Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 4/2022

Open Access 06-05-2021 | Original Paper

Relations of Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors to Social Skills in Toddlers with Autism

Auteurs: Pang Chaxiong, Catherine Burrows, Kelly N. Botteron, Stephen R. Dager, Annette M. Estes, Heather C. Hazlett, Robert T. Schultz, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum, Joseph Piven, Jason Wolff, IBIS Network

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 4/2022

Abstract

We examined the relations of restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB; insistence on sameness, repetitive sensory-motor, self-injurious behavior) to social skills overall and aspects that comprise social skills as measured by the VABS-II (coping skills, play/leisure time, interpersonal relationships) in 24- (n = 63) and 36-month old (n = 35), high-familial-risk toddlers with ASD. Hierarchical linear regression results indicated that repetitive sensory-motor was the best predictor of social skills overall. Secondary results indicated that all three RRB subtypes were associated with each subdomain of social skills; however, repetitive sensory-motor was the strongest and most consistent among these effects. While our results suggests a general negative relation of subtypes of RRB to aspects of adaptive social function, repetitive sensory-motor behaviors may be of particular relevance to the development of social skills during toddlerhood.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-021-05014-8.
Collaborators of IBIS Network are listed in “Acknowledgment”.
Joseph Piven and Jason Wolff have contributed equally to this work.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Although restricted repetitive behaviors (RRB) and social deficits are both diagnostic domains of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), previous research has predominantly focused on social deficits (Richler et al., 2010; Troyb et al., 2016). Much less is known of the developmental trajectories and outcomes related to RRB and even less so concerning the interplay of RRB and social deficits (Harrop et al., 2014; Richler et al., 2010). As both are core features of ASD and co-emerge in the first several years of life, there is a need to characterize their influence on one another in early childhood (Leekam et al., 2011). Previous work has found that RRB are negatively associated with social communication skills in early childhood (e.g., Bruyneel et al., 2019; Larkin et al., 2017; Mirenda et al., 2010; Watt et al., 2008; Wolff et al., 2014); however, empirical evidence of this link continues to be limited and mixed (e.g. Harrop et al., 2014; Thorup et al., 2018). Moreover, much of this rather limited body of work has considered both RRB and social ability as unitary constructs without regard to relations among subtypes or subdomains. Examining whether specific forms of RRB are differentially associated with aspects of early social functioning would further elucidate this relation by informing whether RRB and social deficits are characterized by specific or general phenomena. This knowledge is important for understanding how RRB and social deficits co-emerge and change over time, perhaps informing the developmental process culminating in a diagnosis of ASD. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine the relations between specific subtypes of RRB and social skills in 24- and 36-month-olds with ASD.
Restricted and repetitive behaviors refer to stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech; insistence on sameness, inflexible adherence to routines, or ritualized behavior; and highly fixated interests (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000). More recently, unusual sensory interests or hyper- or hypoactivity to sensory input were introduced to this domain (APA, 2013). Despite the heterogeneity of RRB, previous studies indicate that topographies of RRB load onto superordinate categories. The most consistent of these in the literature are factors described as insistence on sameness (IS) and repetitive-sensory motor (RSM) (Bishop et al., 2013; Hiruma et al., 2021; Uljarević et al., 2017). IS refers to compulsive, ritualistic behaviors and resistance to changes in routines, whereas RSM refers to apparently purposeless, stereotyped movements and preoccupations with a specific interest, activity, or object. Validation studies of the Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al., 2000) specifically have identified an additional factor, self-injurious behavior (SIB), which stems from a subscale unique to that measure (Bishop et al., 2013; Lam & Aman, 2007; Mirenda et al., 2010). SIB refers to repetitive behaviors that cause or have the potential to cause bodily injury ranging from minor to severe. Thus, a three factor model of RRB derived from the RBS-R (i.e., IS, RSM, and SIB) has been recommended for studies of young children with ASD (Mirenda et al., 2010).
Despite being a core feature of ASD, RRB are not unique to individuals with ASD and are also present in individuals with other disabilities as well as those who are typically-developing (Leekam et al., 2011). The accumulation of RRB studies reveal, however, that individuals with ASD present with a higher frequency and severity of RRB and a greater variety of topographies by age two and three (e.g., Cox et al., 1999; Guthrie et al., 2013; Kim & Lord, 2010; Rogers et al., 2003; Wetherby et al., 2004) and potentially as early as 6–12 months (Baranek, 1999; Elison et al., 2014; Purpura et al., 2017; Wolff et al., 2014; Zwaigenbaum, 2005). And, whereas RRB in typically-developing toddlers are assumed to scaffold the learning of more complex skills such as volitional motor activity, their persistence in toddlers with ASD may have the inverse effect by interfering with early opportunities to advance cognitive and social development (Charman et al., 2005; Harrop et al., 2014; Stronach & Wetherby, 2014; Troyb et al., 2016; Wolff et al., 2014). Specifically, elevated engagement in RRB in the early years of life may compete with and limit opportunities to engage in social interaction, contributing to the accumulation of social skill deficits.
With regard to social skills, prospective studies of infants siblings of children with ASD, who are at elevated familial-risk for ASD, suggest that defining social features of ASD may not become evident until the latter half of the second year of life (Ozonoff et al., 2010), though more subtle social differences have been reported (e.g. Sacrey et al., 2018). In a recent longitudinal study of adaptive behavior among infant siblings, Sacrey et al. (2019) found that social skills as measured by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-II (VABS-II; Sparrow et al.., 2005) were in the average range for all siblings regardless of later diagnostic outcome, with trajectories gradually diverging over toddlerhood. Considered alongside prospective studies of RRB in infant siblings (e.g. Elison et al., 2014; Wolff et al., 2014), such findings suggest that differences in RRB may precede significant deviations in social skills and raise the possibility of reciprocal developmental effects.
However, the relation of RRB to social skills in early childhood is not well understood and empirical data are limited. Studies using direct observations of child behaviors (e.g., Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile [CSBS]) have found that repetitive and stereotyped behaviors were negatively associated with social competence and social affect among toddlers with ASD (Morgan et al., 2008; Stronach & Wetherby, 2014; Watt et al., 2008). Ozonoff et al. (2008) found that restricted and repetitive play behaviors (e.g., spinning and unusual visual exploration of objects) were positively associated with communication and social impairments for 12-month-olds who were later diagnosed with ASD. Relatedly, Bruckner and Yoder (2007) found that restricted use of objects in toddlers with ASD was negatively associated with response to joint attention and coordinated attention to an object and person. Using parent-report measures, Schertz et al. (2016) examined the relation between RRB (RBS-R) and social skills (VABS-II) in a sample of 2-years olds with ASD. They found negative correlations between all RRB subtypes and socialization domain scores. Using the same measures with a prospective, high-risk sample of toddlers with ASD, Wolff et al. (2014) found a similar relation between RRB and socialization domain scores at 24 months and further found that RRB was not related to other domains of adaptive behavior. These findings suggest a rather specific behavioral association that may reflect developmental effects unique to early childhood. Moreover, the inverse relation of RRB to social skills may not extend to children who are typically developing. While IS and RSM behaviors may be common among such children during early childhood, there is evidence that aspects of social skills, such as frequency and sophistication of play behavior, are not associated with RRB (Honey et al., 2007).
It is still unclear, however, if the RRB-social skills relation is driven by specific aspects of one or both domains. Finding that a particular subtype of RRB and/or aspect of social skills is related above and beyond others would point to specific phenomena for further study. For example, it may be that the general relation reported in the literature is largely driven by an inverse correlation between specific aspects of RRB and social function (e.g. repetitive motor behavior and play sophistication). In contrast, the absence of significant differentiated relations would suggest a general phenomenon, such that multiple aspects or topographies of RRB are inversely related to an array of social skills in toddlerhood. This knowledge is important for informing our understanding of how RRB and social deficits co-develop during the early unfolding of ASD and may inform specific targets for early intervention.
In the present study, we sought to further elucidate the relation between RRB and social skills in young children with ASD previously reported in Wolff et al. (2014) by examining whether this relation is characterized by specific or general phenomena. To accomplish this, we examined whether subtypes of RRB (IS, RSM, SIB) were differentially associated with (1) social skills overall (VABS-II socialization domain score) and (2) specific aspects that comprise the socialization domain in children with ASD at ages 24 and 36 months. The subdomains of behavior comprising the construct of socialization as defined by the VABS-II include coping skills (CS), play and leisure (PL), and interpersonal relationships (IPR). We hypothesized that higher RRB subtype scores (higher RRB severity) would be associated with lower socialization domain scores at both 24 and 36 months. We further hypothesized that RSM would drive this relation (i.e., explain significant variation in socialization domain scores at both time points). These hypotheses were based on findings from Wolff et al. (2014) as well as studies of older children (e.g. Lampi et al., 2018) which suggest that components of RSM are inversely associated with social skills. We did not have hypotheses about relations between RRB subtypes and specific aspects of social skills as this was exploratory.

