Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research 2/2016

Open Access 20-08-2015

Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints

Auteurs: Marieke Anna de Ruiter, Antoinette Yvonne Narda Schouten-van Meeteren, Dannis Gilbert van Vuurden, Heleen Maurice-Stam, Corrie Gidding, Laura Rachel Beek, Bernd Granzen, Jaap Oosterlaan, Martha Alexandra Grootenhuis

Gepubliceerd in: Quality of Life Research | Uitgave 2/2016

Abstract

Purpose

With more children surviving a brain tumor, neurocognitive consequences of the tumor and its treatment become apparent, which could affect psychosocial functioning. The present study therefore aimed to assess psychosocial functioning of pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS) in detail.

Methods

Psychosocial functioning of PBTS (8–18 years) with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints was compared to normative data on health-related quality of life (HRQOL), self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, and executive functioning (one-sample t tests) and to a sibling control group on fatigue (independent-samples t test). Self-, parent-, and teacher-report questionnaires were included, where appropriate, providing complementary information.

Results

Eighty-two PBTS (mean age 13.4 years, SD 3.2, 49 % males) and 43 healthy siblings (mean age 14.3, SD 2.4, 40 % males) were included. As compared to the normative population, PBTS themselves reported decreased physical, psychological, and generic HRQOL (d = 0.39–0.62, p < 0.008). Compared to siblings, increased fatigue-related concentration problems (d = 0.57, p < 0.01) were reported, although self-reported self-esteem and psychosocial adjustment seemed not to be affected. Parents of PBTS reported more psychosocial (d = 0.81, p < 0.000) and executive problems (d = 0.35–0.43, p < 0.016) in their child than parents of children in the normative population. Teachers indicated more psychosocial adjustment problems for female PBTS aged 8–11 years than for the female normative population (d = 0.69, p < 0.025), but they reported no more executive problems.

Conclusions

PBTS with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints showed increased psychosocial problems, as reported by PBTS, parents, and teachers.

Implications for cancer survivors

Systematic screening of psychosocial functioning is necessary so that tailored support from professionals can be offered to PBTS with neurocognitive complaints.

Introduction

Due to developments in the medical field, survival rates in children with a brain tumor have increased drastically to over 74 % [1]. These successes have led to a growing number of pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS). Tumor- and treatment-induced brain injury exerts negative effects on neurocognitive functions, such as attention, processing speed, and memory [2]. As a result, 40–100 % of PBTS suffer from neurocognitive decline [3]. The decline in neurocognitive functioning appears to increase when the children grow older, resulting in an increasing gap between the PBTS and their peers [46]. Consequently, children treated for a brain tumor may experience lower academic achievements, resulting in lower vocational success, and decreased psychosocial functioning compared to their healthy peers later in life [79].
To date, studies on psychosocial functioning of PBTS are relatively scarce as compared to other types of cancer, especially because children with a brain tumor have often been excluded from studies, due to their atypical outcomes, i.e., they seem to suffer from more serious problems on a variety of domains (e.g., neurocognitive, social, and adjustment problems) than other pediatric cancer survivors [9]. The studies with PBTS found in the literature focused on health-related quality of life (HRQOL), social competence, self-esteem, and fatigue. Attention for HRQOL, a multidimensional construct covering perceived physical, emotional, mental, social, and behavioral components of well-being and functioning [10], in PBTS has started to emerge in the past decades [11]. However, no studies to our knowledge have focused on PBTS with neurocognitive complaints. The results of the studies on HRQOL in PBTS in general were contradictory, with HRQOL comparable to the general population [12], or worse HRQOL in several domains [13]. PBTS reported being bullied, encountering problems with peers, and suffering from stressful and depressive feelings. The researchers mention late effects of the cancer treatment as a possible cause of the decreased HRQOL scores. Decreased neurocognitive functioning was found to be associated with worse HRQOL in PBTS 1 year after treatment [14].
The literature on self-esteem in PBTS is scarce; however, social competence, an aspect of self-esteem which may predict psychosocial functioning, has been investigated in PBTS [15]. In a comprehensive review on social competence, it was concluded that PBTS experienced deficits in this area [16]. In a cross-sectional study, PBTS reported lower social competence than healthy peers and patients with a pediatric brain tumor during treatment, indicative of a decline of social competence of PBTS over time [17]. Furthermore, PBTS experienced more problems with self-confidence and self-esteem compared to leukemia survivors [18].
Fatigue is a common adverse effect of cancer treatment [19, 20]. In addition, due to the nature of their disease, PBTS frequently experience sleep problems and decreased sleep quality, leading to fatigue and negatively influencing daily functioning [21]. Fatigue in childhood cancer survivors and PBTS is associated with worse psychosocial functioning [22, 23].
The influence of executive deficits on psychosocial functioning has been acknowledged [24]. Executive functions, an umbrella term for mental skills concerning planning, behavioral control, and self-regulation, such as attention control, cognitive flexibility, and goal setting are critical skills to function properly in society [24]. Executive functions are often reported to be affected in PBTS [2527].
Since psychosocial functioning is important but understudied in PBTS as compared to other types of cancer [9, 28], we aimed to investigate various domains of psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from parent-reported neurocognitive complaints: HRQOL, self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, executive functioning, and fatigue. Based on the previous, we can conclude that it is especially important to study psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from neurocognitive problems, as the literature indicated that patients with neurocognitive problems are vulnerable to psychosocial problems. We take multiple informants (self-, parent-, and teacher report) into account, providing complementary information on how PBTS function, both at home and at school, investigating psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from parent-reported neurocognitive complaints. We hypothesize that PBTS experience decreased HRQOL, self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, executive functioning, and increased fatigue as reported by PBTS themselves, their parents, and/or teachers.

