Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies 3/2014

01-04-2014 | Original Paper

Relations Among Sociocognitive Abilities and Prosocial Behavior

Auteur: Bilge Yagmurlu

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies | Uitgave 3/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

To further the understanding of the relations among sociocognitive abilities and social behavior, the current study examined theory of mind (ToM), social information processing (SIP), and prosocial behavior in 116 preschoolers (M age = 58.88 months) in Turkey. False belief tasks were utilized to test ToM and cartoons were used to assess SIP patterns. Prosocial behavior was measured with mother-reports and individual assessments. ToM was not related with the attribution of intent and was the only sociocognitive predictor of prosocial behavior, but just in boys. Results also pointed at sex differences in levels of sociocognitive development; girls showed greater ToM and more non-hostile attribution. Findings imply that SIP patterns might be less closely related to positive than antisocial behaviors, and understanding others’ minds might be less needed for positive acts in Turkish girls, who may learn to engage in such behavior as part of their gender role more strongly.
Literatuur
go back to reference Akhtar, N., & Bradley, E. J. (1991). Social information processing deficits of aggressive children: Present findings and implications for social skills training. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 621–644. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(91)90007-H.CrossRef Akhtar, N., & Bradley, E. J. (1991). Social information processing deficits of aggressive children: Present findings and implications for social skills training. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 621–644. doi:10.​1016/​0272-7358(91)90007-H.CrossRef
go back to reference Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2010). Emotions, aggression, and morality in children: Bridging development and psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2010). Emotions, aggression, and morality in children: Bridging development and psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). New York: Psychology Press. Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Goldberg, M. (1982). Helping behavior among preschool children: An observational study. Child Development, 53, 396–402. doi:10.2307/1128982.CrossRef Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Goldberg, M. (1982). Helping behavior among preschool children: An observational study. Child Development, 53, 396–402. doi:10.​2307/​1128982.CrossRef
go back to reference Bierman, K. L., Smooth, D. L., & Aumiller, K. (1993). Characteristics of aggressive-rejected, aggressive (nonrejected), and rejected (nonaggressive) boys. Child Development, 64, 139–151. doi:10.2307/1131442.PubMedCrossRef Bierman, K. L., Smooth, D. L., & Aumiller, K. (1993). Characteristics of aggressive-rejected, aggressive (nonrejected), and rejected (nonaggressive) boys. Child Development, 64, 139–151. doi:10.​2307/​1131442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blatt-Eisengart, I., Drabick, D. A. G., Monahan, K. C., & Steinberg, L. (2009). Sex differences in the longitudinal relations among family risk factors and childhood externalizing symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 45, 491–502. doi:10.1037/a0014942.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Blatt-Eisengart, I., Drabick, D. A. G., Monahan, K. C., & Steinberg, L. (2009). Sex differences in the longitudinal relations among family risk factors and childhood externalizing symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 45, 491–502. doi:10.​1037/​a0014942.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carlo, G., Knight, G. P., Eisenberg, N., & Rotenberg, K. J. (1991). Cognitive processes and prosocial behaviors among children: The role of affective attributions and reconciliations. Developmental Psychology, 27, 456–461. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.456.CrossRef Carlo, G., Knight, G. P., Eisenberg, N., & Rotenberg, K. J. (1991). Cognitive processes and prosocial behaviors among children: The role of affective attributions and reconciliations. Developmental Psychology, 27, 456–461. doi:10.​1037/​0012-1649.​27.​3.​456.CrossRef
go back to reference Carlo, G., Knight, G. P., McGinley, M., Goodvin, R., & Roesch, S. C. (2010). The developmental relations between perspective taking and prosocial behaviors: A meta-analytic examination of the task-specificity hypothesis. In B. Sokol, U. Müller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 234–269). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Carlo, G., Knight, G. P., McGinley, M., Goodvin, R., & Roesch, S. C. (2010). The developmental relations between perspective taking and prosocial behaviors: A meta-analytic examination of the task-specificity hypothesis. In B. Sokol, U. Müller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 234–269). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Carpendale, J. I., & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Development, 67, 1686–1706. doi:10.2307/1131725.CrossRef Carpendale, J. I., & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Development, 67, 1686–1706. doi:10.​2307/​1131725.CrossRef
go back to reference Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
go back to reference Cillessen, A. H. N., van IJzendoorn, H. W., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Hartup, W. W. (1992). Heterogeneity among peer-rejected boys: Subtypes and stabilities. Child Development, 63, 893–905. doi:10.2307/1131241.CrossRef Cillessen, A. H. N., van IJzendoorn, H. W., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Hartup, W. W. (1992). Heterogeneity among peer-rejected boys: Subtypes and stabilities. Child Development, 63, 893–905. doi:10.​2307/​1131241.CrossRef
go back to reference Cirino, R. J., & Beck, S. J. (1991). Social information processing and the effects of reputational, situational, developmental, and gender factors among children’s sociometric groups. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 561–582. Cirino, R. J., & Beck, S. J. (1991). Social information processing and the effects of reputational, situational, developmental, and gender factors among children’s sociometric groups. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 561–582.
