Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-05-2010

Relational Aggression and Hostile Attribution Biases: Testing Multiple Statistical Methods and Models

Auteurs: Stephanie A. Godleski, Jamie M. Ostrov

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 4/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present study used both categorical and dimensional approaches to test the association between relational and physical aggression and hostile intent attributions for both relational and instrumental provocation situations using the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development longitudinal Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (M = 8.3 years-old, SD = 0.19, N = 840). A one standard deviation threshold was used to create categorical aggression status groups. In categorical analyses, children who were both relationally and physically aggressive were found to attribute more hostile intent attributions in relationally provocative situations. Results from the dimensional analyses indicated that female gender, physical aggression, and hostile attribution biases for instrumental provocations each predicted increases in relational aggression over time. Further, relational aggression was not predictive of hostile attribution biases for relational provocations, and instead physical aggression was predictive. Implications for the fields of developmental psychology and psychopathology are discussed.
Voetnoten
1
Mother-report data (n = 903) was also analyzed and results were examined using the same statistical procedures. Deviations between mother- and teacher-report in predicting the various dependent variables are available by contacting the first author. Teacher-report was used instead of mother-report as teachers might be more valid informants of peer relations and aggression as teachers may be more objective and at reduced risk of social desirability concerns. Further, teachers are cited as being more appropriate reporters of relational aggression due to the externalizing nature of the behavior in middle childhood (Crick et al. 2007). There is also limited use of the Children Behavior with Peers measure with parents, as it is most frequently used with teachers (e.g., Crick 1996; Crick et al. 1996) and parent-report of relational aggression appears to be of questionable psychometric properties (Casas et al. 2006).
 
2
Two 4 (Aggression status: Relational, physical, comorbid, non-aggressive) x 2 (Racial Status: Minority and non-minority status) x 2 (Gender) ANOVA’s were conducted with either child-reported hostile attribution biases for relational provocations or instrumental provocations as the dependent variable. There were no significant main effects of racial status [F(1, 840) = 1.66, p = 0.20, ηp 2 = 0.002 for relational provocations and F(1, 840) = 2.05, p = 0.15, ηp 2 = 0.002 for instrumental provocations] and no significant interactions with racial status.
 
