Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 03-08-2024

Psychometric Properties of the Reflective Function Questionnaire for Youth Five-Item Version in Adolescents with Restrictive Eating Disorders

Auteurs: Tom Jewell, Veronica McLaren, Carla Sharp

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment | Uitgave 3/2024

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Mentalizing difficulties are an established correlate of eating disorders, but there is a need to establish the validity and reliability of easy-to-administer instruments to measure the construct in adolescents. This study evaluated the psychometric properties of a five-item version of the Reflective Function Questionnaire for Youth (RFQY-5) in a sample of adolescents with restrictive eating disorders recruited for a prospective observational study. Adolescents completed the 46-item version of the RFQ-Y at the start of treatment (T1) (n = 171) and nine months later (n = 51). The factor structure, internal reliability and convergent validity of the RFQY-5 were assessed at T1. Sensitivity to change was assessed in a subsample of n = 51 who completed the RFQ-Y nine months after the start of treatment (T2). At T1, the RFQY-5 demonstrated inadequate internal reliability (McDonald’s Omega = 0.657) and poor model fit for the unidimensional structure (Χ2(5) = 29.98; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.801; TLI = 0.602; RMSEA = 0.171; SRMR = 0.070). Convergent validity was partially supported. The RFQY-5 was not sensitive to change. The RFQY-5 was found to have inadequate psychometric properties in adolescents with restrictive eating disorders. Psychometric studies are needed to further develop valid and reliable measures of mentalizing for this population.
Extras
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10862-024-10158-9.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Mentalizing refers to the capacity to understand the self and others in terms of intentional mental states such as feelings, desires, attitudes and goals (Fonagy and Target, 2006). Mentalizing difficulties are an established correlate of eating disorders across the age range and may represent a target for prevention and treatment (Jewell et al., 2016; Simonsen et al., 2020). However, the precise mechanisms by which mentalizing and eating disorders are related is not yet clear, and this task is made harder by several key issues. Firstly, mentalizing is a complex, multidimensional concept (Luyten et al., 2020), and eating disorders themselves are heterogeneous, encompassing a broad spectrum of symptomology across different disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Moreover, the ability of the field to make progress is constrained by the availability of adequate measurement tools, and the validity of mentalizing measures in eating disorder populations is low (Bizzi et al., 2023).
At the level of theory, the ability to mentalize is an evolutionarily prewired capacity which requires considerable environmental input to develop fully (Luyten et al., 2020). Initial iterations of the theory emphasised early attachment experiences as the crucible in which a sense of self and emotional agency develop through mentalizing (Fonagy et al., 2002; Fonagy and Bateman, 2019), initially via ‘marked mirroring’ exchanges with the primary caregiver (Gergely & Watson, 1996). Parents who are able to mentalize their child’s internal mental states, and convey these to the child through contingent and marked affective displays, were assumed to help their child to develop secure attachment representations, mentalizing ability and adaptive emotional regulation (Luyten et al., 2020). By contrast, consistently mis-attuned caregiving was theorised to lead to mentalizing difficulties, which then served as a vulnerability for psychopathology, including eating disorders, when faced with later developmental challenges (Fonagy et al., 2002). This core theory was adapted for eating disorders by Robinson et al. (2019) to include genetic predisposition, attachment disturbance and mentalizing dysfunction in a proposed aetiological pathway. However, evidence of an association between early attachment security and later eating pathology is lacking (Jewell et al., 2023a). More recent iterations of mentalizing theory have de-emphasized the role of early attachment (Luyten et al., 2020), and there has been a focus on the role of mentalizing as a transdiagnostic factor that may relate to outcomes of psychotherapy (Luyten et al., 2024).