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Rose, A. J. (2000). Toward a gender-balanced approach to the study of social–emotional development. In P. H. Miller & E. Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 153–168). London: Routledge Press. Crick, N. R., & Rose, A. J. (2000). Toward a gender-balanced approach to the study of social–emotional development. In P. H. Miller & E. Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 153–168). London: Routledge Press.
go back to reference Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege and prestige: Occupations in Australia. Melbourne: Longman Cheshire. Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege and prestige: Occupations in Australia. Melbourne: Longman Cheshire.
go back to reference Davis, M., & Stone, T. (2003). Synthesis: Psychological understanding and social skills. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind. Macquarie monographs in cognitive science (pp. 305–352). Hove, E. Sussex: Psychology Press. Davis, M., & Stone, T. (2003). Synthesis: Psychological understanding and social skills. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind. Macquarie monographs in cognitive science (pp. 305–352). Hove, E. Sussex: Psychology Press.
go back to reference de Castro, B. O., Veerman, W. J., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73, 916–934. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00447.CrossRef de Castro, B. O., Veerman, W. J., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73, 916–934. doi:10.​1111/​1467-8624.​00447.CrossRef
go back to reference Dekovic, M., & Gerris, J. R. M. (1994). Developmental analysis of social cognitive and behavioral differences between popular and rejected children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 367–386. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(94)90038-8.CrossRef Dekovic, M., & Gerris, J. R. M. (1994). Developmental analysis of social cognitive and behavioral differences between popular and rejected children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 367–386. doi:10.​1016/​0193-3973(94)90038-8.CrossRef
go back to reference Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201. doi:10.2307/1130651.CrossRef Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201. doi:10.​2307/​1130651.CrossRef
go back to reference Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), The Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), The Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Minore, D., Mathy, R., Hanish, L., & Brown, T. (1994). Children’s enacted interpersonal strategies: Their relations to social behavior and negative emotionality. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 212–232. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Minore, D., Mathy, R., Hanish, L., & Brown, T. (1994). Children’s enacted interpersonal strategies: Their relations to social behavior and negative emotionality. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 212–232.
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Carlo, G., & Miller, P. A. (1991). The relations of parental characteristics and practices to children’s vicarious emotional responding. Child Development, 62, 1393–1408. doi:10.2307/1130814.PubMedCrossRef Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Carlo, G., & Miller, P. A. (1991). The relations of parental characteristics and practices to children’s vicarious emotional responding. Child Development, 62, 1393–1408. doi:10.​2307/​1130814.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646–718). New York: Wiley. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646–718). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Eron, L. D. (2001). Seeing is believing: How viewing violence alters attitudes and aggressive behavior. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 49–60). Washington: American psychological association. doi:10.1037/10433-002.CrossRef Eron, L. D. (2001). Seeing is believing: How viewing violence alters attitudes and aggressive behavior. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 49–60). Washington: American psychological association. doi:10.​1037/​10433-002.CrossRef
go back to reference Etel, E., & Yagmurlu, B. (2012). Theory of mind, executive function, and social competence: Findings from institution-reared Turkish children. Manuscript submitted for publication. Etel, E., & Yagmurlu, B. (2012). Theory of mind, executive function, and social competence: Findings from institution-reared Turkish children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Feldman, E., & Dodge, K. A. (1987). Social information processing and sociometric status: Sex, age, and situational effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 211–227. doi:10.1007/BF00916350.PubMedCrossRef Feldman, E., & Dodge, K. A. (1987). Social information processing and sociometric status: Sex, age, and situational effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 211–227. doi:10.