3
Simple effects of the physical aggression by gender interaction predicting instrumental hostile attribution biases were also run considering physical aggression as dichotomous (i.e., either no reported aggression or reported aggression). Using this criterion physical aggression was a significant predictor of instrumental hostile attribution biases above and beyond relational aggression and gender when there was reported aggression [F(3,156) = 2.86, p = 0.039, R 2 = 0.052].
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist: 4-18 and profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist: 4-18 and profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
go back to reference Casas, J. F., Weingel, S. M., Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., Woods, K. E., Jansen Yeh, E. A., et al. (2006). Early parenting and children’s relational and physical aggression in the preschool and home contexts. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 209–227.CrossRef Casas, J. F., Weingel, S. M., Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., Woods, K. E., Jansen Yeh, E. A., et al. (2006). Early parenting and children’s relational and physical aggression in the preschool and home contexts. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 209–227.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, L. A. (1993). Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 505–521.CrossRef Clark, L. A. (1993). Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 505–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, L. A. (1999). Dimensional approaches to personality disorder assessment and diagnosis. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 219–244). Washington: American Psychiatric. Clark, L. A. (1999). Dimensional approaches to personality disorder assessment and diagnosis. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 219–244). Washington: American Psychiatric.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1983). The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement, 7(3), 249–253.CrossRef Cohen, J. (1983). The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement, 7(3), 249–253.CrossRef
go back to reference Collett, B., Ohan, J., & Myers, K. (2003). Ten-year review of the rating scales. VI: Scales assessing externalizing behaviors. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1143–1170.CrossRefPubMed Collett, B., Ohan, J., & Myers, K. (2003). Ten-year review of the rating scales. VI: Scales assessing externalizing behaviors. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1143–1170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crain, M. M., Finch, C. L., & Foster, S. L. (2005). The relevance of the social information processing model for understanding relational aggression in girls. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51(2), 213–249.CrossRef Crain, M. M., Finch, C. L., & Foster, S. L. (2005). The relevance of the social information processing model for understanding relational aggression in girls. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51(2), 213–249.CrossRef
go back to reference Crick, N. R. (1995). Relational aggression: the role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation type. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 313–322.CrossRef Crick, N. R. (1995). Relational aggression: the role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation type. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 313–322.CrossRef
go back to reference Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child Development, 67, 2317–2327.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child Development, 67, 2317–2327.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R. (1997). Engagement in gender normative versus non-normative forms of aggression: links to social-psychological adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 610–617.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R. (1997). Engagement in gender normative versus non-normative forms of aggression: links to social-psychological adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 610–617.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.CrossRef Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.CrossRef
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children’s perceptions of their peer experiences: attributions, loneliness, social avoidance, and social anxiety. Developmental Psychology, 29, 244–254.CrossRef Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children’s perceptions of their peer experiences: attributions, loneliness, social avoidance, and social anxiety. Developmental Psychology, 29, 244–254.CrossRef
go back to reference Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differences in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: how do I hurt thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003–1014.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differences in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: how do I hurt thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003–1014.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Bigbee, M. A. (2002). Relationally and physically aggressive children’s intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations. Child Development, 73, 1134–1142.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Bigbee, M. A. (2002). Relationally and physically aggressive children’s intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations. Child Development, 73, 1134–1142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Werner, N. E. (2006). A longitudinal study of relational aggression, physical aggression and children’s social-psychological adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Werner, N. E. (2006). A longitudinal study of relational aggression, physical aggression and children’s social-psychological adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Kawabata, Y. (2007). Relational aggression and gender: An overview. In D. J. Flannery, I. Waldman, & A. Vazsonyi (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression (245–259). Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Kawabata, Y. (2007). Relational aggression and gender: An overview. In D. J. Flannery, I. Waldman, & A. Vazsonyi (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression (245–259).
go back to reference Cross, S. E., & Madsen, L. (1997). Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.CrossRefPubMed Cross, S. E., & Madsen, L. (1997). Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive self-perceptions have a “dark side”? Examination of the link between perceptual bias and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 327–337.CrossRefPubMed David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive self-perceptions have a “dark side”? Examination of the link between perceptual bias and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 327–337.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. (2006). Within-child associations between family income and internalizing and externalizing problems. Developmental Psychology, 37, 230–252. Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. (2006). Within-child associations between family income and internalizing and externalizing problems. Developmental Psychology, 37, 230–252.
go back to reference Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162–170.CrossRefPubMed Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162–170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Development, 53, 620–635.CrossRefPubMed Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Development, 53, 620–635.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. R. (1987). Hostile attributional biases among aggressive boys are exacerbated under conditions of threat to self. Child Development, 58, 213–224.CrossRefPubMed Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. R. (1987). Hostile attributional biases among aggressive boys are exacerbated under conditions of threat to self. Child Development, 58, 213–224.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in children: implications for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 163–173.CrossRefPubMed Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in children: implications for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 163–173.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dodge, K. A, Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, J. (1986). Social competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44(2). Dodge, K. A, Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, J. (1986). Social competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44(2).
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683.CrossRefPubMed Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 719–788). New York: Wiley. Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 719–788). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Dwyer, C. A. (1996). Cut scores and testing: statistics, judgment, truth, and error. Psychological Assessment, 8, 360–362.CrossRef Dwyer, C. A. (1996). Cut scores and testing: statistics, judgment, truth, and error. Psychological Assessment, 8, 360–362.CrossRef