Methods

Participants

Participants were from the ongoing Autism Center of Excellence (ACE) Network Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS). IBIS participants were recruited through research registries, websites, email blasts, flyers, brochures, and community clinics. Exclusion criteria included having (a) genetic or medical conditions that impacted development; (b) significant visual or hearing impairments; (c) a birth weight < 2000 g and/or a gestational age < 36 weeks or significant perinatal adverse experiences; (d) contraindication for MRI; (e) a home language other than English; (f) first-degree relatives with psychosis, schizophrenia, or bipolar disorder; and being (g) a twin, adopted or a half-sibling of a proband. For eligible participants, longitudinal data on cognitive development, adaptive functioning, and ASD characteristics were collected at four time points (i.e., 6, 12, 24, 36 months of age). At each time point, participants were given two $50 incentives for participation in a behavioral assessment and magnetic resonance imaging, in addition to travel reimbursements. Recruitment, screening, and assessment of IBIS participants were conducted at four clinical sites: University of North Carolina, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Washington, and Washington University in St. Louis. Each clinical site received study approval from their Human Subjects Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained and documented for all participants.
Of the IBIS dataset, our study included high-familial-risk participants with completed RBS-R, VABS-II, and Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) assessments at 24 and/or 36-months who met clinical best-estimate diagnostic criteria for ASD based on DSM-IV-TR criteria. Given the recurrence rate of ASD in families, following younger siblings of children with ASD allows for the prospective study of infancy and toddlerhood in children who are later diagnosed. High-risk toddlers were defined as such by virtue of having an older sibling with an existing diagnosis of ASD. Proband diagnoses were confirmed using the Social Communication Questionnaire and Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al. 1994). For their younger siblings (current participants), diagnoses were based on multiple assessments (i.e., Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale, ADI-R, and MSEL) conducted at 24 and/or 36 months of age that were independently verified by a senior clinician blind to classification. This resulted in a final study sample of sixty-three 24- and thirty-five 36-month-olds with ASD. Twenty-six (74%) participants from the 36-month sample were part of the 24-month sample. Participant descriptive information are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Participant characteristics
Characteristic
24-Months-olds
36-Month-olds
N
63
35
Age
24.69 (1.47) [23.40–32.10]
39.03 (4.59) [34.80–57.50]
Sex
  
 Male
49 (77.8%)
29 (82.9%)
 Female
14 (22.2%)
6 (17.1%)
RRB (RBS-R)
  
 IS
5.16 (8.74) [0–61]
10.23 (8.73) [1–38]
 RSM
3.73 (4.31) [0–20]
6.29 (5.12) [0–18]
 SIB
1.27 (1.92) [0–10]
1.34 (2.55) [0–13]
 Total
10.16 (13.93) [0–91]
17.86 (14.40) [2–55]
Socialization (VABS-II)
 