Increasing evidence suggests that reflective function (RF), which refers to the ability to mentalize in the context of attachment relationships (Fonagy et al., 1991), may indeed play a role in the process and outcome of eating disorders treatment. The relevance of attachment to mentalizing relates to the historic theoretical emphasis on mentalizing as a skill learnt in the context of attachment relationships (Fonagy et al., 2002), as well as the initial work to develop measures of RF based on attachment interview transcripts (Fonagy et al., 1991). Both Katznelson et al. (2020) and Jewell et al. (2023b) found baseline RF to predict later therapeutic alliance scores in adult and adolescent eating disorders samples respectively, whilst RF has also predicted treatment outcome in adults with eating disorders (Kuipers et al., 2017). However, the assessment of RF through available observer-rated measures such as the RF Scale (Fonagy et al., 1998) applied to the Adult Attachment Interview (George et al., 1985) or transcripts of therapy sessions (e.g. Talia et al., 2019) is time-consuming, reducing the feasibility of evaluating mentalization in either routine clinical practice or research settings such as large-scale epidemiological studies (Fonagy et al., 2016). Consequently, there has been a need to develop valid and reliable self-report measures of RF.
The Reflective Function Questionnaire (RFQ) (Fonagy et al., 2016) was developed to fill this need. In devising the measure, Fonagy et al. (2016) initially developed a scoring method in which some items were scored on a Likert scale in which the highest scores were at the extreme end of the scale (polar method) whereas other items scored highest for items in the middle of the scale (median method). This approach was taken since, depending on item wording, higher RF might sometimes be reflected in responses in the middle of the Likert scale. This scoring method was used in the first published evaluation of the RFQ, the 46-item RFQ for Youth (RFQ-Y) (Ha et al., 2013), in which half the items were scored using the median method (Scale A) and half by the polar method (Scale B). However, the use of a median scoring approach is psychometrically sub-optimal and was removed from the eight-item adult RFQ8 in its final published version (Fonagy et al., 2016). Meanwhile, to improve the psychometric properties of the RFQ-Y, Sharp et al. (2022) refined the measure using the polar-scored items from Scale B, resulting in the five-item RFQY-5, demonstrating adequate fit for a unidimensional factor structure, good internal consistency and construct validity. However, whilst the RFQY-5 was validated in an adolescent psychiatric sample it has yet to be evaluated in an eating disorder sample. Such an evaluation is particularly important given that the starvation state in restrictive eating disorders is known to impact on theory of mind (Bora & Köse, 2016), and may therefore impact RF. Moreover, as yet there has been no evaluation of the sensitivity to change of the RFQY-5.
In summary, there is a need for a brief, psychometrically-sound self-report measure of adolescent RF to be evaluated in an eating disorder sample. The present study therefore assesses the psychometric properties of the RFQY-5 (Sharp et al., 2022) in a secondary analysis of data derived from a prospective observational study of family therapy treatment in adolescents with restrictive eating disorders (Jewell et al., 2023b).