Methods

Procedures and participants

Data collection took place between January 2010 and August 2012, as part of the PRISMA study, a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind trial to investigate whether neurofeedback can improve neurocognitive functioning in PBTS [29]. Eligible for inclusion were children treated for a brain tumor in the Netherlands, aged 8–18 years, who finished treatment >2 years prior to enrollment and who suffered from neurocognitive complaints as reported by a parent on a screening questionnaire, assessing attention, speed, information processing and memory as well as exclusion criteria. Children with a premorbid diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, a mental or physical condition prohibiting neurocognitive assessment, or insufficient mastery of the Dutch language were excluded from the study.
PBTS (n = 249) who were treated in one of the participating Dutch hospitals (Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, VU University Medical Center Amsterdam, University Medical Center Utrecht, St. Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, and University Medical Center Maastricht) and their parents received a letter via their pediatric oncologist or psychologist informing them about the PRISMA study. Additionally, three patients from other hospitals made contact via email about participation, after they learned about the study.
Of the PBTS, 89 (35 %) did not meet inclusion criteria and 71 (29 %) declined participation (‘non-participants’) (see Fig. 1 for reasons). Parents of PBTS willing to participate (n = 92, 37 %) were provided with an online screening questionnaire concerning their child’s neurocognitive functioning, in order to verify eligibility. Ten PBTS (4 %) were excluded after online screening.
If the included PBTS had a sibling in the age range from 8 to 18 years, he or she was invited via telephone to participate in the control group for the fatigue outcome measure. Siblings were not considered to be optimal as a control group for the other psychosocial outcomes since the cancer diagnosis of the sibling could affect the scores on psychosocial functioning [30, 31].
Informed consent was obtained from the included 82 PBTS (33 %) and 43 siblings. Subsequently, PBTS, parents, siblings, and the case where parents and PBTS gave permission (n = 76), teachers of PBTS, were sent the questionnaires via email. For parent-report questionnaires, the primary caregiver was asked to fill out the questionnaire. For the teacher-report questionnaires, the parent was asked to indicate which teacher was most suitable to fill out questionnaires about the functioning of the child.
The study protocol was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam and was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00961922).

Demographic and medical characteristics

Parents of participating PBTS supplied information on gender and demographics (the parental country of origin and the highest level of parental education). Medical characteristics were taken from the medical records and included tumor histopathology and grade, type of treatment (surgery only vs. chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy with/without surgery), tumor location (supratentorial vs. infratentorial) and prior hydrocephalus, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis.
Medical and demographic data were also collected for a subsample of non-participants (45 out of 71 non-participants) to study selection bias at inclusion. As the non-participants declined participation, they were not assessed. To compare the age of participants and non-participants, ‘age at assessment’ for non-participants was calculated as the difference between the birth date and the average assessment date of participating PBTS.

Outcome measures

It is well-known that proxy report (parent/teacher) on the functioning of chronically ill children often yields discrepancies with self-report, although results of studies have been contradictory [32]. For this reason, we included a combination of self-report, parent-report, and teacher-report questionnaires.

Self-report

HRQOL
The Kidscreen-27 was administered to evaluate HRQOL in children by means of 27 items, scored on a 5-point Likert scale, divided over 5 dimensions: physical well-being, psychological well-being, autonomy and parents, peers and social support, and school environment [33]. In addition, a Generic score was calculated by summing the ten items that comprise the Kidscreen-10, a shorter version of the Kidscreen, derived from the Kidscreen-27 [34]. Raw scores for each dimension were transformed into T values with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in a European sample.
T values of a Dutch normative sample are available. Higher scores indicated better HRQOL. The Kidscreen-27 and Kidscreen-10 have good levels of validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha normative samples >0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS 0.71–0.88) [33, 34]. The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender distribution from the PBTS group (p > 0.10).
Self-esteem
We used the self-perception profile for children (SPPC, age 8–12) and adolescents (SPPA, age 12–18) to investigate self-esteem [3537]. The SPPC consists of 36 items, divided into six scales: scholastic competence, social acceptance, athletic competence, physical appearance, behavioral conduct, and global self-worth. The adolescent version (SPPA) contains 35 items and comprises one additional scale: close friendship. Each item was presented on a 4-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating stronger self-esteem. The SPPC and SPPA have acceptable to good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch manual >0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS 0.62–0.91) [36, 37]. The manual provided mean scores for males and females separately. For comparison with the total group of PBTS, scores of males and females in the normative population were combined after weighting by the gender distribution in the PBTS group. Age was not taken into account, as the SPPC and SPPA have separate norms based on age.
Psychosocial adjustment
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used to assess psychosocial adjustment [38, 39]. The items were scored on a 3-point Likert scale. A total problem score was calculated by adding the scores of 20 items, with a higher score indicating more problems. The SDQ total problem score has good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77) [39]. Dutch norms were available for children aged 11–16 years; therefore, analyses on the SDQ were restricted to PBTS aged 11–16 years. The gender distribution did not differ between the Dutch normative group and the PBTS, but mean age of the normative population was lower than of the PBTS. However, since total problem score is not affected by age, age was not taken into account in the analysis.
Fatigue
Fatigue was measured with the checklist individual strength (CIS) [40]. The CIS is a questionnaire that measures fatigue-related problems and consists of 20 items, scored on a 7-point Likert scale. The four subscales were subjective fatigue, concentration, motivation, and physical activity. A total score was calculated by summing up all items. Higher scores indicated more fatigue-related problems. The CIS has good reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha of the sibling controls and the PBTS 0.72–0.94. The data collected in the sibling control group were used for comparison with the PBTS. The sibling control group and the PBTS did not differ significantly (p > 0.10) in gender and age.

Parent report

Psychosocial adjustment
The SDQ was used to measure the parental perspective of PBTS’ psychosocial adjustment (see ‘Self-report’ for the description of the questionnaire). Reliability of the total problem score is good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77) [39]. PBTS were compared to a Dutch normative sample of children aged 8–16. The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender from the PBTS (p > 0.10).
Executive functioning
Parents rated their child’s behavioral executive functioning using the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) [41]. The BRIEF contains 75 items, scored on a 3-point Likert scale. The scores were summarized in eight scales (inhibit, shift, emotional control, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, organization of materials, and monitor), two indices (behavioral index and metacognition index), and a total score. The raw scores of the scales and indices were transformed into age- and gender-specific standardized T scores, as provided in the manual, with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Higher scores indicated more problems. Validity and reliability range from good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas reported in the manual between 0.78 and 0.96 and Cronbach’s alphas of the PBTS between 0.66 and 0.94 [42].

Teacher report

Psychosocial adjustment
The SDQ was used to measure the teacher perspective of psychosocial adjustment of the PBTS (see ‘Self-report’ for the description of the questionnaire). The reliability of the total problem score of the teacher report is reported to be good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.88; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77) [39]. A Dutch normative population of children aged 8–11 was available; therefore, the answers from teachers of PBTS within that age range were analyzed. The total problem score was analyzed separately for females and males, because the PBTS sample had more females than the normative population. The mean age of the Dutch normative population did not differ from the mean age of the PBTS aged 8–11.
Executive functioning
The BRIEF teacher-report version measures executive functioning of PBTS in the school situation (see ‘Parent-report’ for the description of the BRIEF). Validity and reliability are good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.88 to 0.98 as reported in the manual and between 0.82 and 0.97 of the PBTS [42].