 CS
14.00 (2.08) [8–19]
11.77 (8.73) [8–15]
 PL
13.00 (2.51) [8–19]
11.97 (2.72) [8–16]
 IPR
12.48 (2.01) [7–17]
11.40 (2.39) [7–17]
 Domain/overall
89.73 (9.64) [61–116]
81.29 (11.45) [61–101]
Mullen ELC
80.68 (15.47) [49–115]
76.49 (18.66) [49–109]
RRB restricted repetitive behavior, RBS-R Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised, VABS-II Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, IS insistence on sameness, RSM repetitive sensory motor, SIB self-injurious behavior, CS coping skills, PL play and leisure time, IPR interpersonal relationships, Mullen ELC Mullen early learning composite

Measures

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al., 2000)

The RBS-R is a parent rating scale that measures RRB based on parent/caregiver observations over the last month that has been used in samples of individuals with ASD from early childhood to adulthood (e.g., Antezana et al., 2019; Boyd et al., 2010; Gabriels et al., 2008; McDermott et al., 2020; Oakes et al., 2016; Schertz et al., 2016; Wolff et al., 2014, Wolff et al., 2019). It consists of 43 items that correspond to six RRB subscales: stereotyped, self-injurious, compulsive, ritualistic, sameness, and restricted. Subscale scores are calculated by summing the ratings (0 = “behavior does not occur” to 3 = “behavior is a severe problem”) of all their correspondent items, representing the severity of each type of RRB. An overall RRB score is calculated by summing the subscale scores. The RBS-R has been shown to have good to excellent psychometric properties, including evidence of reliability and construct validity, particularly for derived three- and five-factor models (Scahill et al., 2015). In the present sample, there was strong internal consistency (0.80 to 0.91) for items comprising RSM and IS factors at both 24 and 36 months. Internal consistency for SIB, however, was poor (0.50 to 0.55), which may reflect the relatively fewer items or lower endorsement/lower variability for this factor. This aspect of construct validity should be considered in the interpretation of SIB-related results within this sample.
To feasibly examine distinct relations between RRB and social skills, we elected to examine RRB items in three subtypes, following the three-factor model recommended by Mirenda et al. (2010) for fit and parsimony: insistence on sameness (IS), repetitive sensory-motor (RSM), and self-injurious behavior (SIB). Items from the compulsive, ritualistic, and sameness subscales were combined to create the IS subtype, and items from the stereotyped and restricted behavior subscales were combined to create the RSM subtype. The SIB subtype consisted of items from the self-injurious subscale. IS, RSM, and SIB subtype scores were calculated by summing their correspondent subscale scores.
There were several reasons for adopting a three-factor model to individual RRB items. First, examining individual items exacerbates the multiple comparisons problem, while also posing a multicollinearity issue. Additionally, collapsing similar RRB items into larger categories increases reliability and also eases cross-study communication of RRB and may be more useful for diagnostic and intervention purposes. Thus, we determined that a three-factor model would allow for feasible examination of distinct relations between RRB and social skills without inflating type one error rates, while acknowledging that alternative RRB factor structures are plausible.

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II (VABS-II; Sparrow et al., 2005)

The VABS-II is a standardized, norm-referenced, semi-structured parent/caregiver interview that assesses daily adaptive behavior in individuals from birth to 90 years of age. Our analyses utilize the socialization domain score (M = 100, SD 15, range = 0–160) and the CS, PL, and IPR subdomain scores (M = 15, SD 3, range = 0–25). The VABS-II provides both standard and age-equivalent scores; however, only standard scores were used in this study. Strong reliability and validity of the VABS-II have been demonstrated, including internal consistency ranging from 0.84 to 0.92 for the Socialization domain and good to excellent test–retest reliability (Sparrow et al., 2005). Previous studies of young children with ASD as well as high-risk infant sibling samples suggest good sensitivity (Chatham et al., 2018) and predictive (Sacrey et al., 2019) and convergent validity (Dupuis et al., 2020) for socialization domain and subdomain scores.

Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995)

We used the Mullen Early Learning Composite (Mullen ELC) score from the MSEL as a proxy of general cognitive ability. The MSEL is a standardized developmental assessment consisting of five brief scales that describe early cognitive and motor development in children birth to 68 months of age. The MSEL was administered by trained research staff; reliability across all study sites was established prior to administration and was maintained throughout data collection.

Statistical Analyses

Research Question 1

Simple correlations were generated to describe binary relations between each RRB subtype (IS, RSM, SIB) and socialization domain score. We then conducted a four-step hierarchical linear regression to examine whether different RRB subtypes explained unique variance in socialization domain scores at 24 and 36 months. Mixed effects regression was considered given the potential for age effects; however, the addition of an age interaction term was not significant for any model, and thus was not used for the analyses presented herein. Sex and Mullen ELC score were entered at Step 1 as covariates, serving as our base model. Sex was included given previous evidence of differential effects with type or frequency of RRB (Kim & Lord, 2010; Knutsen et al., 2019). Mullen ELC score was included given evidence that general cognitive ability may be negatively or positively associated with different RRB subtypes (Militerni et al., 2002; Richler et al., 2010). The moderating effect of Mullen ELC score was further evaluated as an interaction term. However, this did not improve fit for any model, and thus was entered as a main effect only. RSM was entered at step 2 (Model 2), followed by IS at step 3 (Model 3) and SIB at step 4 (Model 4; see Fig. 1). This order was determined in alignment with our hypothesis that RSM would drive the RRB-social skills relation. Change in R2 was examined to determine whether models with the addition of each RRB subtype explained additional variation in socialization domain scores above and beyond the previous model.