Hypotheses

We tested the following hypotheses:
1)
Structural validity: confirmatory factor analysis will demonstrate a unidimensional structure of the RFQY-5 within established parameters of model fit (see Methods for details).
 
2)
Internal consistency: The RFQY-5 will demonstrate acceptable internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega values > 0.7.
 
3)
Construct validity: The RFQY-5 will be significantly, and negatively, correlated with specific scales of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, 2004), as found in the Sharp et al. (2022) study: specifically, we predict medium-sized correlations (Pearson’s r or Spearman’s rho around 0.3) for the Impulse Control Difficulties and Lack of Emotional Clarity, with a large correlation (r or rho > 0.5) with the Lack of Emotional Awareness scale. This was hypothesised since the RFQY-5 item content has an overlap with these scales, with some highly similar items contained in the Lack of Emotional Awareness scale. In addition, reflective functioning and emotional regulation are theoretically related concepts (Luyten et al., 2020), with evidence of an association between them across clinical and non-clinical samples (e.g. Sharp et al., 2011; Schwarzer et al., 2021).
 
4)
Sensitivity to change: we predict significant increases in RF between baseline (T1) scores and nine months later (T2) assessed using a paired t-test. Whilst improving RF is not a specific treatment target of family therapy for eating disorders, we hypothesised that the impact of starvation (such as psychological rigidity and reduced theory of mind) would lead to lower RF at baseline; by contrast, we hypothesised that weight restoration plus psychological progress towards eating disorder recovery would lead to increased RF at nine months.
 