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 20.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). To be able to detect possible confounders, one-sample t tests (age) and binomial tests (gender) were performed to test differences between PBTS and the normative population. Independent-samples t tests (age) and Chi-square tests (gender, country of birth, education) were used to test differences between PBTS and sibling controls. A p value of <0.10 was considered statistically significant for these analyses.
Differences between the participating PBTS and the subsample of 45 non-participating PBTS were tested with one-sample t tests (age at assessment, age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis), binomial tests (gender, tumor grade, tumor location, treatment, and hydrocephalus), or Chi-square test (tumor type).
One-sample t tests were used to evaluate differences between PBTS and the normative population regarding self-reported HRQOL, self-esteem, and regarding psychosocial adjustment, and proxy-reported psychosocial adjustment and executive functioning. Self-reported fatigue was analyzed with independent-samples t test (PBTS vs. sibling controls).
Effect sizes were calculated in terms of Cohen’s d, with 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80, reflecting small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively [43]. To adjust for multiple testing, Bonferroni correction was applied to the significance levels, as follows: HRQOL and self-esteem 8–11 years 0.05/6 = 0.008; self-esteem 12–18 years 0.05/7 = 0.007; fatigue 0.05/5 = 0.01; indices/total executive functioning 0.05/3 = 0.016; scales executive functioning 0.05/8 = 0.006. Differences with p values <0.05 in combination with effect size >0.35 were considered to be trends.

Results

Participants

The inclusion flowchart is depicted in Fig. 1. One enrolled PBTS, three enrolled siblings, and three teachers of enrolled PBTS did not complete the questionnaires. Self-report data were therefore available for 81 PBTS, 40 siblings, and teacher-report data for 73 PBTS.
Characteristics of the participating PBTS, the sibling control group and the non-participating PBTS are depicted in Table 1. Regarding the demographics, 20 of the participating PBTS received special education (24 %) and 39 have been held back a class (48 %). The participating and non-participating PBTS were comparable in age at assessment, gender, and education (p > 0.062). Participants and non-participants did not differ in tumor location, but they did differ with regard to the distribution of tumor grade, with more high-grade tumors in the participants than in the non-participants (p < 0.05). The participants were younger at diagnosis (p < 0.05) and had a longer interval past diagnosis than the non-participants (p < 0.05). More participating than non-participating PBTS underwent radiotherapy (p < 0.05) and chemotherapy (p = 0.001). The participating PBTS and the sibling control group were comparable in age, gender, parental country of origin, and the highest level of parental education (p > 0.324).
Table 1
Demographics and medical information of participating pediatric brain tumor survivors, sibling controls, and non-participating pediatric brain tumor survivors
 
PBTS participants
Controls
PBTS non-participants
n = 82
n = 43
n = 45
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Age
 Age at assessment
13.85
3.15
14.27
2.44
14.28
3.04
 Age diagnosis
6.87
3.77
8.23
3.95
 Time since diagnosis
6.98
3.57
6.05
3.31
 
n
%
n
%
n
%
Gender
 Boys
40
49
17
40
26
58
Country of origin mother
 Netherlands
71
87
37
86
n/a
n/a
 Other
11
13
9
14
n/a
n/a
Country of origin father
 Netherlands
73
89
40
93
n/a
n/a
 Other
9
11
3
7
n/a
n/a
Highest education parenta
 Low or Intermediate
39
48
23
54
n/a
n/a
 High
43
52
20
46
n/a
n/a
Education
 Regular education
62
80
n/a
n/a
28
62
 Special education
20
20
n/a
n/a
11
24
 Unknown
0
0
n/a
n/a
6
13
Tumor type and grade
 High grade
34
42
13
29
  Medulloblastoma
12
15
6
13
  Supratentorial PNET
8
10
2
4
  Ependymoma
5
6
2
4
  Astrocytoma gr III
5
6
1
2
  Germ cell tumor
4
5
2
4
 Low grade
48
59
32
71
  Low grade glioma
35
43
26
58
  Craniopharyngioma
7
9
5
11
  Plexus papilloma
6
7
1
2
Treatment
 Radiotherapy
34
42
14
31
 Chemotherapy
35
43
12
27
 Surgeryb
72
88
41
91
 Other
2
2
1
2
  Biopsy only
1
1
1
2
  CSF pressure relief only
1
1
0
0
Location
 Supratentorial
46
56
22
49
 Infratentorial
36
44
23
51
Hydrocephalus
 Yes
39
48
n/a
n/a
 No
43
52
n/a
n/a
The information was available for 45 of 71 non-participanting PBTS. The siblings did not differ significantly from the participating PBTS on any of the variables. The non-participanting PBTS differed from the participants on age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, tumor type, tumor grade, radiotherapy and chemotherapy
PBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, M mean, SD standard deviation, n/a not available
p < .05; ** p < .001
aHighest education of father or mother is reported: Low or Intermediate = Primary education, general secondary education and secondary vocational education; High = Higher vocational education and university
b37 patients were treated with surgery only

