Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5-6/2005

01-06-2005 | Original Article

Processing abstract sequence structure: learning without knowing, or knowing without learning?

Auteurs: Maud Boyer, Arnaud Destrebecqz, Axel Cleeremans

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5-6/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Constant interaction with a dynamic environment—from riding a bicycle to segmenting speech—makes sensitivity to the sequential structure of the world a fundamental dimension of information processing. Accounts of sequence learning vary widely, with some authors arguing that parsing and segmentation processes are central, and others proposing that sequence learning involves mere memorization. In this paper, we argue that sequence knowledge is essentially statistical in nature, and that sequence learning involves simple associative prediction mechanisms. We focus on a choice reaction situation introduced by Lee (1997), in which participants were exposed to material that follows a single abstract rule, namely that stimuli are selected randomly, but never appear more than once in a legal sequence. Perhaps surprisingly, people can learn this rule very well. Or can they? We offer a conceptual replication of the original finding, but a very different interpretation of the results, as well as simulation work that makes it clear how highly abstract dimensions of the stimulus material can in fact be learned based on elementary associative mechanisms. We conclude that, when relevant, memory is optimized to facilitate responding to events that have not occurred recently, and that sequence learning in general always involves sensitivity to repetition distance.
Voetnoten
1
All networks involved dual connection weights as described in Cleeremans and McClelland (1991). Slow and fast weights were associated with learning rates of .04 and .45 respectively. Momentum was .9, and the fast weights decayed at a rate of .4. For each simulation study described in the text, twelve networks initialized with different random weights selected in the −.5–+.5 range were each trained in a total of 30,240 trials (720 sequences × 6 elements × 7 epochs), and their responses averaged.
 