Method

Data for this study were drawn from Jewell et al.’s (2023b) study in which 192 adolescents were recruited through consecutive sampling into a study of predictors of outcome in family therapy within specialist community eating disorder treatment centers. Adolescent participants were aged between 10 and 17 years and were diagnosed by a clinician with anorexia nervosa (AN) or a restrictive Other Specified Feeding or Eating Disorder (OSFED) using DSM-5 criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Recruitment took place across three specialist community eating disorder centers in England. Participants had a mean age of 14.7 years, were 88.4% female, and 79.8% were of White British ethnicity. Family therapy for adolescent anorexia nervosa (FT-AN) was offered to families as part of routine treatment in line with national guidelines recommending this as the first-line treatment for AN and similar presentations in adolescence (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2017). Participants were recruited at the start of their family therapy treatment and completed a range of self-report measures of attachment, mentalization and emotion regulation difficulties. Full details on inclusion criteria, ethical approval, original sample size calculation and recruitment (including flowchart) are included in the original paper. In brief, the study received ethical approval, and all participants provided written consent. Participants could withdraw from the study at any time without providing a reason.
The final sample for the present study comprises 171 adolescents for whom Time 1 data were available, out of 192 participants recruited to the Jewell et al. (2023b) study. Exclusions were as follows: 15 adolescents did not complete any measures for the study; two participants withdrew their consent; four were recruited to the study in error without meeting inclusion criteria.
For the present study, we consulted the latest COSMIN guidelines (Mokkink et al., 2019) on sample sizes for studies evaluating psychometric properties. COSMIN criteria for ‘very good’ design were satisfied by a sample > 100 for structural validity of a five-item measure, internal consistency and construct validity, and by a sample > 50 for sensitivity to change. To maximize the available sample size, our analyses included both male and female participants, and these findings are reported in the main body of this paper. However, given our largely female sample, as a secondary analysis we also repeated all analyses on a female-only sample and report these findings as an appendix.

Measures

Reflective Function Questionnaire – Youth
(RFQ-Y - (Ha et al., 2013). This 46-item measure yields a total score, with higher scores representing greater reflective function. The RFQ-Y has adequate internal reliability; McDonald’s Omega was 0.75 in this study. Convergent, criterion, discriminant and construct validity have been demonstrated, including convergence with an interview-based measure of RF (Ha et al., 2013). For the purposes of this study, we utilized only the five items belonging to the RFQY-5 (Sharp et al., 2022) for all analyses. Items are scored on a 6-point scale from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. A sample item is: ‘I pay attention to my feelings’.
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Strategies Scale
(DERS - (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). This 36-item measure assesses emotion regulation difficulties and is comprised of six subscales: Non-acceptance of Emotional Responses, Difficulties Engaging in Goal-directed Behaviors, Impulse Control Difficulties, Lack of Emotional Awareness, Limited Access to Effective Emotion Regulation Strategies and Lack of Emotional Clarity. The measure has demonstrated high internal consistency and good construct and predictive validity in clinical and non-clinical populations (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) and has been evaluated in adolescents. High scores represent greater emotion regulation difficulties. McDonald’s Omega for DERS subscales in this study ranged from 0.81 to 0.94. Items are scored on a 5-point scale from ‘almost never’ to ‘almost always’. A sample item is: ‘When I’m upset, I feel out of control’.