Outcomes

In Tables 2 and 3, the results of the self-report and proxy-report questionnaires’ analyses are displayed, respectively. Figure 2 is a graphical summary of the results (effect sizes) from Tables 2 and 3, showing the profile of psychosocial functioning in PBTS. For self-esteem, average effect sizes are depicted, weighted by the number of PBTS who completed the SPPC and SPPA.
Table 2
Psychosocial functioning of the pediatric brain tumor survivors compared to the controls; self-report
Measure
n
PBTS
Controls
Group differences
M
SD
M
SD
d
p
HRQOL—KIDSCREEN-2 [35]/Kidscreen-10 [36]
 Physical well-being
81
46.69
9.69
52.88
10.02
0.62
<0.001
 Psychological well-being
81
49.09
9.09
52.79
9.46
0.39
<0.001
 Autonomy and parents
81
51.99
8.21
53.95
9.51
0.21
0.035
 Peers and social support
81
49.11
10.94
52.36
9.04
0.36
0.009
 School environment
78
51.08
8.70
53.06
9.71
0.20
0.049
 Generic (Kidscreen-10)
78
49.55
8.32
54.10
10.40
0.44
<0.001
Self-esteem—SPPC (8–12) [37]
 Scholastic competence
24
14.79
3.79
16.60
3.46
0.52
0.028
 Social acceptance
24
18.33
3.91
17.55
3.68
0.21
0.334
 Athletic competence
24
16.08
4.24
17.88
3.24
0.56
0.049
 Physical appearance
24
20.67
3.50
19.24
3.95
0.36
0.058
 Behavioral conduct
24
20.29
3.58
17.72
2.85
0.91
0.002
 Global self-worth
24
20.63
3.32
19.55
3.06
0.35
0.126
Self-esteem—SPPA (12–18) [38]
 Scholastic competence
57
13.23
2.93
13.88
2.51
0.26
0.097
 Social acceptance
57
15.14
2.97
15.34
2.73
0.07
0.607
 Athletic competence
57
12.37
4.26
13.74
3.35
0.41
0.018
 Physical appearance
57
13.67
3.50
13.82
3.27
0.05
0.742
 Behavioral conduct
57
15.49
3.67
14.24
2.88
0.44
0.013
 Global self-worth
57
16.47
3.52
17.08
2.80
0.22
0.196
 Close friendship
57
15.28
3.71
15.52
2.84
0.08
0.634
Psychosocial adjustment—SDQ (11–16) [39]
 Total problem score
48
10.02
5.09
9.90
4.90
0.02
0.870
Fatigue—CIS [41]a
 Subjective fatigue
76
23.57
11.16
20.53
10.75
0.25
0.168
 Concentration
76
19.09
7.78
14.45
7.19
0.57
0.003
 Motivation
76
11.29
4.91
9.82
4.87
0.27
0.133
 Physical activity
76
9.27
5.00
6.97
4.16
0.45
0.011
 Total score
76
63.23
21.80
51.76
21.88
0.47
0.010
Significant differences after Bonferroni correction are presented in bold. Effect sizes ‘d’ were calculated by dividing the difference in mean score between the PBTS and the normative population or sibling controls by the pooled standard deviation. Lower scores reflect worse HRQOL and Self-Esteem. Higher scors reflect more problems on Psychosocial adjustment and fatigue
PBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, HRQOL health related quality of life, M mean, SD standard deviation
aSibling controls. n = 40
Table 3
Psychosocial functioning of the pediatric brain tumor survivors compared to the controls; proxy report
 
n
PBTS
Controls
Group differences
M
SD
M
SD
d
p
Parent report
Psychosocial adjustment—SDQ (8–16) [39]
 Total problem score
67
11.01
5.16
6.70
5.30
0.81
<0.001
Behavioral executive functioning—BRIEF [43]
 Behavioral index
82
53.48
11.22
50.00
10.00
0.35
0.002
 Metacognition index
82
54.09
8.11
50.00
10.00
0.41
<0.001
 Total score
82
54.29
8.45
50.00
10.00
0.43
<0.001
Teacher report
Psychosocial adjustment—SDQ (8–12) [39]
 Total problem score males
9
10.33
9.44
9.10
6.60
0.19
0.582
 Total problem score females
21
9.71
5.60
5.80
5.70
0.69
0.004
Behavioral executive functioning—BRIEF [43]
 Behavioral index
73
50.89
12.41
50.00
10.00
0.09
0.511
 Metacognition index
73
51.89
13.90
50.00
10.00
0.19
0.249
 Total score
73
51.29
12.72
50.00
10.00
0.13
0.395
Significant differences after Bonferroni correction are presented in bold. Effect sizes ‘d’ were calculated by dividing the difference in mean score between the PBTS and the normative population by the pooled standard deviation. Higher scores reflect worse psychosocial adjustment and behavioral functioning
PBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, M mean, SD standard deviation

Self-report

HRQOL
PBTS showed significantly worse HRQOL than the normative sample (p < 0.008) on 2 subscales of the Kidscreen-27: physical well-being and psychological well-being, and on the generic scale (medium-to-large effect sizes). A tendency toward lower HRQOL in PBTS than the norm was found for peers and social support (p < 0.05; medium effect size).
Self-esteem
PBTS aged 8–11 years obtained significantly higher behavioral conduct scores compared to the normative population (p < 0.008, large effect size), indicating higher self-esteem regarding their behavior; PBTS aged 12–18 tended toward higher self-esteem on this scale (p < 0.05, medium effect size). A trend toward lower self-esteem in PBTS aged 8–11 was found for scholastic competence and for athletic competence, and also for athletic competence of PBTS aged 12–18 (p < 0.05, medium effect size). No differences between the normative population and the PBTS were observed on the other scales.
Psychosocial adjustment
PBTS between 11 and 16 years of age did not experience more psychosocial adjustment problems than the normative population as shown by their total problem score of the SDQ.
Fatigue
PBTS reported more concentration problems than the sibling control group (p < 0.01, medium effect size). A trend toward decreased physical activity in PBTS compared to the sibling control group was found as well as a trend toward a higher total scale compared to the siblings (p < 0.05, medium effect sizes), indicating more fatigue-related problems. The PBTS did not differ from the siblings on subjective fatigue and motivation problems.

Parent report

Psychosocial adjustment
The parent-reported total problem score of psychosocial adjustment (SDQ) was higher in the PBTS than in the norm (p < 0.001, large effect size), indicating more problems in psychosocial adjustment.
Executive functioning
Parents of PBTS considered their children’s behavioral expressions of executive functioning to be significantly worse than parents in the normative population. More specifically, PBTS had lower scores regarding the two indices and the total score (p < 0.016; medium effect sizes). Subsequent analyses showed worse functioning on the scales’ emotional control, and on initiate and working memory (p < 0.006; ds 0.47, 0.59, and 0.71, respectively). No significant differences were found on the other subscales.

Teacher report

Psychosocial adjustment
For the female PBTS aged 8–11 years (n = 9), teachers reported significantly higher total problem scores (SDQ) than the norm (p < 0.01, large effect size). No difference was found between the male PBTS (n = 21) and the males in the normative population.
Executive functioning
According to the teacher report, no differences were found between the PBTS and the normative population on the indices and the total score of the BRIEF.