Research Question 2

Next, we conducted multiple linear regressions to explore whether RRB subtypes were differentially associated with socialization subdomains (CS, PL, IPR) at 24- and 36-months. Mullen ELC was included as a covariate in multiple linear regressions for 36-month-old data, as it was a significant predictor of socialization domain scores at this age in all models from Research Question 1.
Standardized RRB subtype scores were used in all analyses. To account for multiple comparisons in research questions one and two, results are presented with Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels (\(\alpha\) = 0.008 and \(\alpha\) = 0.003, respectively) and without (\(\alpha\) = 0.05). This analysis plan was determined after careful examination of assumptions for conducting linear regressions. Density and scatter plots of residuals indicated that assumptions of normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity were tenable. All analyses were conducted in the R software (R Core Team, 2019).

Results

Research Question 1

We first asked whether RRB subtypes (IS, RSM, SIB) were differentially associated with socialization domain scores (social skills overall) among high-risk children with ASD at 24 and 36 months. It was hypothesized that higher RRB subtype scores would be associated with lower socialization domain scores at both 24 and 36 months, with RSM explaining significant variation in socialization domain scores at both time points. As expected, correlations between RRB subtypes and socialization domain scores were negative and relatively consistent at both 24 months, IS (r = − 0.51, 95% CI [− 0.29, − 0.67]); RSM (r = − 0.53, 95% CI [− 0.32, − 0.69]); SIB (r = − 0.39, 95% CI [− 0.16, − 0.58]); and 36 months, IS (r =  − 0.46, 95% CI [− 0.15, − 0.69]); RSM (r = − 0.66, 95% CI [− 0.42, − 0.81]); SIB (r = − 0.39, 95% CI [− 0.07, − 0.64]). Hierarchical linear regression results presented in Table 2 reveal negative relations between severity of RRB subtypes and socialization domain scores at both ages. The addition of RSM to our base model (sex and Mullen ELC) best explained variation in socialization domain scores; subsequent models with IS and SIB did not explain variation above and beyond the model with RSM alone.
Table 2
Hierarchical multiple regression analyses for RRB Subtype (RBS-R) predicting socialization domain scores (VABS-II) at 24 and 36 months
 
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
24 Months
       
 Sex
− 1.145
.745
.563
.677
 IQ
.095
.085
.125
.130
 RSM
 
− 5.081***
− 2.463
− 2.331
 IS
  
− 3.213
− 2.900
 SIB
   
− .607
 F
.838
8.342***
7.246***
5.743***
 \({R}^{2}\)
.027
.298
.333
.335
 Δ\({R}^{2}\)
 
.271***
.035
.002
36 Months
    
 Sex
4.340
5.515
5.453
7.341
 IQ
.336***
.227**
.224**
.216**
 RSM
 
− 6.312***
− 6.723**
− 5.710*
 IS
  
.501
.356
 SIB
   
− 2.139
F
7.693**
15.191***
11.058***
9.430***
 \({R}^{2}\)
.325
.595
.596
.619
 Δ \({R}^{2}\)
 
.270***
.000
.023
Standardized regression coefficients. 24 months (N = 63). 36 months (N = 35). Bold face indicates Δ \({R}^{2}\) that survive the Bonferroni adjusted alpha level (\(\alpha\) = .008)
RRB restricted repetitive behavior, RBS-R Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised, VABS-II Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, IS insistence on sameness, RSM repetitive sensory motor, SIB self-injurious behavior, IQ Mullen early learning composite
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

24 Months

For 24-month-olds, the base model with sex and Mullen ELC score accounted for R2 = 0.03 and, as a full model, did not differ significantly from zero, F(2, 60) = 0.8, p = 0.438. Neither sex (β = − 1.15, p = 0.699) nor Mullen ELC (β = 1.47, p = 0.239) were significant predictors of socialization domain scores. In Model 2, RSM was added as a predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.30 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(3, 59) = 8.3, p < 0.001. The addition of RSM resulted in a significant change in R2, \(\Delta\)R2 = 0.27, F(1, 59) = 22.7, p < 0.001, which remained significant at the adjusted alpha level (\(\alpha\) = 0.008). Controlling for sex and Mullen ELC, RSM was a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = − 5.08, p < 0.001). In Model 3, IS was added as a second predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.33 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(4, 58) = 7.2, p < 0.001. The addition of IS did not result in a significant change in R2, however, \(\Delta\)R2 = 0.04, F(1, 58) = 3.08, p = 0.085. Controlling for sex, Mullen ELC and RSM, IS was not a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = − 3.21, p = 0.085). In Model 4, SIB was added as a third predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.34 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(5, 57) = 5.7, p < 0.001. The addition of SIB also did not result in a significant change in R2, \(\Delta\) R2 < 0.01, F(1, 57) = 0.15, p = 0.696. Controlling for sex, Mullen ELC, RSM and IS, SIB was not a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = − 0.61, p = 0.696).

36 Months

For 36-month-olds, the base model with sex and Mullen ELC score accounted for R2 = 0.32 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(2, 32) = 7.7, p = 0.002. Sex was not a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = 4.34, p = 0.326); however, Mullen ELC was (β = 0.34, p < 0.001). In Model 2, RSM was added as a predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.60 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(3, 31) = 8.3, p < 0.001. The addition of RSM resulted in a significant change in R2, \(\Delta\)R2 = 0.27, F(1, 31) = 20.7, p < 0.001, which remained significant at the adjusted alpha level (α = 0.008). Controlling for sex and Mullen ELC, RSM was a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = − 6.31, p < 0.001). In Model 3, IS was added as a second predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.60 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(4, 30) = 11.1, p < 0.001. The addition of IS did not result in a significant change in R2, however, \(\Delta\)R2 < 0.01, F(1, 30) = 0.1, p = 0.821. Controlling for sex, Mullen ELC and RSM, IS was not a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = 0.50, p = 0.821). In Model 4, SIB was added as a third predictor, accounting for R2 = 0.62 and, as a full model, did differ significantly from zero, F(5, 29) = 9.4, p < 0.001. The addition of SIB also did not result in a significant change in R2, \(\Delta\)R2 = 0.02, F(1, 29) = 1.8, p = 0.193. Controlling for sex, Mullen ELC, RSM and IS, SIB was not a significant predictor of socialization domain scores (β = − 2.14, p = 0.193).
Altogether, the results reveal that higher RRB severity was associated with lower parent-reported social skills. Furthermore, Model 2 with RSM best predicted social skills overall. Models 3 and 4 with IS and SIB, respectively, did not predict social skills overall above and beyond that of Model 2. This was the case at both 24 and 36 months of age. As covariates, general cognitive ability played a more significant role at 36 than at 24 months while sex did not appear to play a meaningful role at either time points.