Data Analytic Strategy

Primary analyses were conducted in SPSS version 28.0 with Hayes’ omega macro to calculate omega (Hayes & Coutts, 2020). Factor analyses were conducted in MPlus version 8.6 (Muthén & Muthén, 2017). The best fitting model was determined by examining the following fit indices: the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), with values of less than 0.08 indicating reasonable fit and values above 0.10 suggesting poor fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993); the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), with values between 0.95 and 1.00 indicating excellent fit and values between 0.90 and 0.95 indicating acceptable fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR), with values less than 0.08 indicating acceptable fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Results of chi-square tests are reported; however, the chi-square test is sensitive to sample size, so these results were not given as much weight as the fit indices listed above (Fan et al., 1999). Sensitivity to change was assessed using paired sample t-tests. The normality of data and presence of outliers were inspected visually using histograms and Q-Q plots, as well as through checking of values for skewness and kurtosis. Values for skewness and kurtosis for the RFQY-5 at Time 1 and Time 5 were within acceptable limits for normality. Values for two subscales of the DERS were outside of acceptable limits, therefore non-parametric tests were used for correlations.

Results

The sample included 151 females and 20 male participants, with a mean age of 14.7 years. Characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the sample and means for main study variables
 
N
Minimum
Maximum
Mean
SD
 
Age
171
11
17
14.72
1.52
 
% Median Body Mass Index
171
62.27
113.79
83.75
8.98
 
Duration of illness (Months)
171
1
65
10.50
10.51
 
RFQY-5 (T1)
171
11
30
21.26
3.85
 
RFQY-5 (T2)
51
11
29
21.43
3.77
 
DERS Nonacceptance (T1)
171
6
30
18.56
7.55
 
DERS Goals (T1)
171
5
25
17.99
5.19
 
DERS Impulse (T1)
171
6
30
17.57
6.82
 
DERS Awareness (T1)
171
6
30
17.59
5.11
 
DERS Strategies (T1)
171
8
40
25.77
8.59
 
DERS Clarity (T1)
171
5
25
14.54
4.99
 
 
N
%
    
Sex:
      
 - Female
151
88.3
    
 - Male
20
11.7
    
Lives with:
      
 - Biological/ adoptive parents
128
74.9
    
 - Mother alone
24
14.0
    
 - Other
19
11.1
    
Ethnicity:
      
 - White British
135
78.9
    
 - Other White
14
8.2
    
 - Dual Heritage
10
5.8
    
 - British Asian/Other South Asian
9
5.2
    
 - Other
3
1.8
    
Diagnosis:
      
 - Anorexia Nervosa (restricting sub-type)
139
81.3
    
 - Anorexia Nervosa (binge/purge sub-type)
9
5.3
    
 - Other Specified Feeding or Eating Disorder
23
13.5
    
Self-harm reported at assessment
34
19.9
    
Abbreviations: RFQY-5, Reflective Function Questionnaire for Youth – 5-item version. DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Strategies Scale; DERS Nonacceptance, DERS Nonacceptance of Emotional Responses; DERS Goals, DERS Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behaviors; DERS Impulse, DERS Impulse Control Difficulties; DERS Awareness, DERS Lack of Emotional Awareness; DERS Strategies, DERS Limited Access to Effective Emotion Regulation Strategies; DERS Clarity, DERS Lack of Emotional Clarity; Notes: Weight data are reported as a percentage of median Body Mass Index (%mBMI), adjusted for height, age, and sex. Duration of illness in months refers to the number of months that the young person had been unwell for prior to their assessment in clinic.

Structural Validity

Factor analysis revealed poor model fit for the unidimensional structure (Χ2(5) = 29.98; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.801; TLI = 0.602; RMSEA = 0.171; SRMR = 0.070).

Internal Consistency

At baseline, McDonald’s omega = 0.657, and Cronbach’s alpha = 0.633. Inter-item correlations are presented in Table 2. The mean inter-item correlation was 0.254, with item 4 showing extremely low correlations with most other items, pulling down the average.
Table 2
Inter-item correlations at baseline (n = 171)
 
1
2
3
4
1. I pay attention to my feelings
    
2. In an argument, I keep the others person’s point of view in mind.
0.356**
   
3. I like to think about reasons behind my actions.
0.198**
0.356**
  
4. I’m often curious about the meaning behind others’ actions.
0.003
0.036
0.386**
 
5. I pay attention to the impact of my actions on others’ feelings.
0.278**
0.389**
0.455**
0.078
*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)

Construct Validity

The RFQY-5 was significantly negatively correlated with the DERS Impulse Control Difficulties (Spearman’s rho = –0.245, p < 0.01) and Lack of Emotional Awareness scale (Spearman’s rho = –0.522, p < 0.01), but not Lack of Emotional Clarity (Spearman’s rho = –0.150, p = 0.050). A correlation matrix is available in Appendix A.