Discussion

This study provides the first multidimensional (self-, parent- and teacher report) view of psychosocial functioning of PBTS with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints. The multidimensional approach is an advantage of the study because of the symptom burden of patients and complexity of their social situation. PBTS showed decreased psychosocial functioning on a number of the tested domains: self-reported HRQOL and fatigue, parent-reported psychosocial adjustment and executive functioning, and teacher-reported psychosocial adjustment for females only. These results are in line with a study by Meeske et al. [20], who reported PBTS to exhibit problems in physical, social, psychosocial, school, cognitive domains, and fatigue. The decreased HRQOL scores of PBTS on psychological well-being may be caused by the neurocognitive complaints from which they suffer. This is supported by the trend we found toward lower self-esteem regarding scholastic competence the PBTS show and by the literature [13]. This should be further studied in future studies. However, despite the neurocognitive complaints, PBTS functioned within normal ranges in several psychosocial domains or showed only trends toward worse functioning: self-reported self-esteem and psychosocial adjustment, and teacher-reported executive functioning and psychosocial adjustment for males.
Physical functioning was specifically compromised in PBTS. Besides worse physical HRQOL, a tendency toward decreased self-reported athletic competence (domain of self-esteem) and decreased physical activity (domain of the fatigue questionnaire) was observed. It is known that PBTS are at increased risk of functional impairments, which is related to physical self-esteem [44]. It is important that professionals working with PBTS are aware of these possible late effects and monitor physical well-being in relation to self-esteem and HRQOL.
Regarding self-esteem, no problems other than the trends toward physical-related and scholastic-related problems were seen. PBTS behavioral conduct scores were even better than the norm. This positive finding has been observed previously, e.g., after a trauma [45], but has recently also received attention in pediatric oncology literature [46]. It has been attributed, among other factors, to the resilience of the PBTS and posttraumatic growth.
The PBTS in our sample did not report more psychosocial adjustment problems as assessed with the SDQ than their peers, in contrast with parents and teachers who did report psychosocial adjustment problems in PBTS. This finding is not surprising, as in the literature it has been found that both healthy children and childhood cancer survivors typically report different levels of psychosocial problems than their parents and/or teachers [47]. Some studies found child-reported levels of problems to be higher than parent- and/or teacher-reported levels [48], while other studies found the opposite [49]. Sato et al. [50] concluded that parent and child ratings are influenced by different factors. Among others, parents’ perception was influenced by their level of distress, whereas the child’s perception tended to be dependent on trait anxiety. Others found that childhood cancer survivors may report less psychosocial problems influenced by social desirability, stress-related growth, or a positive coping strategy [51, 52]. In this study, the diagnosis and treatment resulting neurocognitive consequences might have led to increased parental distress, causing parents to report more problems than their children. Another possible reason for the discrepancy between the observed scores of the different informants in our study is the age difference in self-reports versus parent and teacher reports concerning psychosocial adjustment: Due to age-restricted normative data, the results on the self-reported SDQ were based on PBTS aged 11–16 (n = 48), whereas parent- and teacher-reported results were based on PBTS aged 8–16 (n = 67) and 8–12 (n = 30), respectively.
Teachers reported no executive problems in PBTS, whereas parents did, especially regarding emotional control, initiation, and working memory. This discrepancy could be the result of the ‘observation environment.’ Teachers observed the PBTS in a school environment, which is more structured than the home situation. Possibly, the problems parents saw at home did not exist in the same way in structured settings like school. Turner and colleagues describe problems of PBTS to increase as they leave the structured school environment [53]. This implies that PBTS may benefit from a structured environment. Another reason for the difference between the parent and teacher perspective could be that they have a different reference background. Parents know the child’s premorbid functioning, whereas teachers have the behavior of classmates as a reference. A large proportion of the children in our sample were in special education (24 %), where many classmates suffered from chronic conditions, which could also affect psychosocial functioning [54].
This study has some limitations to take into account. The results are not generalizable to the PBTS population as a whole, since PBTS in this study were selected on the basis of parent-reported neurocognitive problems and the willingness to participate in a study of a treatment aimed to improve neurocognitive functioning. This may have led to an overestimation of the psychosocial problems. It is easy to consider this a non-representative sample, but we have to take into account that many children with a brain tumor suffer from neurocognitive problems (40–100 %). Therefore, this study sheds light on a vulnerable group of PBTS. Awareness for their psychosocial functioning from a complementary perspective is of utmost importance. Another limitation of the study is that normative data were not available for all questionnaires within all age groups. So for some outcomes, especially the SDQ, comparison with the normative population was possible for only small subgroups of PBTS. This limits the reliability and generalizability of the results. For this reason, we would like to urge future studies to aim at collecting norm data for broader age ranges. Nevertheless, this study adds to the existing knowledge as it provides a broad, multidimensional profile of functioning of PBTS with neurocognitive complaints, based on multiinformants.
Better insight into psychosocial functioning in the growing group of PBTS with neurocognitive complaints will help professionals to identify those patients susceptible to developing psychosocial problems. Timely identification is important to prevent problems from escalating. Screening for possible psychosocial late effects should be done in a systematic way, preferably by using the perspective of the patient, parent, and teacher. In daily clinical practice, patient- and/or parent-reported outcomes (PROs) are recommended, because this will increase awareness of and attention for psychosocial functioning during routine checkups. Increased awareness can improve provision of aftercare [55]. Furthermore, providing tailored support to this group of vulnerable children is necessary. Interventions for PBTS with (imminent) psychosocial problems should be aimed at improving HRQOL, coping with fatigue, and providing structure in daily life.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Dutch Cancer Society (KWF Kankerbestrijding, Grant No. UVA 2008-4013). The authors would like to thank Juliette Greidanus for her contributions to the data collection of the study. Furthermore, we would like to acknowledge all participating parents, patients, and their siblings for their cooperation.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study and their parents.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Podotherapeut Totaal

Binnen de bundel kunt u gebruik maken van boeken, tijdschriften, e-learnings, web-tv's en uitlegvideo's. BSL Podotherapeut Totaal is overal toegankelijk; via uw PC, tablet of smartphone.