Research Question 2

We next explored the relations between each RRB subtype and the three subdomains of socialization (CS, PL, IPR). Mullen ELC score was included as a covariate in multiple linear regressions at 36 months based on its significance as a predictor in socialization domain scores from research question one. Unadjusted regression results in Table 3 suggest that the three RRB subtypes were related to the three socialization subdomains across 24 and 36 months; adjusted regression results, however, suggest that RSM best predicted socialization subdomains at both time points.
Table 3
Multiple linear regression analyses for RRB subtype (RBS-R) predicting socialization subdomains (VABS-II) at 24 and 36 months
 
RRB
IS
RSM
SIB
Subdomain
Age
24
36
24
36
24
36
CS
βRRB
− .71**
− .68*
− .59*
− 1.06***
− .37
− .67*
 
BIQ
 
.04*
 
.03
 
.04*
 
\({R}^{2}\)
.12
.31
.08
.45
.03
.30
 
F
7.96**
7.08**
5.36*
13.02***
1.99
6.91**
PL
βRRB
− .90**
− .93*
− 1.29***
− 1.51***
− .76*
− .90*
 
βIQ
 
.05*
 
.03
 
.05*
 
\({R}^{2}\)
.13
.29
.27
.45
.09
.27
 
F
8.88**
6.38**
21.99***
12.96***
6.22*
6.07**
IPR
βRRB
− .90***
− .80*
− .76**
− .77*
− .75**
− .31
 
βIQ
 
.07***
 
.06**
 
.07***
 
\({R}^{2}\)
.20
.45
.14
.43
.14
.35
 
F
15.19***
13.02***
10.18**
12.18***
9.86**
8.79***
Standardized regression coefficients. 24 months (N = 63). 36 months (N = 35). Bold face indicates βRRB that survive the Bonferroni adjusted alpha level (\(\alpha\) = .003)
RRB restricted repetitive behavior, RBS-R Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised, VABS-II Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, IS insistence on sameness, RSM repetitive sensory motor, SIB self-injurious behavior, CS coping skills, PL play and leisure time, IPR interpersonal relationships, IQ Mullen early learning composite
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

24 Months

For 24-month-olds, unadjusted regression results indicated that IS significantly predicted CS (β = − 0.71, p = 0.006), PL (β = − 0.90, p = 0.004), and IPR (β = − 0.90, p < 0.001); however, IS was a significant predictor of only IPR at the adjusted alpha level (\(\alpha\) = 0.003). RSM significantly predicted CS (β = − 0.59, p = 0.024), PL (β = − 1.29, p < 0.001) and IPR (β = − 0.76, p = 0.002). At the adjusted alpha level, RSM remained a significant predictor of PL and IPR. SIB significantly predicted PL (β = − 0.76, p = 0.015) and IPR (β = − 0.75, p = 0.003) but did not significantly predict CS (β = − 0.40, p = 0.163). At the adjusted alpha level, SIB remained a significant predictor of only IPR.

36 Months

For 36-month-olds, regression results indicated that IS significantly predicted CS (β = − 0.68, p = 0.030), PL (β = − 0.93, p = 0.031) and IPR (β = − 0.80, p = 0.017), controlling for Mullen ELC. At the adjusted alpha level (\(\alpha\) = 0.003), however, IS was no longer a significant predictor of any socialization subdomain. RSM also significantly predicted CS (β = − 1.06, p < 0.001), PL (β = − 1.51, p < 0.001) and IPR (β = − 0.77, p = 0.028), controlling for Mullen ELC. At the adjusted alpha level, RSM remained a significant predictor of CS and PL. SIB significantly predicted CS (β = − 0.67, p = 0.035) and PL (β = − 0.90, p = 0.040) but did not significantly predict IPR (β = − 0.31, p = 0.375), controlling for Mullen ELC. At the adjusted alpha level, SIB was no longer a significant predictor of any socialization subdomain.
Overall, unadjusted results reveal that the three RRB subtypes were significant predictors of the three aspects of social skills, with SIB being a less consistent predictor across the two time points. Adjusted results indicate that RSM was the only RRB subtype to significantly predict two of three aspects of social skills at both 24 and 36 months.

Follow-Up Analyses

To ensure that our adoption of a three-factor RRB model did not mask distinct relations between RRB and socialization subdomains, we conducted a follow-up series of analyses using as predictors the six subscale breakdown from the RBS-R (i.e., stereotyped, self-injurious, compulsive, ritualistic, sameness, and restricted). Analyses with the six RBS-R subscales yielded results that were consistent with the three-factor model used in our primary analysis. Standardized regression analyses are provided in supplemental Tables 1 and 2.

