoby, L., & Brooks, L. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception, and concept learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 18. Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–47). New York: Academic. Jacoby, L., & Brooks, L. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception, and concept learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 18. Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–47). New York: Academic.
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2003). Sequential modulations of stimulus–response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198 – 205. Kunde, W. (2003). Sequential modulations of stimulus–response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198 – 205.
go back to reference Law, M. B., Pratt, J., & Abrams, R. A. (1995). Color-based inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 402–408. Law, M. B., Pratt, J., & Abrams, R. A. (1995). Color-based inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 402–408.
go back to reference Leboe, J., Whittlesea, B., & Milliken, B. (2005). Selective and non-selective transfer: Positive and negative priming in a multiple task environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory. and Cognition (in press). Leboe, J., Whittlesea, B., & Milliken, B. (2005). Selective and non-selective transfer: Positive and negative priming in a multiple task environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory. and Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Lupiáñez, J., Ruz, M., Funes, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2005). The manifestation of attentional capture: facilitation or IOR depending on task demands. Psychological Research (Special Issue on: Integration in and across perception and action). Lupiáñez, J., Ruz, M., Funes, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2005). The manifestation of attentional capture: facilitation or IOR depending on task demands. Psychological Research (Special Issue on: Integration in and across perception and action).
go back to reference Malley, G. B., & Strayer, D. L. (1995). Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 657–667. Malley, G. B., & Strayer, D. L. (1995). Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 657–667.
go back to reference Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 777–787.CrossRefPubMed Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 777–787.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Bryck, R. L. (2005). Low-level constraints on high-level selection: Switching tasks with and without switching stimulus/response objects. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action). Mayr, U., & Bryck, R. L. (2005). Low-level constraints on high-level selection: Switching tasks with and without switching stimulus/response objects. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action).
go back to reference Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85 (3), 207–238.CrossRef Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85 (3), 207–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P., & Seiffert, A. (1998). Selective attention: a re-evaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, 203–229.PubMedCrossRef Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P., & Seiffert, A. (1998). Selective attention: a re-evaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, 203–229.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533.CrossRef Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533.CrossRef
go back to reference Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.CrossRef Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861 – 872.CrossRefPubMed Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861 – 872.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Neill, W. T., & Mathis, K. M. (1998). Transfer-inappropriate processing: Negative priming and related phenomena. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 38. Advances in research and theory (pp.1–44). San Diego, CA: Academic. Neill, W. T., & Mathis, K. M. (1998). Transfer-inappropriate processing: Negative priming and related phenomena. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 38. Advances in research and theory (pp.1–44). San Diego, CA: Academic.
go back to reference Neill, W. T, Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 993–1000.PubMedCrossRef Neill, W. T, Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 993–1000.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential Congruency. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action). Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential Congruency. Psychological Research (Special issue on: Integration in and across perception and action).
go back to reference Park, J. & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 613–623.CrossRefPubMed Park, J. & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 613–623.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1987). Components of visual orienting. In H. Houma, & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1987). Components of visual orienting. In H. Houma, & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95, 385–408.CrossRefPubMed Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95, 385–408.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & DeHouwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming: Evidence form task switching studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition (in press). Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & DeHouwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming: Evidence form task switching studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, D., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 1–17.CrossRef Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, D., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 1–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W. (1988). Micro experimental laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, 206–217. Schneider, W. (1988). Micro experimental laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, 206–217.
go back to reference Strayer, D. L., & Grison, S. (1999). Negative identity priming is contingent on stimulus repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25 (1), 24–38.CrossRefPubMed Strayer, D. L., & Grison, S. (1999). Negative identity priming is contingent on stimulus repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25 (1), 24–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Taylor, T. L. & Klein, R. M. (1998). Inhibition of return to color: A replication and non-extension of Law, Pratt, and Abrams (1995). Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1452–1456. Taylor, T. L. & Klein, R. M. (1998). Inhibition of return to color: A replication and non-extension of Law, Pratt, and Abrams (1995). Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1452–1456.
go back to reference Tenpenny, P. L. (1995). Abstractionist verses episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 339–363. Tenpenny, P. L. (1995). Abstractionist verses episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 339–363.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Rafal, R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Starrveldt, Y., Ro, T., Egly, R., et al. (1997). Object-based facilitation and inhibition from visual orienting in the human split-brain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1522–1532.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P., Rafal, R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Starrveldt, Y., Ro, T., Egly, R., et al. (1997). Object-based facilitation and inhibition from visual orienting in the human split-brain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1522–1532.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Selst, M. A., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 47, 631–650. Van Selst, M. A., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 47, 631–650.
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, B. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, B. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wood, T., & Milliken, B. (1998). Negative priming without ignoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 470–475. Wood, T., & Milliken, B. (1998). Negative priming without ignoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 470–475.
Metagegevens
Titel
Repetition costs in word identification: evaluating a stimulus–response integration account
Auteurs
Bruce Milliken
Juan Lupianez
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0036-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2007

Psychological Research 1/2007 Naar de uitgave