Literatuur
2.
go back to reference Tonning Olsson, I., Perrin, S., Lundgren, J., et al. (2014). Long-term cognitive sequelae after pediatric brain tumor related to medical risk factors, age, and sex. Pediatric Neurology, 51, 515–521.CrossRefPubMed Tonning Olsson, I., Perrin, S., Lundgren, J., et al. (2014). Long-term cognitive sequelae after pediatric brain tumor related to medical risk factors, age, and sex. Pediatric Neurology, 51, 515–521.CrossRefPubMed
3.
go back to reference Glauser, T. A., & Packer, R. J. (1991). Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood brain tumors. Child’s Nervous System, 7, 7–12.CrossRef Glauser, T. A., & Packer, R. J. (1991). Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood brain tumors. Child’s Nervous System, 7, 7–12.CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Stargatt, R., Rosenfeld, J. V., Anderson, V., et al. (2006). Intelligence and adaptive function in children diagnosed with brain tumour during infancy. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 80, 295–303.CrossRefPubMed Stargatt, R., Rosenfeld, J. V., Anderson, V., et al. (2006). Intelligence and adaptive function in children diagnosed with brain tumour during infancy. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 80, 295–303.CrossRefPubMed
5.
go back to reference Saury, J. M. G., & Emanuelson, I. (2011). Cognitive consequences of the treatment of medulloblastoma among children. Pediatric Neurology, 44, 21–30.CrossRefPubMed Saury, J. M. G., & Emanuelson, I. (2011). Cognitive consequences of the treatment of medulloblastoma among children. Pediatric Neurology, 44, 21–30.CrossRefPubMed
6.
go back to reference De Ruiter, M. A., van Mourik, R., Schouten-van Meeteren, A. Y. N., et al. (2013). Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: A meta-analysis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55, 408–417.CrossRefPubMed De Ruiter, M. A., van Mourik, R., Schouten-van Meeteren, A. Y. N., et al. (2013). Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: A meta-analysis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55, 408–417.CrossRefPubMed
7.
go back to reference Ellenberg, L., Liu, Q., Gioia, G. A., et al. (2009). Neurocognitive status in long-term survivors of childhood CNS malignancies: A report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Neuropsychology, 23, 705–717.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Ellenberg, L., Liu, Q., Gioia, G. A., et al. (2009). Neurocognitive status in long-term survivors of childhood CNS malignancies: A report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Neuropsychology, 23, 705–717.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
8.
go back to reference Wakefield, C. E., McLoone, J., Goodenough, B., et al. (2010). The psychosocial impact of completing childhood cancer treatment: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35, 262–274.CrossRefPubMed Wakefield, C. E., McLoone, J., Goodenough, B., et al. (2010). The psychosocial impact of completing childhood cancer treatment: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35, 262–274.CrossRefPubMed
9.
go back to reference Patenaude, A. F., & Kupst, M. J. (2005). Psychosocial functioning in pediatric cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 9–27.CrossRefPubMed Patenaude, A. F., & Kupst, M. J. (2005). Psychosocial functioning in pediatric cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 9–27.CrossRefPubMed
11.
go back to reference Gudrunardottir, T., Lannering, B., Remke, M., et al. (2014). Treatment developments and the unfolding of the quality of life discussion in childhood medulloblastoma: A review. Child’s Nervous System, 30, 979–990.CrossRefPubMed Gudrunardottir, T., Lannering, B., Remke, M., et al. (2014). Treatment developments and the unfolding of the quality of life discussion in childhood medulloblastoma: A review. Child’s Nervous System, 30, 979–990.CrossRefPubMed
12.
go back to reference Zebrack, B. J., Gurney, J. G., Oeffinger, K., et al. (2004). Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: A report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 22, 999–1006.CrossRefPubMed Zebrack, B. J., Gurney, J. G., Oeffinger, K., et al. (2004). Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: A report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 22, 999–1006.CrossRefPubMed
13.
go back to reference Aukema, E. J., Schouten-van Meeteren, A. Y. N., Last, B. F., et al. (2013). Childhood brain tumor survivors at risk for impaired health-related quality of life. Journal of Pediatric Hematology/Oncology, 35, 603–609.CrossRefPubMed Aukema, E. J., Schouten-van Meeteren, A. Y. N., Last, B. F., et al. (2013). Childhood brain tumor survivors at risk for impaired health-related quality of life. Journal of Pediatric Hematology/Oncology, 35, 603–609.CrossRefPubMed
14.
go back to reference Penn, A., Shortman, R. I., Lowis, S. P., et al. (2010). Child-related determinants of health-related quality of life in children with brain tumours 1 year after diagnosis. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 55, 1377–1385.CrossRef Penn, A., Shortman, R. I., Lowis, S. P., et al. (2010). Child-related determinants of health-related quality of life in children with brain tumours 1 year after diagnosis. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 55, 1377–1385.CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Harter, S. (1985). Self-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver. Harter, S. (1985). Self-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver.
16.
go back to reference Schulte, F., & Barrera, M. (2010). Social competence in childhood brain tumor survivors: A comprehensive review. Supportive Care in Cancer, 18, 1499–1513.CrossRefPubMed Schulte, F., & Barrera, M. (2010). Social competence in childhood brain tumor survivors: A comprehensive review. Supportive Care in Cancer, 18, 1499–1513.CrossRefPubMed
17.
go back to reference Hardy, K. K., Willard, V. W., Watral, M. A., et al. (2010). Perceived social competency in children with brain tumors: Comparison between children on and off therapy. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 27, 156–163.CrossRefPubMed Hardy, K. K., Willard, V. W., Watral, M. A., et al. (2010). Perceived social competency in children with brain tumors: Comparison between children on and off therapy. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 27, 156–163.CrossRefPubMed
18.
go back to reference Chou, L.-N., & Hunter, A. (2009). Factors affecting quality of life in Taiwanese survivors of childhood cancer. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 65, 2131–2141.CrossRefPubMed Chou, L.-N., & Hunter, A. (2009). Factors affecting quality of life in Taiwanese survivors of childhood cancer. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 65, 2131–2141.CrossRefPubMed
19.
go back to reference Campos, M., Hassan, B., Riechelmann, R., et al. (2011). Cancer-related fatigue: A review. Revista Da Associacao Medica Brasileira, 57, 206–214.CrossRef Campos, M., Hassan, B., Riechelmann, R., et al. (2011). Cancer-related fatigue: A review. Revista Da Associacao Medica Brasileira, 57, 206–214.CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Meeske, K., Katz, E. R., Palmer, S. N., et al. (2004). Parent proxy-reported health-related quality of life and fatigue in pediatric patients diagnosed with brain tumors and acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Cancer, 101, 2116–2125.CrossRefPubMed Meeske, K., Katz, E. R., Palmer, S. N., et al. (2004). Parent proxy-reported health-related quality of life and fatigue in pediatric patients diagnosed with brain tumors and acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Cancer, 101, 2116–2125.CrossRefPubMed
21.
