Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

14-03-2024 | Research

Proactive response preparation contributes to contingency learning: novel evidence from force-sensitive keyboards

Auteurs: Daniel H. Weissman, James R. Schmidt

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2024

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Contingency learning can involve learning that the identity of one stimulus in a sequence predicts the identity of the next stimulus. It remains unclear, however, whether such learning speeds responses to the next stimulus only by reducing the threshold for triggering the expected response after stimulus onset or also by preparing the expected response before stimulus onset. To distinguish between these competing accounts, we manipulated the probabilities with which each of two prime arrows (Left and Right) were followed by each of two probe arrows (Up and Down) in a prime-probe task while using force-sensitive keyboards to monitor sub-threshold finger force. Consistent with the response preparation account, two experiments revealed greater force just before probe onset on the response key corresponding to the direction in which the probe was more (versus less) likely to point (e.g., Up vs. Down). Furthermore, mirroring sequential contingency effects in behavior, this pre-probe force effect vanished after a single low-probability trial. These findings favor the response preparation account over the threshold only account. They also suggest the possibility that contingency learning in our tasks indexes trial-by-trial expectations regarding the utility of the prime for predicting the upcoming probe.
Voetnoten
1
In these studies, contingency learning effects appear when the prime and probe appear simultaneously. Such effects, however, are stronger when the prime appears before the probe (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2016b).
 
2
The finding may alternatively be explained by the fact that the relative frequencies with which high- and medium-contingency trials appeared (50% vs. 25%) differed more than the relative frequencies with which medium- and low-frequency trials appeared (25% vs. 17%).
 
3
We mistakenly reported this partial-eta-squared value as 0.41 in our pre-registration.
 
4
To be consistent with our prior studies of the prime-probe task, we used the same key-hand mapping for all participants, rather than mapping the left and right arrow-direction keys to the left hand and the up and down arrow-direction keys to the right hand in half the participants and using the opposite key-hand mapping in the other half. Using a constant mapping does not lead to a design confound because we use the same mapping in all conditions.
 
5
We identified outliers using both correct and error RTs in the analysis of the mean ER data, not only correct RTs as in the analysis of the mean RT data. Thus, these analyses produced slightly different percentages of outliers.
 
6
In our pre-registration, we stated that we would analyze force in trials with correct responses to both the prime and the probe. An anonymous reviewer pointed out a valid concern with this approach, however. Excluding trials with an incorrect response to the probe could bias the results to show greater pre-probe force on the high-contingency response key than on the low-contingency response key. Indeed, randomly increasing pre-probe force on the high-contingency response key (e.g., due to guessing) could facilitate a correct probe response such that the trial is included in the analysis. In contrast, randomly increasing pre-probe force on the low-contingency response key could facilitate an incorrect probe response such that the trial is not included. To prevent such a bias, we analyzed pre-probe force regardless of whether the subsequent probe response was correct. We note that this change to our pre-registered analysis plan did not change any of the inferences that we describe below.
 
7
The authors thank Alexander Weigard at the University of Michigan for bringing this possibility to their attention.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 4, 480–506.CrossRef Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 4, 480–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, J. N. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 166–174.CrossRef Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, J. N. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 166–174.CrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M. (2019). Learning simple associations. Canadian Psychology, 60(1), 3.CrossRef MacLeod, C. M. (2019). Learning simple associations. Canadian Psychology, 60(1), 3.CrossRef
go back to reference Rousseeuw, P. J., & Crouz, C. (1993). Alternatives to the median absolute deviation. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88(424), 1273–1283.CrossRef Rousseeuw, P. J., & Crouz, C. (1993). Alternatives to the median absolute deviation. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88(424), 1273–1283.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, J. R. (2018). Best not to bet on the horserace: A comment on Forrin and MacLeod (2017) and a relevant stimulus-response compatibility view of colour-word contingency learning asymmetries. Memory and Cognition, 46, 326–335.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt, J. R. (2018). Best not to bet on the horserace: A comment on Forrin and MacLeod (2017) and a relevant stimulus-response compatibility view of colour-word contingency learning asymmetries. Memory and Cognition, 46, 326–335.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Weissman, D. H. (2019). Let your fingers do the walking: Finger force distinguishes competing accounts of the congruency sequence effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1619–1626.CrossRef Weissman, D. H. (2019). Let your fingers do the walking: Finger force distinguishes competing accounts of the congruency sequence effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1619–1626.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Proactive response preparation contributes to contingency learning: novel evidence from force-sensitive keyboards
Auteurs
Daniel H. Weissman
James R. Schmidt
Publicatiedatum
14-03-2024
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2024
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01940-1