Discussion

Motivated by previous findings (Wolff et al., 2014), we sought to determine whether the negative relation between RRB and social skills is characterized by specific or general phenomena by examining whether subtypes of RRB are differentially associated with social skills overall and aspects of the domain in toddlers with ASD. Our results suggest that RSM may drive the RRB-social skills relation. First, our base model with the addition of RSM explained a significant amount of variation in social skills overall, while the subsequent addition of IS and SIB did not contribute significantly. Secondly, although each of the three RRB subtypes were negatively associated with at least one social skill subdomain at 24 and 36 months, RSM was characterized by the greatest effect sizes and most consistently survived adjustment for multiple comparisons. This was particularly true for the relation of RSM to play and leisure skills, which is perhaps unsurprising given the rapid development of play over this age range. In summary, while we identified that while RSM, IS, and SIB were negatively associated with at least some aspects of social skills in familial-risk toddlers who developed ASD, RSM appears to most consistently explain variance in adaptive social behavior.
The present findings are consistent with reports showing that RSM is most prominent during toddlerhood but begins to gradually decline by preschool age in children with ASD (Lord et al., 2015; Richler et al., 2007). Conversely, these same studies indicate that IS remains relatively steady or gradually increases into school age. Given that patterns of RRB change across development, we expect that so to do their relation adaptive social function. With collection of school-age follow-up currently underway, we will soon be able to examine whether and how these relations change with age. Interestingly, the contribution of cognitive ability in our modeling was trivial at age 24 months but increased in effect by age 36 months. This is consistent with our previous findings (Wolff et al., 2014) as well as work by others suggesting that the role of cognitive ability to both social skills and RRB development may strengthen from late toddlerhood onward (Bishop et al., 2006; Tomaszewski et al., 2020).
As RRB and social communication deficits are both core features of ASD, understanding whether and how one impacts the development of the other could inform the early unfolding of ASD. Although a causal direction is unclear, one possibility is that children with ASD present with social communication deficits and, thus, engage in RSM and other forms of RRB to occupy time not filled by competing social interactions or typical play behavior (Lampi et al., 2018; Ray-Subramanian & Ellis Weismer, 2012). Relatedly, and in consideration of the young age of our participants, it is possible that early RRB, irrespective of topography, interferes with the acquisition of social skills. Emergent literature suggests that RRB may be more than independent, passive symptoms of ASD and instead potentially contribute to its emergence by encroaching on opportunities to develop and practice adaptive social behaviors, thereby contributing to social deficits (Richler et al., 2010). This possibility is now more viable with accumulating research indicating that timing of RRB emergence precedes marked social deficits. Elison et al. (2014), for example, found that RRB are observed in 1-year old children who are later diagnosed with ASD, prior to reported divergence in social skills (Ozonoff et al., 2010). Notably, these RRB occurred in the context of standardized social communicative presses, often in place of an expected response. Furthermore, it is possible that the relation between RRB and social skills begins as unidirectional, becoming bidirectional– and mutually sustaining– over development (i.e., RRB perpetuate social deficits, and social deficits in turn precipitate RRB). An example of this might be repetitive behaviors that take on the function of a coping mechanism in lieu of more conventional responses (Manor-Binyamini & Schreiber-Divon, 2019), a possibility supported by our finding that RRB and coping skills are negatively associated, with the effect strengthening from 24 to 36 months.
Finally, despite strong negative associations, it is also possible that RRB and social skill deficits co-occur but do not contribute to the development of one another. By definition, both features are present in individuals with ASD; this alone does not imply an interaction or causal relation. The present work cannot rule out the possibility that these aspects of ASD develop independently. However, experimental studies demonstrating that engaging in social behavior resulted in reduced rates of RRB suggest that RRB and social interactions may be incompatible, wherein engagement in one set of behaviors reduces opportunity to engage in the other. For example, Lee et al. (2007) found that play initiation from typically-developing peers led to an increase in the percentage of time elementary children with ASD were socially engaged and a simultaneous decrease in their stereotypic behaviors. Lampi et al. (2018) found that school-aged children with ASD displayed the fewest number of RRB during experimental tasks in which they were highly engaged in social and motor behaviors. Similar observations of an increase in social behavior and a simultaneous decrease in RRB were also found in earlier studies examining school-aged children with ASD (e.g., Donnellan et al., 1984; Lee & Odom, 1996; Lord & Hopkins, 1986). Such a collateral relation offers some evidence that RRB and adaptive social function likely influence one another.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. Our sample size at 36 months (n = 35) is half the size of our sample at 24 months (n = 63), and thus provided less power to detect a small or even medium effect, potentially resulting in a type II error. Relatedly, the majority of participants in both samples were male thus limiting conclusions that can be drawn about the effects of sex. Follow-up work with larger samples, including girls with ASD, are warranted. Only 13 of 60 socialization items on the VABS-II are appropriate for children under 36 months and are unevenly distributed: seven pertaining to IPR, four pertaining to PL, and two pertaining to CS. While there was sufficient variability in subdomain scores, the low number of items contributing to each subscale, particularly for CS, presents validity concerns that should be considered when interpreting the results regarding specific relations between RRB and socialization subdomains. It is clear that the results of the exploratory analysis of socialization subdomains requires follow-up with additional measures of social behavior suitable for this age group. Relatedly, the RBS-R is not specific to children in this age range and, thus, parents were asked to respond to items that may not have been developmentally appropriate for their child. Future studies should consider quantitative measures of RRB that are specifically designed for very young children. Furthermore, we examined the relation between RRB and social skills at a sub-categorical level (i.e., three RRB subtypes and three socialization subdomains) as opposed to item level. It is possible that a three-factor RRB model and the VABS-II breakdown of social skills masks important item-level relations. Future studies might consider examining theory-driven item-level relations using structural equation modeling to build upon the present findings.

