Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-01-2013 | Original paper

(Re-)conceptualisation in Asperger’s Syndrome and Typical Individuals with Varying Degrees of Autistic-like Traits

Auteurs: Hollie G. Burnett, Tjeerd Jellema

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 1/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The abilities to form new concepts from scratch (conceptualisation), and to flexibly switch from one concept to another (re-conceptualisation), were investigated in adults with Asperger’s Syndrome and in typically-developed adults with low and high autism spectrum quotients. In consecutively presented morphs, containing increasing percentages of animate or inanimate objects, the emerging objects had to be identified. The abilities to conceptualise and reconceptualise became increasingly impaired with increasing autistic(-like) traits. Across both tasks, all groups recognised animate objects quicker than inanimate objects. However, this ‘animate advantage’ was differently affected by the two tasks. In the Reconceptualisation task, the ‘animate advantage’ gradually disappeared with increasing autistic(-like) traits, whereas in the Conceptualisation task it remained present.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469–479.PubMedCrossRef Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469–479.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 267–278.PubMedCrossRef Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 267–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). A new step towards understanding embedded figures test performance in the autism spectrum: The radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 374–381.PubMedCrossRef Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). A new step towards understanding embedded figures test performance in the autism spectrum: The radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 374–381.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathising system: A revision of the 1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind. Guilford: Guilford Publications Inc. Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathising system: A revision of the 1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind. Guilford: Guilford Publications Inc.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., & Swettenham, J. (1997). Theory of mind in autism: Its relationship to executive functioning and central coherence. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley. Baron-Cohen, S., & Swettenham, J. (1997). Theory of mind in autism: Its relationship to executive functioning and central coherence. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, S., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger’s Syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, S., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger’s Syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bayliss, A. P., Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 631–650. Bayliss, A. P., Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 631–650.
go back to reference Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism-spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 95–114.PubMedCrossRef Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism-spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 95–114.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (2002). Parallel visual motions processing streams for manipulable objects and human movements. Neuron, 34, 149–159.PubMedCrossRef Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (2002). Parallel visual motions processing streams for manipulable objects and human movements. Neuron, 34, 149–159.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blair, R. J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abel, F., & Cipolotti, L. (2001). Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 108–118.CrossRef Blair, R. J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abel, F., & Cipolotti, L. (2001). Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40, 108–118.CrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.PubMed Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.PubMed
go back to reference Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.PubMedCrossRef Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bowler, D. M., & Thommen, E. (2000). Attribution of mechanical and social causality to animated displays by children with autism. Autism, 4, 147–171.CrossRef Bowler, D. M., & Thommen, E. (2000). Attribution of mechanical and social causality to animated displays by children with autism. Autism, 4, 147–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience, 1, 27–51. Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience, 1, 27–51.
go back to reference Bruner, J., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1967). A study of thinking. New York: Science Editions. Bruner, J., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1967). A study of thinking. New York: Science Editions.
go back to reference Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger Syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger Syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cesaroni, L., & Garber, M. (1991). Exploring the experience of autism through first-hand accounts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 201–303.CrossRef Cesaroni, L., & Garber, M. (1991). Exploring the experience of autism through first-hand accounts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 201–303.CrossRef
go back to reference Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.PubMedCrossRef Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cipolotti, L., Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Frith, U. (1999). Fractionation of visual memory: Evidence from a case with multiple neurodevelopmental impairments. Neuropsychologia, 37, 455–465.PubMedCrossRef Cipolotti, L., Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Frith, U. (1999). Fractionation of visual memory: Evidence from a case with multiple neurodevelopmental impairments. Neuropsychologia, 37, 455–465.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebella patients. Behavioural Neuroscience, 108, 848–865.CrossRef Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebella patients. Behavioural Neuroscience, 108, 848–865.CrossRef
go back to reference Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.PubMedCrossRef Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Winter, J., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Contour-based object identification and segmentation: Stimuli, norms and data, and software tools. Behaviour Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36, 604–624.CrossRef De Winter, J., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Contour-based object identification and segmentation: Stimuli, norms and data, and software tools. Behaviour Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36, 604–624.CrossRef
go back to reference Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The delis–kaplan executive function system. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The delis–kaplan executive function system. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Downing, P. E., Bray, D., Rogers, J., & Childs, C. (2004). Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected. Cognition, 93, 27–38.CrossRef Downing, P. E., Bray, D., Rogers, J., & Childs, C. (2004). Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected. Cognition, 93, 27–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Dunbar, R. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178–190.CrossRef Dunbar, R. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Eylen, L. V., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1390–1401.CrossRef Eylen, L. V., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1390–1401.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
go back to reference Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds: A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.PubMedCrossRef Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds: A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder—autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 433–438.PubMedCrossRef Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder—autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 433–438.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fuggetta, G., Rizzo, S., Pobric, G., Lavidor, M., & Walsh, V. (2009). Functional representation of living and nonliving domains across the cerebral hemispheres: A combined event-related potential/transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 403–414.PubMedCrossRef Fuggetta, G., Rizzo, S., Pobric, G., Lavidor, M., & Walsh, V. (2009). Functional representation of living and nonliving domains across the cerebral hemispheres: A combined event-related potential/transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 403–414.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 74–82.PubMedCrossRef Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 74–82.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press. Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures. New York: Doubleday Publisher. Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures. New York: Doubleday Publisher.