2
An error in the stimulus generation program resulted in one instance of a stimulus associated with a lag of 21.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
go back to reference Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science, 2, 296–408. Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science, 2, 296–408.
go back to reference Anderson, N. H. (1960). Effect of first-order conditional probability in a two-choice situation.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 73–93. Anderson, N. H. (1960). Effect of first-order conditional probability in a two-choice situation.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 73–93.
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The capacity for generating information by randomization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 119–129. Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The capacity for generating information by randomization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 119–129.
go back to reference Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, UK: Erlbaum. Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Boyer, M., Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (1998). The serial reaction time task: Learning without knowing, or knowing without learning? In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 167–172). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Boyer, M., Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (1998). The serial reaction time task: Learning without knowing, or knowing without learning? In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 167–172). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Brooks, L. R. (1978). Non-analytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and concepts (pp. 16–211). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brooks, L. R. (1978). Non-analytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and concepts (pp. 16–211). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Budescu, D. V. (1987). A Markov model for generation of random binary sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 25–39. Budescu, D. V. (1987). A Markov model for generation of random binary sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 25–39.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A. (1993). Mechanisms of implicit learning: Connectionist models of sequence processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cleeremans, A. (1993). Mechanisms of implicit learning: Connectionist models of sequence processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A. (1997). Principles for implicit learning. In D. C. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning? (pp. 195–234). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cleeremans, A. (1997). Principles for implicit learning. In D. C. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning? (pp. 195–234). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A. (forthcoming). Being virtual. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cleeremans, A. (forthcoming). Being virtual. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (in press). Real rules are conscious: Open peer commentary to Pothos. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (in press). Real rules are conscious: Open peer commentary to Pothos. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (1998). Implicit sequence learning: The truth is in the details. In M. A. Stadler & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 323–364). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (1998). Implicit sequence learning: The truth is in the details. In M. A. Stadler & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 323–364). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness: An empirical, computational and philosophical consensus in the making? (pp. 1–40). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness: An empirical, computational and philosophical consensus in the making? (pp. 1–40). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–253. Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–253.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406–416. Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406–416.
go back to reference Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J., & Keele, S. W. (1998). Sequence learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 275–281. Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J., & Keele, S. W. (1998). Sequence learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 275–281.
go back to reference Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little, Brown Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little, Brown
go back to reference Dominey, P. F. (1998). Influences of temporal organization on sequence learning and transfer: Comments on Stadler (1995) and Curran and Keele (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 234–248. Dominey, P. F. (1998). Influences of temporal organization on sequence learning and transfer: Comments on Stadler (1995) and Curran and Keele (1993). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 234–248.
go back to reference Dominey, P. F., Lelekov, T., Ventre-Dominey, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Dissociable processes for learning the surface structure and abstract structure of sensorimotor sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 734–751. Dominey, P. F., Lelekov, T., Ventre-Dominey, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Dissociable processes for learning the surface structure and abstract structure of sensorimotor sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 734–751.
go back to reference Edwards, W. (1961). Probability learning in 1000 trials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 597–599. Edwards, W. (1961). Probability learning in 1000 trials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 597–599.
go back to reference Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179–211. Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179–211.
go back to reference Feldman, J. (1959). On the negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 291–297. Feldman, J. (1959). On the negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 291–297.
go back to reference Frith, C. D., Perry, R., & Lumer, E. (1999). The neural correlates of conscious experience: An experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 105–114. Frith, C. D., Perry, R., & Lumer, E. (1999). The neural correlates of conscious experience: An experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 105–114.
go back to reference Hinton, G. E. (1986). Learning distributed representations of concepts. InProceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1–12). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hinton, G. E. (1986). Learning distributed representations of concepts. InProceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1–12). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Hintzmann, D. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review,93, 411–428. Hintzmann, D. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review,93, 411–428.
go back to reference Jarvik, M. E. (1951). Probability learning and a negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 291–297. Jarvik, M. E. (1951). Probability learning and a negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 291–297.
go back to reference Jiménez, L., Mendez, C., & Cleeremans, A. (1996). Comparing direct and indirect measures of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 22, 948–969. Jiménez, L., Mendez, C., & Cleeremans, A. (1996). Comparing direct and indirect measures of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 22, 948–969.
go back to reference Keren, G., & Lewis, C. (1994). The two fallacies of gamblers: Type I and Type II. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 75–89. Keren, G., & Lewis, C. (1994). The two fallacies of gamblers: Type I and Type II. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 75–89.
go back to reference Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 138–146. Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 138–146.
go back to reference Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). Patterns, chunks, and hierarchies in serial reaction-time tasks. Psychological Research, 63, 22–35. Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). Patterns, chunks, and hierarchies in serial reaction-time tasks. Psychological Research, 63, 22–35.
go back to reference Lee, Y. S. (1997). Learning and awareness in the serial reaction time task. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lee, Y. S. (1997). Learning and awareness in the serial reaction time task. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24–37. Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24–37.
go back to reference Lopes, L. L. (1982). Doing the impossible: A note on induction and the experience of randomness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 626–636. Lopes, L. L. (1982). Doing the impossible: A note on induction and the experience of randomness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 626–636.
go back to reference Luce, R. D. (1963). Detection and recognition. In R. R. Busch & E. Galanter (Eds), Handbook of mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. New York: Wiley. Luce, R. D. (1963). Detection and recognition. In R. R. Busch & E. Galanter (Eds), Handbook of mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Lupiañez, J., Milliken, B., Solano, C., Weaver, B., & Tipper, S. P. (2001). On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 753–773. Lupiañez, J., Milliken, B., Solano, C., Weaver, B., & Tipper, S. P. (2001). On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 753–773.
go back to reference Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning? Oxford: Oxford University Press. Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283, 77–80. Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283, 77–80.
go back to reference McClelland, J. L., & Plaut, D. C. (1999). Does generalization in infant learning implicate abstract algebra-like rules?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 166–168. McClelland, J. L., & Plaut, D. C. (1999). Does generalization in infant learning implicate abstract algebra-like rules?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 166–168.
go back to reference Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can do: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can do: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirement of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32. Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirement of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.
go back to reference Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426. Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426.
go back to reference Perruchet, P. (1985). A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 20, 163–170. Perruchet, P. (1985). A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 20, 163–170.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., & Gallego, G. (1997). A subjective unit formation account of implicit learning. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit knowledge? (pp. 124–161). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Perruchet, P., & Gallego, G. (1997). A subjective unit formation account of implicit learning. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit knowledge? (pp. 124–161). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2003). The self-organizing consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297. Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2003). The self-organizing consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297.
go back to reference Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (in preparation). When automatic priming prevails over conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Perruchet, P., Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (in preparation). When automatic priming prevails over conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., & Vaughan, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 211–228. Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., & Vaughan, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 211–228.
go back to reference Pothos, E. M. (in press). The rules versus similarity distinction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Pothos, E. M. (in press). The rules versus similarity distinction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
go back to reference Rapoport, A., & Budescu, D. V. (1997). Randomization in individual choice behavior. Psychological Review, 104, 603–617. Rapoport, A., & Budescu, D. V. (1997). Randomization in individual choice behavior. Psychological Review, 104, 603–617.
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 855–863. Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 855–863.
go back to reference Redington, M., & Chater, N. (1996). Transfer in artificial grammar learning: A reevaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 123–138. Redington, M., & Chater, N. (1996). Transfer in artificial grammar learning: A reevaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 123–138.
go back to reference Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 585–594. Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 585–594.
go back to reference Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, 101–105. Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, 101–105.
go back to reference Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52. Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52.
go back to reference Servan-Schreiber, D., Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks. Machine Learning, 7, 161–193. Servan-Schreiber, D., Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks. Machine Learning, 7, 161–193.
go back to reference Servan-Schreiber, E., & Anderson, J. R. (1990). Learning artificial grammar with competitive chunking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 592–608. Servan-Schreiber, E., & Anderson, J. R. (1990). Learning artificial grammar with competitive chunking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 592–608.
go back to reference Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–447. Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–447.
go back to reference Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case for rules in reasoning. Cognitive Science, 16, 1–40. Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case for rules in reasoning. Cognitive Science, 16, 1–40.
go back to reference Stadler, M. A. (1992). Statistical structure and implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 318–327. Stadler, M. A. (1992). Statistical structure and implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 318–327.
go back to reference Taylor, T. L., & Klein, R. M. (1998). On the causes and effects of inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 625–643. Taylor, T. L., & Klein, R. M. (1998). On the causes and effects of inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 625–643.
go back to reference Wagenaar, W. A. (1972). Generation of random sequences by human subjects: A critical survey of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 65–72. Wagenaar, W. A. (1972). Generation of random sequences by human subjects: A critical survey of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 65–72.
go back to reference Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427, 352–355. Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427, 352–355.
go back to reference Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press. Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Processing abstract sequence structure: learning without knowing, or knowing without learning?
Auteurs
Maud Boyer
Arnaud Destrebecqz
Axel Cleeremans
Publicatiedatum
01-06-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5-6/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0207-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5-6/2005

Psychological Research 5-6/2005 Naar de uitgave