​1007/​BF00916350.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fraser, M. W., Galinsky, M. J., Smokowski, P. R., Day, S. H., Terzian, M. A., Rose, R. A., et al. (2005). Social information-processing skills training to promote social competence and prevent aggressive behavior in the third grade. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1045–1055. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.6.1045.PubMedCrossRef Fraser, M. W., Galinsky, M. J., Smokowski, P. R., Day, S. H., Terzian, M. A., Rose, R. A., et al. (2005). Social information-processing skills training to promote social competence and prevent aggressive behavior in the third grade. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1045–1055. doi:10.​1037/​0022-006X.​73.​6.​1045.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 281–313). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 281–313). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Huessman, L. R., & Reynolds, M. A. (2001). Cognitive processes and the development of aggression. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 249–269). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10433-012.CrossRef Huessman, L. R., & Reynolds, M. A. (2001). Cognitive processes and the development of aggression. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 249–269). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.​1037/​10433-012.CrossRef
go back to reference Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2010). Do early social cognition and executive function predict individual differences in preschoolers’ prosocial and antisocial behavior? In B. Sokol, U. Müller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 418–442). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2010). Do early social cognition and executive function predict individual differences in preschoolers’ prosocial and antisocial behavior? In B. Sokol, U. Müller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 418–442). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Iannotti, J. J., Cummings, E. M., Pierrehumbert, B., Milano, M. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Parental influences on prosocial behavior and empathy in early childhood. In J. M. A. M. Janssens & J. R. M. Gerris (Eds.), Child rearing. Influence on prosocial and moral development (pp. 77–100). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. Iannotti, J. J., Cummings, E. M., Pierrehumbert, B., Milano, M. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Parental influences on prosocial behavior and empathy in early childhood. In J. M. A. M. Janssens & J. R. M. Gerris (Eds.), Child rearing. Influence on prosocial and moral development (pp. 77–100). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.
go back to reference Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 46, 203–220. Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 46, 203–220.
go back to reference Kagitcibasi, C. (1982). Sex roles, value of children and fertility in Turkey. In C. Kagitcibasi (Ed.), Sex roles, family, and community in Turkey (pp. 151–180). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Kagitcibasi, C. (1982). Sex roles, value of children and fertility in Turkey. In C. Kagitcibasi (Ed.), Sex roles, family, and community in Turkey (pp. 151–180). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
go back to reference Krevans, J., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Parents’ use of inductive discipline: Relations to children’s empathy and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67, 3263–3277. doi:10.2307/1131778.PubMedCrossRef Krevans, J., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Parents’ use of inductive discipline: Relations to children’s empathy and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67, 3263–3277. doi:10.​2307/​1131778.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Dodge, K. A., Crozier, J. C., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2006). A 12-year prospective study of patterns of social information processing problems and externalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 709–718. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9057-4.CrossRef Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Dodge, K. A., Crozier, J. C., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2006). A 12-year prospective study of patterns of social information processing problems and externalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 709–718. doi:10.​1007/​s10802-006-9057-4.CrossRef
go back to reference Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2007). The socialization of gender. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization. Theory and research (pp. 561–587). New York: The Guilford Press. Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2007). The socialization of gender. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization. Theory and research (pp. 561–587). New York: The Guilford Press.