go back to reference Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Van Beek, P., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2009). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1278–1290.PubMedCrossRef Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Van Beek, P., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2009). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1278–1290.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hill, E., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger Syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.PubMedCrossRef Hill, E., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger Syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hobson, P. R. (1987). The autistic child’s recognition of age and sex related characteristics of people. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 63–69.PubMedCrossRef Hobson, P. R. (1987). The autistic child’s recognition of age and sex related characteristics of people. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 63–69.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hock, H. S., Kelso, J. A. S., & Schoner, G. (1993). Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 63–80.PubMedCrossRef Hock, H. S., Kelso, J. A. S., & Schoner, G. (1993). Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 63–80.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.CrossRef Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555–1566.CrossRef
go back to reference Horng, L. I. (2003). An adaptive smoothing approach for fitting digital planar curves with line segments and circular arcs. Pattern Recognition Letters, 24, 565–577.CrossRef Horng, L. I. (2003). An adaptive smoothing approach for fitting digital planar curves with line segments and circular arcs. Pattern Recognition Letters, 24, 565–577.CrossRef
go back to reference Hughes, C. H., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 10–498.CrossRef Hughes, C. H., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 10–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Jellema, T., Lorteije, J. A. M., Van Rijn, S., Van ‘t Wout, M., De Haan, E. H. F., Van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary processing of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192–204.PubMedCrossRef Jellema, T., Lorteije, J. A. M., Van Rijn, S., Van ‘t Wout, M., De Haan, E. H. F., Van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary processing of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192–204.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden objects. Attention and Performance, 19, 356–380. Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Coding of visible and hidden objects. Attention and Performance, 19, 356–380.
go back to reference Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Neural basis for the perception of goal-directed actions. In A. Easton & N. J. Emery (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of social behaviour (pp. 81–112). New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef Jellema, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Neural basis for the perception of goal-directed actions. In A. Easton & N. J. Emery (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of social behaviour (pp. 81–112). New York: Psychology Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, C. R., & Rakison, D. H. (2006). Early categorization of animate/inanimate concepts in young children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 18, 73–89.CrossRef Johnson, C. R., & Rakison, D. H. (2006). Early categorization of animate/inanimate concepts in young children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 18, 73–89.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: Cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with aspergers syndrome or high functioning autism as measured on the computerised version of the Wisconsin card sorting test. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 38, 1161–1165.CrossRef Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: Cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with aspergers syndrome or high functioning autism as measured on the computerised version of the Wisconsin card sorting test. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 38, 1161–1165.CrossRef
go back to reference Kanner, L., & Lesser, L. I. (1958). Early infantile autism. Paediatrics Clinic of North America, 5, 711–730. Kanner, L., & Lesser, L. I. (1958). Early infantile autism. Paediatrics Clinic of North America, 5, 711–730.
go back to reference Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and asperger’s disorder. Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 379–401.PubMedCrossRef Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and asperger’s disorder. Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 379–401.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 111–124.PubMedCrossRef Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 111–124.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–1122.PubMedCrossRef Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–1122.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.PubMedCrossRef Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2002). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review, Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.CrossRef Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2002). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review, Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.CrossRef
go back to reference Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A disassociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.PubMedCrossRef Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A disassociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.PubMedCrossRef Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228.PubMedCrossRef Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Russell, J. (1998). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Russell, J. (1998). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Rutherford, M. D., Pennington, B. P., & Rogers, S. J. (2006). The perception of animacy in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 983–992.PubMedCrossRef Rutherford, M. D., Pennington, B. P., & Rogers, S. J. (2006). The perception of animacy in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 983–992.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 139–161.CrossRef Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 139–161.CrossRef
go back to reference Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.PubMedCrossRef Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Snodgrass, J. G., & Van der Wart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.CrossRef Snodgrass, J. G., & Van der Wart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Stewart, M. E., Watson, J., Allcock, A. J., & Yaqoob, T. (2009). Autistic traits predict performance on the block design. Autism, 13, 133–142.PubMedCrossRef Stewart, M. E., Watson, J., Allcock, A. J., & Yaqoob, T. (2009). Autistic traits predict performance on the block design. Autism, 13, 133–142.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 747–753.PubMedCrossRef Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 747–753.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van ‘t Wout, M., Van Rijn, S., Jellema, T., Kahn, R. S., & Aleman, A. (2009). Deficits in implicit attention to social signals in schizophrenia and high risk groups: Behavioural evidence from a new illusion. PLoS ONE, 4(e5581), 1–9. Van ‘t Wout, M., Van Rijn, S., Jellema, T., Kahn, R. S., & Aleman, A. (2009). Deficits in implicit attention to social signals in schizophrenia and high risk groups: Behavioural evidence from a new illusion. PLoS ONE, 4(e5581), 1–9.
go back to reference Verbeke, E., Peeters, W., Kerkhof, I., Bijttebier, P., Steyaert, J., & Wagemans, J. (2005). Lack of motivation to share intentions: Primary deficit in autism? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 718–719.CrossRef Verbeke, E., Peeters, W., Kerkhof, I., Bijttebier, P., Steyaert, J., & Wagemans, J. (2005). Lack of motivation to share intentions: Primary deficit in autism? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 718–719.CrossRef
go back to reference Verstijnen, I. M., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Ambiguous figures: Living versus nonliving objects. Perception, 33, 531–546.PubMedCrossRef Verstijnen, I. M., & Wagemans, J. (2004). Ambiguous figures: Living versus nonliving objects. Perception, 33, 531–546.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.PubMedCrossRef Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale–third edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Wechsler adult intelligence scale–third edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wheatly, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.CrossRef Wheatly, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.CrossRef
go back to reference Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al. (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.PubMedCrossRef Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al. (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD–10 classification of diseases clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO. World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD–10 classification of diseases clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO.
Metagegevens
Titel
(Re-)conceptualisation in Asperger’s Syndrome and Typical Individuals with Varying Degrees of Autistic-like Traits
Auteurs
Hollie G. Burnett
Tjeerd Jellema
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 1/2013
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1567-z