go back to reference Verberne, L. M., Maurice-Stam, H., Grootenhuis, M. A., et al. (2012). Sleep disorders in children after treatment for a CNS tumour. Journal of Sleep Research, 21, 461–469.CrossRefPubMed Verberne, L. M., Maurice-Stam, H., Grootenhuis, M. A., et al. (2012). Sleep disorders in children after treatment for a CNS tumour. Journal of Sleep Research, 21, 461–469.CrossRefPubMed
22.
go back to reference Meeske, K. A., Patel, S. K., Palmer, S. N., et al. (2007). Factors associated with health-related quality of life in pediatric cancer survivors. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 49, 298–305.CrossRef Meeske, K. A., Patel, S. K., Palmer, S. N., et al. (2007). Factors associated with health-related quality of life in pediatric cancer survivors. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 49, 298–305.CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Daniel, L. C., Brumley, L. D., & Schwartz, L. A. (2013). Fatigue in adolescents with cancer compared to healthy adolescents. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 60, 1902–1907.CrossRef Daniel, L. C., Brumley, L. D., & Schwartz, L. A. (2013). Fatigue in adolescents with cancer compared to healthy adolescents. Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 60, 1902–1907.CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 39–64.CrossRefPubMed Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 39–64.CrossRefPubMed
25.
go back to reference Aarsen, F. K., Paquier, P. F., Arts, W.-F. M., et al. (2009). Cognitive deficits and predictors 3 years after diagnosis of a pilocytic astrocytoma in childhood. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 27, 3526–3532.CrossRefPubMed Aarsen, F. K., Paquier, P. F., Arts, W.-F. M., et al. (2009). Cognitive deficits and predictors 3 years after diagnosis of a pilocytic astrocytoma in childhood. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 27, 3526–3532.CrossRefPubMed
26.
go back to reference Maddrey, A. M., Bergeron, J. A., Lombardo, E. R., et al. (2005). Neuropsychological performance and quality of life of 10 year survivors of childhood medulloblastoma. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 72, 245–253.CrossRefPubMed Maddrey, A. M., Bergeron, J. A., Lombardo, E. R., et al. (2005). Neuropsychological performance and quality of life of 10 year survivors of childhood medulloblastoma. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 72, 245–253.CrossRefPubMed
27.
go back to reference Wolfe, K. R., Madan-Swain, A., & Kana, R. K. (2012). Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: A systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol, 37, 153–175.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Wolfe, K. R., Madan-Swain, A., & Kana, R. K. (2012). Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: A systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol, 37, 153–175.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
28.
go back to reference Bhat, S. R., Goodwin, T. L., Burwinkle, T. M., et al. (2005). Profile of daily life in children with brain tumors: An assessment of health-related quality of life. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 23, 5493–5500.CrossRefPubMed Bhat, S. R., Goodwin, T. L., Burwinkle, T. M., et al. (2005). Profile of daily life in children with brain tumors: An assessment of health-related quality of life. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 23, 5493–5500.CrossRefPubMed
29.
go back to reference De Ruiter, M. A., Schouten-Van Meeteren, A. Y. N., Van Mourik, R., et al. (2012). Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: Design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. BMC Cancer, 12, 581.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed De Ruiter, M. A., Schouten-Van Meeteren, A. Y. N., Van Mourik, R., et al. (2012). Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: Design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. BMC Cancer, 12, 581.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
30.
go back to reference Houtzager, B. A., Grootenhuis, M. A., Caron, H. N., et al. (2004). Quality of life and psychological adaptation in siblings of paediatric cancer patients, 2 years after diagnosis. Psychooncology, 13, 499–511.CrossRefPubMed Houtzager, B. A., Grootenhuis, M. A., Caron, H. N., et al. (2004). Quality of life and psychological adaptation in siblings of paediatric cancer patients, 2 years after diagnosis. Psychooncology, 13, 499–511.CrossRefPubMed
31.
go back to reference Alderfer, M. A., Long, K. A., Lown, E. A., et al. (2010). Psychosocial adjustment of siblings of children with cancer: A systematic review. Psychooncology, 19, 789–805.CrossRefPubMed Alderfer, M. A., Long, K. A., Lown, E. A., et al. (2010). Psychosocial adjustment of siblings of children with cancer: A systematic review. Psychooncology, 19, 789–805.CrossRefPubMed
32.
go back to reference Eiser, C., & Varni, J. W. (2013). Health-related quality of life and symptom reporting: Similarities and differences between children and their parents. European Journal of Pediatrics, 172, 1299–1304.CrossRefPubMed Eiser, C., & Varni, J. W. (2013). Health-related quality of life and symptom reporting: Similarities and differences between children and their parents. European Journal of Pediatrics, 172, 1299–1304.CrossRefPubMed
33.
go back to reference Ravens-Sieberer, U., Auquier, P., Erhart, M., et al. (2007). The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: Psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Quality of Life Research, 16, 1347–1356.CrossRefPubMed Ravens-Sieberer, U., Auquier, P., Erhart, M., et al. (2007). The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: Psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Quality of Life Research, 16, 1347–1356.CrossRefPubMed
34.
go back to reference Ravens-Sieberer, U., Erhart, M., Rajmil, L., et al. (2010). Reliability, construct and criterion validity of the KIDSCREEN-10 score: A short measure for children and adolescents’ well-being and health-related quality of life. Quality of Life Research, 19, 1487–1500.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Ravens-Sieberer, U., Erhart, M., Rajmil, L., et al. (2010). Reliability, construct and criterion validity of the KIDSCREEN-10 score: A short measure for children and adolescents’ well-being and health-related quality of life. Quality of Life Research, 19, 1487–1500.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
35.
go back to reference Harter, S. (1985). Social-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver. Harter, S. (1985). Social-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver.
36.
go back to reference Veerman, J. W., Straathof, M. A. E., Treffers, P. D. A., van den Bergh, B. R. H., & ten BL. (1997). Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children—Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger Veerman, J. W., Straathof, M. A. E., Treffers, P. D. A., van den Bergh, B. R. H., & ten BL. (1997). Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children—Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger
37.
go back to reference Treffers, P., Goedhart, A., Veerman, J., et al. (2002). Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger. Treffers, P., Goedhart, A., Veerman, J., et al. (2002). Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
38.
go back to reference Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.CrossRefPubMed Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.CrossRefPubMed
39.
go back to reference Van Widenfelt, B. M., Goedhart, A. W., Treffers, P. D. A., et al. (2003). Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 281–289.CrossRefPubMed Van Widenfelt, B. M., Goedhart, A. W., Treffers, P. D. A., et al. (2003). Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 281–289.CrossRefPubMed
40.