Sensitivity to Change

Change between baseline and 9 months was not significant (T(50) = 0.643; p = 0.261).

Analysis of Female-Only Sample

Given that girls comprised 88% of the sample, we conducted all of the above analyses in a female-only sample. These results are presented in Appendix B. There were no notable differences between these results and those obtained for the mixed sample, other than significant correlations between the RFQY-5 and four of the six DERS subscales, in contrast to two out of four subscales for the mixed sample.

Discussion

Our findings point to the RFQY-5 having inadequate psychometric properties in adolescents with restrictive eating disorders. Whilst convergent validity with two scales from the DERS (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) was demonstrated, we found the structural validity and internal reliability to be below accepted threshold values. In addition, the RFQY-5 was not sensitive to change between baseline and nine months. This begs the question: what explains this sub-optimal performance in adolescents with eating disorders?
We suggest that our findings be considered through the lens of ‘trait vs. state’. The internal reliability of the RFQY-5 in our sample (alpha = 0.63) was lower than the satisfactory value reported by Sharp et al. (2022) (alpha = 0.75) even taking into account adjusted internal reliability standards for very short measures (Vaske et al., 2017). Our table of inter-item correlations (Table 2) identifies Item 4 (“I’m often curious about the meaning behind others’ actions”) as performing poorly, since it correlates significantly with only one other RFQ-5 item. In terms of states, it is possible that the impact of starvation could be at play. Bora and Köse (2016) found meta-analytic evidence of impaired theory of mind in individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN), with larger effect sizes for participants with acute compared with recovered AN. It is thus possible that adolescents who are acutely unwell report lower curiosity about others’ mental states, and this might change with physical recovery. However, by nine months, there was no significant change in scores on the RFQY-5, raising two possibilities – firstly, that nine months is too short an interval for changes in mentalizing to be apparent on a self-reported measure; secondly, that the RFQY-5 items are insensitive to change and might capture trait-like aspects of adolescent mentalizing. Relatedly, one must consider that rates of autism are raised in adolescent AN (Westwood et al., 2018) and this might also influence measurement variance of the RFQY-5 in this population. Finally, the RFQY-5 is regarded as an evaluation of a person’s capacity for mentalizing other minds (Sharp et al., 2022). It is possible that the measurement variance identified in this study is explained by differences in mentalizing of others’ minds in adolescents with AN relative to their peers. Future studies could utilise case-control designs to investigate both mentalizing of the self as well as of the other, to better understand the extent to which mentalizing within this population may differ from controls.
Our study has several strengths and limitations which should be borne in mind. Our sample is representative of UK clinic-attending adolescents with restrictive eating disorders, and the sample size is sufficient to undertake our analyses. However, the majority of participants were female and White British, precluding conclusions about how the RFQY-5 might perform in other groups or indeed nationalities. In addition, the five items were selected off the 46-item data which is not ideal. A future validation study should administer only the five items given known effects of the impact items have on each other when administered together.
An important implication of our study is that clinicians and researchers seeking a psychometrically robust self-report measure of reflective function in adolescents with eating disorders currently do not have strong evidence for the validity and reliability of any measure. One option is to utilise the full-length RFQ-Y (Ha et al., 2013), which demonstrated acceptable internal reliability in this population, as reported in Jewell et al. (2023b). In this present study, our focus was on evaluating the psychometric properties of the 5-item version due to its brevity, and we were unable to do a full psychometric evaluation of the 46 items since standards recommend a larger sample size than we have available for a measure of that length. However, Sharp et al. (2022) have highlighted some of the shortcomings of the 46-item RFQ-Y, such as its sub-optimal scoring method.
Our findings highlight the need to develop valid and reliable measures of mentalizing in adolescents and adults with eating disorders. Existing measures which could be investigated in this population include the Certainty About Mental States Questionnaire (Müller et al., 2023) and the Mentalization Scale (Dimitrijević et al., 2018). The RFQ8 (Fonagy et al., 2016) represents another potential choice, but the current scoring method, in which several items contribute to two different scales, is not ideal, and its factor structure is unclear, with recent research suggesting a unidimensional factor structure, rather than the two-factor structure reported initially (Müller et al., 2022; Woźniak-Prus et al., 2022). We also recommend that clinicians and researchers carefully consider the item content of available measures when choosing instruments and interpreting results. The item coverage provided by a self-report measure such as the RFQ in any of its forms is unlikely to capture differences in mentalizing ‘modes’ (Fonagy et al., 2002; Luyten et al., 2020) that have been seen as clinically relevant in eating disorders (Robinson et al., 2019). For the adolescent eating disorders field, recent qualitative data supports the idea that mentalizing changes are important in the recovery process for adolescents with AN (Baudinet et al., 2023). Further investigation of the role of mentalizing in recovery via quantitative means will require adequate measurement tools that are sensitive to change.
In summary, there is a need for brief, psychometrically robust measures of reflective function in eating disorder samples. Our study suggests that the RFQY-5 does not perform adequately in this population, despite having performed well in a prior study by Sharp et al. (2022) that included adolescent community and psychiatric inpatient samples. Future validation studies should include not only self-report but also task-based and observer-rated measures that can assess mentalizing in ecologically valid ways, as well as capturing the more nuanced aspects of mentalizing that may be seen as clinically important. The development of new measures should involve those with lived experience of eating disorders to ensure that tasks and items are salient. Validation studies should recruit diverse samples and assess measurement invariance across cultures, ethnicities, and genders, as well as in autistic individuals.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