go back to reference Grotpeter, J. F., & Crick, N. R. (1996). Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Development, 67, 2328–2338.CrossRefPubMed Grotpeter, J. F., & Crick, N. R. (1996). Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Development, 67, 2328–2338.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Leff, S. S., Kupersmidt, J. B., Patterson, C. J., & Power, T. J. (1999). Factors influencing teacher identification of peer bullies and victims. School Psychology Review, 28(3), 505–517. Leff, S. S., Kupersmidt, J. B., Patterson, C. J., & Power, T. J. (1999). Factors influencing teacher identification of peer bullies and victims. School Psychology Review, 28(3), 505–517.
go back to reference Leff, S. S., Crick, N. R., Angelucci, J., Haye, K., Jawad, A. F., Grossman, M., et al. (2006). Social cognition in context: validating a cartoon-based attributional measure for urban girls. Child Development, 77(5), 1351–1358.CrossRefPubMed Leff, S. S., Crick, N. R., Angelucci, J., Haye, K., Jawad, A. F., Grossman, M., et al. (2006). Social cognition in context: validating a cartoon-based attributional measure for urban girls. Child Development, 77(5), 1351–1358.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Leff, S. S., Lakin Gullan, R., Paskewich, B. S., Abdul-Kabir, S., Jawad, A. F., Grossman, M., et al. (2009). An initial evaluation of a culturally adapted social problem-solving and relational aggression prevention program for urban African–American relationally aggressive girls. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 37(4), 260–274.CrossRef Leff, S. S., Lakin Gullan, R., Paskewich, B. S., Abdul-Kabir, S., Jawad, A. F., Grossman, M., et al. (2009). An initial evaluation of a culturally adapted social problem-solving and relational aggression prevention program for urban African–American relationally aggressive girls. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 37(4), 260–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Little, T. D., Jones, S. M., Henrich, C. C., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behavior. International Journal of Behavior Development, 27(2), 122–133.CrossRef Little, T. D., Jones, S. M., Henrich, C. C., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behavior. International Journal of Behavior Development, 27(2), 122–133.CrossRef
go back to reference MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practices of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 19–40.CrossRefPubMed MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practices of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 19–40.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Maughan, B. (2005). Developmental trajectory modeling: a view from developmental psychopathology. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 602, 118–130.CrossRef Maughan, B. (2005). Developmental trajectory modeling: a view from developmental psychopathology. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 602, 118–130.CrossRef
go back to reference Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstrap taxometrics: solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, 50(4), 266–275.CrossRefPubMed Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstrap taxometrics: solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, 50(4), 266–275.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: an overview of the NICHD study of early child care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 457–492.CrossRef NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: an overview of the NICHD study of early child care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 457–492.CrossRef
go back to reference NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004).Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4). NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004).Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4).
go back to reference NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and child development: Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. New York: Guilford. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and child development: Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. New York: Guilford.
go back to reference NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, NICHD, & Duncan, G. J. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children’s preschool cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 1485–1506.CrossRef NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, NICHD, & Duncan, G. J. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children’s preschool cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 1485–1506.CrossRef
go back to reference Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monsouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916–934.CrossRefPubMed Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monsouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916–934.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Forms and functions of adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49(3), 310–342.CrossRef Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Forms and functions of adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49(3), 310–342.CrossRef
go back to reference Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social information- processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 1305–1320.CrossRefPubMed Quiggle, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social information- processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development, 63, 1305–1320.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rabiner, D. L., & Coie, J. D. (1989). The effect of expectancy inductions on rejected children’s acceptance by unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 450–457.CrossRef Rabiner, D. L., & Coie, J. D. (1989). The effect of expectancy inductions on rejected children’s acceptance by unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 450–457.CrossRef
go back to reference Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.CrossRefPubMed Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Lindberg, N., Herzberg, D., & Daley, S. E. (2000). Toward an interpersonal life-stress model of depression: the developmental context of stress generation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 215–234.CrossRefPubMed Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Lindberg, N., Herzberg, D., & Daley, S. E. (2000). Toward an interpersonal life-stress model of depression: the developmental context of stress generation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 215–234.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Hubbard, J. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Lemerise, E. A., et al. (1998). Social-cognitive and behavioral correlates of aggression and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(6), 431–440.CrossRefPubMed Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Hubbard, J. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Lemerise, E. A., et al. (1998). Social-cognitive and behavioral correlates of aggression and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(6), 431–440.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 251–286.CrossRefPubMed Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 251–286.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 601–609.CrossRefPubMed Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 601–609.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Widiger, T. A. (1992). Categorical versus dimensional classification: implications from and for research. Journal of Personality Disorder, 6, 287–300. Widiger, T. A. (1992). Categorical versus dimensional classification: implications from and for research. Journal of Personality Disorder, 6, 287–300.
go back to reference Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005). Diagnostic categories or dimensions? A question for the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-fifth edition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 494–504.CrossRefPubMed Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005). Diagnostic categories or dimensions? A question for the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-fifth edition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 494–504.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62(2), 71–83.CrossRefPubMed Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62(2), 71–83.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zalecki, C., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2004). Overt and relational aggression in girls with attention- deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 131–143. Zalecki, C., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2004). Overt and relational aggression in girls with attention- deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 131–143.
Metagegevens
Titel
Relational Aggression and Hostile Attribution Biases: Testing Multiple Statistical Methods and Models
Auteurs
Stephanie A. Godleski
Jamie M. Ostrov
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 4/2010
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9391-4