Conclusions

Although differences in social function and RRB represent diagnostic characteristics of ASD, both have largely been studied separately, and their relation to one another is not well understood. Previous studies have found negative associations between RRB and social skills; however, whether this relation is characterized by general or specific phenomena has been unclear. Our study provides additional evidence that RRB and adaptive social skills may develop in inverse relation to one another at ages two and three. Indeed, it would appear that each RRB subtype is associated with at least some aspect of social skills. Nonetheless, RSM appears to best explain variance in social skills during toddlerhood and may represent a specific target for further study. Striving to characterize the causal direction and nuances of this relation may enable more effective and targeted intervention for supporting adaptive social function by elucidating the role of RRB as well as contribute to our understanding of the early emergence of ASD.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health under awards R01MH116961, P30HD03110, and R01HD05574; Autism Speaks, and the Simons Foundation. We wish to express our gratitude to IBIS families for continued participation in this research.

IBIS Network

J. Piven (IBIS Network PI), Clinical Sites: University of North Carolina: H.C. Hazlett, C. Chappell, M. Shen, M. Swanson; University of Washington: S. Dager, A. Estes, D. Shaw, T. St. John; Washington University: K. Botteron, J. Constantino; Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia: R. Schultz, J. Pandey; Behavior Core: University of Washington: A. Estes; University of Alberta: L. Zwaigenbaum; University of Minnesota: J. Elison, J. Wolff; Imaging Core: University of North Carolina: M. Styner; New York University: G. Gerig; Washington University in St. Louis: R. McKinstry, J. Pruett; Data Coordinating Center: Montreal Neurological Institute: A.C. Evans, D.L. Collins, V. Fonov, L. MacIntyre; S. Das; Statistical Analysis Core: University of North Carolina: H. Gu, K. Truong; Environmental risk core: John Hopkins University: H. Volk; Genetics Core: John Hopkins University: D. Fallin; UNC: M. Shen.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRef
go back to reference Chatham, C. H., Taylor, K. I., Charman, T., LiogierD’ardhuy, X., Eule, E., Fedele, A., Hardan, A. Y., Loth, E., Muragh, L., del Valle Rubido, M., San Jose Caceres, A., Sevigny, J., Sikich, L., Snyder, L., Tillmann, J. E., Ventola, P. E., Walton-Bowe, K. L., Wang, P. P., Willgoss, T., & Bolognani, F. (2018). Adaptive behavior in autism: Minimal clinically important differences on the Vineland-II. Autism Research, 11(2), 270–283. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1874CrossRefPubMed Chatham, C. H., Taylor, K. I., Charman, T., LiogierD’ardhuy, X., Eule, E., Fedele, A., Hardan, A. Y., Loth, E., Muragh, L., del Valle Rubido, M., San Jose Caceres, A., Sevigny, J., Sikich, L., Snyder, L., Tillmann, J. E., Ventola, P. E., Walton-Bowe, K. L., Wang, P. P., Willgoss, T., & Bolognani, F. (2018). Adaptive behavior in autism: Minimal clinically important differences on the Vineland-II. Autism Research, 11(2), 270–283. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​1874CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 719–732. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00488CrossRefPubMed Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 719–732. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​1469-7610.​00488CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dupuis, A., Moon, M. J., Brian, J., Georgiades, S., Levy, T., Anagnostou, E., Nicholson, R., Schachar, R., & Crosbie, J. (2020). Concurrent validity of the ABAS-II questionnaire with the Vineland II interview for adaptive behavior in a pediatric ASD sample: High correspondence despite systematically lower scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04597-yCrossRef Dupuis, A., Moon, M. J., Brian, J., Georgiades, S., Levy, T., Anagnostou, E., Nicholson, R., Schachar, R., & Crosbie, J. (2020). Concurrent validity of the ABAS-II questionnaire with the Vineland II interview for adaptive behavior in a pediatric ASD sample: High correspondence despite systematically lower scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-020-04597-yCrossRef
go back to reference Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Miller, L. J., Gralla, J., Pan, Z., Goldson, E., Ledbetter, J. C., Dinkins, J. P., & Hooks, E. (2008). Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 660–670. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.02.002CrossRef Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Miller, L. J., Gralla, J., Pan, Z., Goldson, E., Ledbetter, J. C., Dinkins, J. P., & Hooks, E. (2008). Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 660–670. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​rasd.​2008.​02.​002CrossRef
go back to reference Guthrie, W., Swineford, L. B., Nottke, C., & Wetherby, A. M. (2013). Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation: Stability of early diagnosis and symptoms in ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 582–590. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12008CrossRefPubMed Guthrie, W., Swineford, L. B., Nottke, C., & Wetherby, A. M. (2013). Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation: Stability of early diagnosis and symptoms in ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 582–590. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12008CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hiruma, L., Pretzel, R. E., Tapia, A. L., Bodfish, J. W., Bradley, C., Wiggins, L., Hsu, M., Lee, L., Levy, S. E., & Daniels, J. (2021). A distinct three-factor structure of restricted and repetitive behaviors in an epidemiologically sound sample of preschool-age children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04776-xCrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Hiruma, L., Pretzel, R. E., Tapia, A. L., Bodfish, J. W., Bradley, C., Wiggins, L., Hsu, M., Lee, L., Levy, S. E., & Daniels, J. (2021). A distinct three-factor structure of restricted and repetitive behaviors in an epidemiologically sound sample of preschool-age children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-020-04776-xCrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Lampi, A., Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2018). Understanding the influence of social and motor context on the co-occurring frequency of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 1479–1496. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3698-3CrossRef Lampi, A., Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2018). Understanding the influence of social and motor context on the co-occurring frequency of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 1479–1496. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-018-3698-3CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172145CrossRefPubMed Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​BF02172145CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lord, C., Bishop, S., & Anderson, D. (2015). Developmental trajectories as autism phenotypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C, 169, 198–208CrossRef Lord, C., Bishop, S., & Anderson, D. (2015). Developmental trajectories as autism phenotypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C, 169, 198–208CrossRef