go back to reference Lucariello, J. M., Durand, T. M., & Yarnell, L. (2007). Social versus intrapersonal ToM: Social ToM is a cognitive strength for low- and middle-SES children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 285–297. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2007.04.001.CrossRef Lucariello, J. M., Durand, T. M., & Yarnell, L. (2007). Social versus intrapersonal ToM: Social ToM is a cognitive strength for low- and middle-SES children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 285–297. doi:10.​1016/​j.​appdev.​2007.​04.​001.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayeux, L., & Cillessen, A. H. (2003). Development of social problem solving in early childhood: stability, change, and associations with social competence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 153–173.PubMedCrossRef Mayeux, L., & Cillessen, A. H. (2003). Development of social problem solving in early childhood: stability, change, and associations with social competence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 153–173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olson, D. R., Astington, J. W., & Harris, P. L. (1988). Introduction. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 1–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Olson, D. R., Astington, J. W., & Harris, P. L. (1988). Introduction. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 1–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137. doi:10.2307/1131316.CrossRef Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137. doi:10.​2307/​1131316.CrossRef
go back to reference Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Brown, M. M. (1988). Early family experience, social problem solving patterns, and children’s social competence. Child Development, 59, 107–120.CrossRef Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Brown, M. M. (1988). Early family experience, social problem solving patterns, and children’s social competence. Child Development, 59, 107–120.CrossRef
go back to reference Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social information processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 1305–1320. doi:10.2307/1131557.PubMedCrossRef Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social information processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 1305–1320. doi:10.​2307/​1131557.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Zelazo, P. D., Boivin, M., et al. (2010). Relations between theory of mind and indirect and physical aggression in kindergarten: Evidence of the moderating role of prosocial behaviors. Social Development, 19, 535–555. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00552.x.CrossRef Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Zelazo, P. D., Boivin, M., et al. (2010). Relations between theory of mind and indirect and physical aggression in kindergarten: Evidence of the moderating role of prosocial behaviors. Social Development, 19, 535–555. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1467-9507.​2009.​00552.​x.CrossRef
go back to reference Rubin, K., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1992). Interpersonal problem solving and social competence in children. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of social development: A lifespan perspective (pp. 283–323). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef Rubin, K., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1992). Interpersonal problem solving and social competence in children. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of social development: A lifespan perspective (pp. 283–323). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Sanson, A., Hemphill, S., Yagmurlu, B., & McClowry, S. G. (2011). Temperament and social development. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (2nd ed., pp. 227–245). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRef Sanson, A., Hemphill, S., Yagmurlu, B., & McClowry, S. G. (2011). Temperament and social development. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (2nd ed., pp. 227–245). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRef
go back to reference Schultz, D., Ambike, A., Logie, S. K., Bohner, K. E., Stapleton, L. M., VanderWalde, H., et al. (2010). Assessment of social information processing in early childhood: Development and initial validation of the Schultz test of emotion processing-preliminary version. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 601–613. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9390-5.PubMedCrossRef Schultz, D., Ambike, A., Logie, S. K., Bohner, K. E., Stapleton, L. M., VanderWalde, H., et al. (2010). Assessment of social information processing in early childhood: Development and initial validation of the Schultz test of emotion processing-preliminary version. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 601–613. doi:10.​1007/​s10802-010-9390-5.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Suess, G. J., Grossman, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to individual organisation of self. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 43–65. doi:10.1177/016502549201500103.CrossRef Suess, G. J., Grossman, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to individual organisation of self. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 43–65. doi:10.​1177/​0165025492015001​03.CrossRef
go back to reference Sullivan, K., & Winner, E. (1991). When 3-year-olds understand ignorance, false belief and representational change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 159–171.CrossRef Sullivan, K., & Winner, E. (1991). When 3-year-olds understand ignorance, false belief and representational change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 159–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique of the ‘social skills deficit’ view of anti-social behavior. Social Development, 8, 117–127. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00083.CrossRef Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique of the ‘social skills deficit’ view of anti-social behavior. Social Development, 8, 117–127. doi:10.​1111/​1467-9507.​00083.CrossRef
go back to reference Vitaro, F., & Pelletier, D. (1991). Assessment of children’s social problem-solving skills in hypothetical and actual conflict situations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 505–518. doi:10.1007/BF00925817.PubMedCrossRef Vitaro, F., & Pelletier, D. (1991). Assessment of children’s social problem-solving skills in hypothetical and actual conflict situations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 505–518. doi:10.​1007/​BF00925817.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Werner, R. S., Cassidy, K. W., & Juliano, M. (2006). The role of social-cognitive abilities in preschoolers’ aggressive behavior. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 775–799. doi:10.1348/026151005X78799.CrossRef Werner, R. S., Cassidy, K. W., & Juliano, M. (2006). The role of social-cognitive abilities in preschoolers’ aggressive behavior. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 775–799. doi:10.​1348/​026151005X78799.CrossRef
go back to reference Yagmurlu, B., Citlak, B., Dost, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2009). Child socialization goals of Turkish mothers: An investigation of education related within-culture variation. Turkish Journal of Psychology, 24, 1–15. Yagmurlu, B., Citlak, B., Dost, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2009). Child socialization goals of Turkish mothers: An investigation of education related within-culture variation. Turkish Journal of Psychology, 24, 1–15.
Metagegevens
Titel
Relations Among Sociocognitive Abilities and Prosocial Behavior
Auteur
Bilge Yagmurlu
Publicatiedatum
01-04-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Child and Family Studies / Uitgave 3/2014
Print ISSN: 1062-1024
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2843
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9726-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2014

Journal of Child and Family Studies 3/2014 Naar de uitgave