go back to reference Stulemeijer, M., de Jong, L. W. A. M., Fiselier, T. J. W., et al. (2005). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: Randomised controlled trial. BMJ, 330, 14.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Stulemeijer, M., de Jong, L. W. A. M., Fiselier, T. J. W., et al. (2005). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: Randomised controlled trial. BMJ, 330, 14.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
41.
go back to reference Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., et al. (2000). TEST REVIEW behavior rating inventory of executive function behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235–238.CrossRefPubMed Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., et al. (2000). TEST REVIEW behavior rating inventory of executive function behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 235–238.CrossRefPubMed
42.
go back to reference Smidts, D. P., & Huizinga, M. (2009). BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers. Smidts, D. P., & Huizinga, M. (2009). BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers.
43.
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1962). The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: A review. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 145–153.CrossRefPubMed Cohen, J. (1962). The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: A review. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 145–153.CrossRefPubMed
44.
go back to reference Boman, K. K., Hörnquist, L., De Graaff, L., et al. (2013). Disability, body image and sports/physical activity in adult survivors of childhood CNS tumors: Population-based outcomes from a cohort study. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 112, 99–106.CrossRefPubMed Boman, K. K., Hörnquist, L., De Graaff, L., et al. (2013). Disability, body image and sports/physical activity in adult survivors of childhood CNS tumors: Population-based outcomes from a cohort study. Journal of Neuro-oncology, 112, 99–106.CrossRefPubMed
45.
go back to reference Blix, I., & Birkeland, M. S. (2015). posttraumatic growth and centrality of event: A longitudinal study in the aftermath of the 2011 Oslo Bombing. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7, 18–23.CrossRef Blix, I., & Birkeland, M. S. (2015). posttraumatic growth and centrality of event: A longitudinal study in the aftermath of the 2011 Oslo Bombing. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7, 18–23.CrossRef
46.
go back to reference Zebrack, B. J., Stuber, M. L., Meeske, K. A., et al. (2012). Perceived positive impact of cancer among long-term survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Psychooncology, 21, 630–639.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Zebrack, B. J., Stuber, M. L., Meeske, K. A., et al. (2012). Perceived positive impact of cancer among long-term survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Psychooncology, 21, 630–639.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
47.
go back to reference Sattoe, J. N. T., van Staa, A., & Moll, H. A. (2012). The proxy problem anatomized: Child–parent disagreement in health related quality of life reports of chronically ill adolescents. Health Qual Life Outcomes, 10, 10.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Sattoe, J. N. T., van Staa, A., & Moll, H. A. (2012). The proxy problem anatomized: Child–parent disagreement in health related quality of life reports of chronically ill adolescents. Health Qual Life Outcomes, 10, 10.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
48.
go back to reference Theunissen, N. C., Vogels, T. G., Koopman, H. M., et al. (1998). The proxy problem: Child report versus parent report in health-related quality of life research. Quality of Life Research, 7, 387–397.CrossRefPubMed Theunissen, N. C., Vogels, T. G., Koopman, H. M., et al. (1998). The proxy problem: Child report versus parent report in health-related quality of life research. Quality of Life Research, 7, 387–397.CrossRefPubMed
49.
go back to reference Chang, P.-C., & Yeh, C.-H. (2005). Agreement between child self-report and parent proxy-report to evaluate quality of life in children with cancer. Psychooncology, 14, 125–134.CrossRefPubMed Chang, P.-C., & Yeh, C.-H. (2005). Agreement between child self-report and parent proxy-report to evaluate quality of life in children with cancer. Psychooncology, 14, 125–134.CrossRefPubMed
50.
go back to reference Sato, I., Higuchi, A., Yanagisawa, T., et al. (2013). Factors influencing self- and parent-reporting health-related quality of life in children with brain tumors. Quality of Life Research, 22, 185–201.CrossRefPubMed Sato, I., Higuchi, A., Yanagisawa, T., et al. (2013). Factors influencing self- and parent-reporting health-related quality of life in children with brain tumors. Quality of Life Research, 22, 185–201.CrossRefPubMed
51.
go back to reference O’Leary, T. E., Diller, L., & Recklitis, C. J. (2007). The effects of response bias on self-reported quality of life among childhood cancer survivors. Quality of Life Research, 16, 1211–1220.CrossRefPubMed O’Leary, T. E., Diller, L., & Recklitis, C. J. (2007). The effects of response bias on self-reported quality of life among childhood cancer survivors. Quality of Life Research, 16, 1211–1220.CrossRefPubMed
52.
go back to reference Liptak, C., Manley, P., & Recklitis, C. J. (2012). The feasibility of psychosocial screening for adolescent and young adult brain tumor survivors: The value of self-report. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 6, 379–387.CrossRefPubMed Liptak, C., Manley, P., & Recklitis, C. J. (2012). The feasibility of psychosocial screening for adolescent and young adult brain tumor survivors: The value of self-report. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 6, 379–387.CrossRefPubMed
53.
go back to reference Turner, C. D., Rey-Casserly, C., Liptak, C. C., et al. (2009). Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor survivors. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 24, 1455–1463. Turner, C. D., Rey-Casserly, C., Liptak, C. C., et al. (2009). Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor survivors. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 24, 1455–1463.
54.
go back to reference Van Cleave, J., & Davis, M. M. (2006). Bullying and peer victimization among children with special health care needs. Pediatrics, 118, e1212–e1219.CrossRefPubMed Van Cleave, J., & Davis, M. M. (2006). Bullying and peer victimization among children with special health care needs. Pediatrics, 118, e1212–e1219.CrossRefPubMed
55.
go back to reference Engelen, V., Detmar, S., Koopman, H., et al. (2012). Reporting health-related quality of life scores to physicians during routine follow-up visits of pediatric oncology patients: Is it effective? Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 58, 766–774.CrossRef Engelen, V., Detmar, S., Koopman, H., et al. (2012). Reporting health-related quality of life scores to physicians during routine follow-up visits of pediatric oncology patients: Is it effective? Pediatric Blood & Cancer, 58, 766–774.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints
Auteurs
Marieke Anna de Ruiter
Antoinette Yvonne Narda Schouten-van Meeteren
Dannis Gilbert van Vuurden
Heleen Maurice-Stam
Corrie Gidding
Laura Rachel Beek
Bernd Granzen
Jaap Oosterlaan
Martha Alexandra Grootenhuis
Publicatiedatum
20-08-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer International Publishing
Gepubliceerd in
Quality of Life Research / Uitgave 2/2016
Print ISSN: 0962-9343
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2649
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-015-1091-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2016

Quality of Life Research 2/2016 Naar de uitgave