Ethics approval

The study was approved by the Camden and King’s Cross ethics committee of the Health Research Authority, United Kingdom.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (fifth ed). American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (fifth ed). American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Baudinet, J., Eisler, I., Konstantellou, A., Hunt, T., Kassamali, F., McLaughlin, N., Simic, M., & Schmidt, U. (2023). Perceived change mechanisms in multi-family therapy for anorexia nervosa: A qualitative follow-up study of adolescent and parent experiences. European Eating Disorders Review, 31(6), 822–836. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.3006 Baudinet, J., Eisler, I., Konstantellou, A., Hunt, T., Kassamali, F., McLaughlin, N., Simic, M., & Schmidt, U. (2023). Perceived change mechanisms in multi-family therapy for anorexia nervosa: A qualitative follow-up study of adolescent and parent experiences. European Eating Disorders Review, 31(6), 822–836. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​erv.​3006
go back to reference Bizzi, F., Riva, A., Charpentier Mora, S., Tironi, M., Sforza, S. E., Milani, L. M., & Nacinovich, R. (2023). Investigating Functioning Profile of adolescents with Anorexia before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional study on Mentalizing, Alexithymia, and impulsiveness. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(4). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043670. Article 4. Bizzi, F., Riva, A., Charpentier Mora, S., Tironi, M., Sforza, S. E., Milani, L. M., & Nacinovich, R. (2023). Investigating Functioning Profile of adolescents with Anorexia before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional study on Mentalizing, Alexithymia, and impulsiveness. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(4). https://​doi.​org/​10.​3390/​ijerph20043670. Article 4.
go back to reference Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Sage. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Sage.
go back to reference Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-functioning manual, version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London, 161–162. Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-functioning manual, version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London, 161–162.
go back to reference Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization and the development of the self. Routledge. Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization and the development of the self. Routledge.
go back to reference Fonagy, P. & Bateman, A. (2019). Introduction. In A. Bateman, & P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of mentalizing in mental health practice (pp. 3–20). American Psychiatric Publishing. Fonagy, P. & Bateman, A. (2019). Introduction. In A. Bateman, & P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of mentalizing in mental health practice (pp. 3–20). American Psychiatric Publishing.
go back to reference George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Attachment interview for adults [Unpublished manuscript]. George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Attachment interview for adults [Unpublished manuscript].
go back to reference Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1996). The social biofeedback model of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 1181–1212.PubMed Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1996). The social biofeedback model of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 1181–1212.PubMed
go back to reference Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional Assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.CrossRef Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional Assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.CrossRef
go back to reference Jewell, T., Collyer, H., Gardner, T., Tchanturia, K., Simic, M., Fonagy, P., & Eisler, I. (2016). Attachment and mentalization and their association with child and adolescent eating pathology: A systematic review. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 49(4), 354–373. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22473.CrossRefPubMed Jewell, T., Collyer, H., Gardner, T., Tchanturia, K., Simic, M., Fonagy, P., & Eisler, I. (2016). Attachment and mentalization and their association with child and adolescent eating pathology: A systematic review. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 49(4), 354–373. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​eat.​22473.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jewell, T., Apostolidou, E., Sadikovic, K., Tahta-Wraith, K., Liston, S., Simic, M., Eisler, I., Fonagy, P., & Yorke, I. (2023a). Attachment in individuals with eating disorders compared to community controls: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 56(5), 888–908. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23922.CrossRefPubMed Jewell, T., Apostolidou, E., Sadikovic, K., Tahta-Wraith, K., Liston, S., Simic, M., Eisler, I., Fonagy, P., & Yorke, I. (2023a). Attachment in individuals with eating disorders compared to community controls: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 56(5), 888–908. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​eat.​23922.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kuipers, G. S., den Hollander, S., van der Ark, L. A., & Bekker, M. H. J. (2017). Recovery from eating disorder 1 year after start of treatment is related to better mentalization and strong reduction of sensitivity to others. Eating and Weight disorders - studies on Anorexia. Bulimia and Obesity, 22(3), 535–547. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-017-0405-x.CrossRef Kuipers, G. S., den Hollander, S., van der Ark, L. A., & Bekker, M. H. J. (2017). Recovery from eating disorder 1 year after start of treatment is related to better mentalization and strong reduction of sensitivity to others. Eating and Weight disorders - studies on Anorexia. Bulimia and Obesity, 22(3), 535–547. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s40519-017-0405-x.CrossRef
go back to reference Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2017). Mplus. Handbook of Item Response Theory. Chapman and Hall/CRC. Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2017). Mplus. Handbook of Item Response Theory. Chapman and Hall/CRC.
go back to reference Robinson, P., Skårderud, F., & Sommerfeldt, B. (2019). Hunger: Mentalization-based treatments for eating disorders. Springer. Robinson, P., Skårderud, F., & Sommerfeldt, B. (2019). Hunger: Mentalization-based treatments for eating disorders. Springer.
go back to reference Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., Katznelson, H., Pedersen, S. H., Steele, H., Schröder, P., Origlieri, A., Scharff, F. B., Giovanardi, G., Andersson, M., Lingiardi, V., Safran, J. D., Lunn, S., Poulsen, S., & Taubner, S. (2019). Mentalizing in the presence of another: Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process. Psychotherapy Research, 29(5), 652–665. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2017.1417651.CrossRefPubMed Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., Katznelson, H., Pedersen, S. H., Steele, H., Schröder, P., Origlieri, A., Scharff, F. B., Giovanardi, G., Andersson, M., Lingiardi, V., Safran, J. D., Lunn, S., Poulsen, S., & Taubner, S. (2019). Mentalizing in the presence of another: Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process. Psychotherapy Research, 29(5), 652–665. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​10503307.​2017.​1417651.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Psychometric Properties of the Reflective Function Questionnaire for Youth Five-Item Version in Adolescents with Restrictive Eating Disorders
Auteurs
Tom Jewell
Veronica McLaren
Carla Sharp
Publicatiedatum
03-08-2024
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment / Uitgave 3/2024
Print ISSN: 0882-2689
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3505
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-024-10158-9