go back to reference Mirenda, P., Smith, I. M., Vaillancourt, T., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Fombonne, E., Roberts, W., Volden, J., Waddell, C., Zwaigenbaum, L., & The Pathways in ASD Study Team. (2010). Validating the repetitive behavior scale-revised in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1521–1530. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1012-0CrossRefPubMed Mirenda, P., Smith, I. M., Vaillancourt, T., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Fombonne, E., Roberts, W., Volden, J., Waddell, C., Zwaigenbaum, L., & The Pathways in ASD Study Team. (2010). Validating the repetitive behavior scale-revised in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1521–1530. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-010-1012-0CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. AGS Publishing. Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. AGS Publishing.
go back to reference Purpura, G., Constanzo, V., Chericoni, N., Puopolo, M., Scattoni, M. L., Muratori, F., & Apicella, F. (2017). Bilateral patterns of repetitive movements in 6- to 12-month old infants with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1168CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Purpura, G., Constanzo, V., Chericoni, N., Puopolo, M., Scattoni, M. L., Muratori, F., & Apicella, F. (2017). Bilateral patterns of repetitive movements in 6- to 12-month old infants with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1168CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Sacrey, L. R., Bryson, S., Zwaigenbaum, L., Brian, J., Smith, I. M., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Vaillancourt, T., Roncadin, C., & Garon, N. (2018). The Autism Parent Screen for Infants: Predicting risk of autism spectrum disorder based on parent-reported behavior observed at 6–24 months of age. Autism, 22(3), 322–334. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316675120CrossRefPubMed Sacrey, L. R., Bryson, S., Zwaigenbaum, L., Brian, J., Smith, I. M., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Vaillancourt, T., Roncadin, C., & Garon, N. (2018). The Autism Parent Screen for Infants: Predicting risk of autism spectrum disorder based on parent-reported behavior observed at 6–24 months of age. Autism, 22(3), 322–334. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​1362361316675120​CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sacrey, L. A. R., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Smith, I. M., Raza, S., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Vaillancourt, T., Roncadin, C., & Garon, N. (2019). Developmental trajectories of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorder: A high-risk sibling cohort. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 60(6), 697–706. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12985CrossRefPubMed Sacrey, L. A. R., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Smith, I. M., Raza, S., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Vaillancourt, T., Roncadin, C., & Garon, N. (2019). Developmental trajectories of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorder: A high-risk sibling cohort. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 60(6), 697–706. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12985CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scahill, L., Aman, M. G., Lecavalier, L., Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S. L., Bodfish, J. W., Grondhuis, S., Jones, N., Horrigan, J. P., Cook, E. H., Handen, B. L., King, B. H., Pearson, D. A., McCracken, J. T., Sullivan, K. A., & Dawson, G. (2015). Measuring repetitive behaviors as a treatment endpoint in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 19(1), 38–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313510069CrossRefPubMed Scahill, L., Aman, M. G., Lecavalier, L., Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S. L., Bodfish, J. W., Grondhuis, S., Jones, N., Horrigan, J. P., Cook, E. H., Handen, B. L., King, B. H., Pearson, D. A., McCracken, J. T., Sullivan, K. A., & Dawson, G. (2015). Measuring repetitive behaviors as a treatment endpoint in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 19(1), 38–52. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​1362361313510069​CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (2005). Vineland adapativebehavior scales. (2nd ed.). AGS Publishing. Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (2005). Vineland adapativebehavior scales. (2nd ed.). AGS Publishing.
go back to reference Tomaszewski, B., Hepburn, S., Blakeley-Smith, A., & Rogers, S. J. (2020). Developmental trajectories of adaptive behavior from toddlerhood to middle childhood in autism spectrum disorder. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 125, 155–169CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tomaszewski, B., Hepburn, S., Blakeley-Smith, A., & Rogers, S. J. (2020). Developmental trajectories of adaptive behavior from toddlerhood to middle childhood in autism spectrum disorder. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 125, 155–169CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Uljarević, M., Arnott, B., Carrington, S. J., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., McConachie, H., Le Couteur, A., & Leekam, S. R. (2017). Development of restricted and repetitive behaviors from 15 to 77 months: Stability of two distinct subtypes? Developmental Psychology, 53(10), 1859–1868. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000324CrossRefPubMed Uljarević, M., Arnott, B., Carrington, S. J., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., McConachie, H., Le Couteur, A., & Leekam, S. R. (2017). Development of restricted and repetitive behaviors from 15 to 77 months: Stability of two distinct subtypes? Developmental Psychology, 53(10), 1859–1868. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​dev0000324CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolff, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., Hazlett, H. C., Pandey, J., Paterson, S. J., Schultz, R. T., Zwaigenbaum, L., Piven, J., & IBIS Network. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(8), 945–953. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12207CrossRefPubMed Wolff, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., Hazlett, H. C., Pandey, J., Paterson, S. J., Schultz, R. T., Zwaigenbaum, L., Piven, J., & IBIS Network. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(8), 945–953. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12207CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolff, J. J., Dimian, A. F., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Hazlett, H. C., Schultz, R. T., Zwaigenbaum, L., Piven, J., & the IBIS Network. (2019). A longitudinal study of parent-reported sensory responsiveness in toddlers at-risk for autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(3), 314–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12978CrossRefPubMed Wolff, J. J., Dimian, A. F., Botteron, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Hazlett, H. C., Schultz, R. T., Zwaigenbaum, L., Piven, J., & the IBIS Network. (2019). A longitudinal study of parent-reported sensory responsiveness in toddlers at-risk for autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(3), 314–324. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12978CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Relations of Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors to Social Skills in Toddlers with Autism
Auteurs
Pang Chaxiong
Catherine Burrows
Kelly N. Botteron
Stephen R. Dager
Annette M. Estes
Heather C. Hazlett
Robert T. Schultz
Lonnie Zwaigenbaum
Joseph Piven
Jason Wolff
IBIS Network
Publicatiedatum
06-05-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 4/2022
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05014-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2022

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 4/2022 Naar de uitgave