Current Study
The current study focuses on the impact of a family-based poverty reduction intervention on family functioning, as reported by children. This emphasis on children’s perspectives is significant because it challenges traditional adult-centric approaches to family studies and provides unique insights into family dynamics from the children’s viewpoint. Understanding children’s perceptions and experiences is essential for grasping the impact of the family environment on their development and well-being. Our study contributes to the growing body of research aimed at understanding children’s lived experiences and how they perceive, interpret, and navigate their daily lives (Gross-Manos et al.,
2021).
Furthermore, this study addresses a notable gap in the literature by examining potential variations in how these interventions affect family functioning based on a child’s gender and orphanhood status. This focus is crucial, as a child’s gender can significantly influence family dynamics, support systems, and responses to stressors. Extensive research highlights the importance of gender socialization and gender variations in child-parent relationships (Tenenbaum & May,
2013), emphasizing the need to address this gap. Evidence indicates significant gender differences in children’s relationships with caregivers and the unique roles that family support and specific caregivers play in children’s well-being (Colarossi & Eccles,
2003; Feldman et al.,
2018; Figlio et al.,
2019; Raley & Bianchi,
2006; Rueger et al.,
2008; Van Polanen et al.,
2017). Gender-based expectations and socialization practices within families often influence how children cope with stress and the support they receive. (Endendijk et al.,
2016). Parental adherence to traditional gender roles shapes interactions and opportunities for children, impacting overall family dynamics (Few‐Demo & Allen,
2020). Studies suggest that communication patterns often reflect gender stereotypes, with a tendency to discuss emotions more with daughters than sons, affecting family bonds, support systems, and overall family functioning (Aznar & Tenenbaum,
2015; Chaplin et al.,
2005; Fivush et al.,
2019).
In contrast, the impact of orphanhood status on family functioning and the effectiveness of poverty reduction interventions is less understood, highlighting a significant gap in the literature. AIDS-orphaned children, in particular, face compounded challenges, such as the loss of primary caregivers, economic instability, and social stigma, which can exacerbate their vulnerabilities (Ashaba et al.,
2019; Raymond & Zolnikov,
2018). These children often experience heightened economic stress and rely more heavily on extended family or community networks for support (Heymann et al.,
2007; Karimli et al.,
2012; Motha,
2018), where family functioning plays a crucial role in mitigating the effects of poverty and can lead to different dynamics and outcomes compared to children with living parents. Understanding how orphanhood status intersects with intervention outcomes is essential for tailoring effective support strategies and addressing the unique needs of these children.
Our study represents the first comprehensive investigation into the intervention’s effect on family functioning, with a specific focus on the nuanced effects of a child’s gender and orphanhood status. Drawing upon the asset theory framework discussed earlier, we also recognize the potential variations in how poverty and economic stressors influence family functioning relative to a child’s gender and orphanhood status. To address the existing research gap in this area, we investigate two primary research questions: (1) What is the effect of a family-based poverty reduction intervention on family functioning, as perceived and reported by children? (2) How does this impact differ based on the child’s gender and orphanhood status?
Methods
Study Design and Sampling
The study utilized data from 1410 children (n = 621 boys and n = 789 girls) enrolled in a three-arm randomized controlled trial. To prevent cross-arm contamination, randomization was conducted at the school level (n = 48). The initial screening covered 88 public primary schools across four study districts: Rakai, Masaka, Lwengo, and Kalungu in south-central Uganda. Among these, 48 schools were selected based on their medium-sized student population. Schools with enrollments exceeding 900 students were excluded, as they surpassed the average size, and those with fewer than 35 students per grade were also excluded, as they fell below the typical enrollment threshold of at least 50 students per grade. The final selection comprised schools that committed to participating in the study.
Recruitment relied heavily on collaboration with schools and local district administrations to identify eligible participants and facilitate enrollment. Schools organized informational meetings for families to learn about the study, during which research staff were present. These meetings provided parents with the opportunity to inquire about the study either individually or as part of a larger group. Children were eligible for enrollment if they met the following inclusion criteria: (1) being an AIDS orphan (having lost one or both parents to AIDS), (2) living in a family household, and (3) attending a public primary school at the fifth or sixth grade level (equivalent to sixth or seventh grades in the U.S. educational system). At the time of enrollment, the average age of children was 12.7 years, with ages ranging from 10 to 16 years.
Data were collected from the children and adolescents participating in the study over a period of five years (2011–2016) at the following intervals: baseline, 12 months, 24 months, 36 months, and 48 months. The intervention was administered over a 24-month period. By the end of the 48-month study period, the attrition rates were 8.8 and 10.6% for the two treatment arms, and 8.6% for the control arm. A design-based test for the independence of loss to follow-up indicated no significant differences in attrition rates between study conditions (Ssewamala et al.,
2021). Additionally, we conducted Little’s MCAR test (Little,
1988) to determine if the missing data were independent of both observed and unobserved variables, indicating no systematic missingness. The test compares the means of each variable across different patterns of missing data and calculates a chi-square statistic based on the differences between observed and expected means under the MCAR assumption. Our results, with a p-value of 0.485, suggested that the data might be MCAR, as the probability of missingness appeared unrelated to the data itself. Given these results, we consider the data missing at random and apply complete case analyses to manage the missing data.
A 90-minute structured survey was administered by Ugandan interviewers who had been trained in good clinical practice and had obtained the Collaborative Institutional Training Initiative (CITI) Certificate before interacting with study participants. Informed assent was obtained from adolescents, and informed consent was obtained from their caregivers for participation in the study. The study protocol was approved by Columbia University Institutional Review Board (AAAI1950) and the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology (SS2586). The study protocol is registered at Clinicaltrial.gov (ID# NCT01447615).
Measures
All measures used in this study had been previously tested in studies involving children and adolescents affected by AIDS in Uganda (Karimli et al.,
2019; Ssewamala et al.,
2009; Ssewamala et al.,
2016; Ssewamala et al.,
2021).
Outcome measures
Building on previous research that used the Family Assessment Measure (FAM) and Family Environment Scale (FES) to assess family dynamics (Booysen et al.,
2021), we utilize the three measures detailed below to examine family functioning.
Intervention
As previously described, the intervention included two treatment arms, with the match rate (i.e., financial incentive for saving) being the only difference between them. The average amount of savings accumulated in this intervention was around US$ 25 over the intervention period (Wang et al.,
2021), which does not hold conceptual significance for the purposes of our study. Therefore, our analyses combined both arms into a single treatment group, creating a binary measure of intervention (0=control group; 1=treatment group).
Statistical Analyses Procedures
We followed the CONSORT (Appendix 1) guidelines to report baseline sample characteristics. To examine baseline differences across the study arms, we reported adjusted Wald F-statistics (design-based F), which account for individual-level variations and potential between-school correlations.
The analyses, run in Stata 16, accounted for the multilevel nature of data with repeated measures being nested within individuals, time points, and schools. Given the randomization at the school level, there was a potential risk of correlation for within-school observations, which could violate the independence assumption (Raudenbush & Bryk,
2002). To address this, we ran multilevel mixed-effects models, incorporating both between-school and within-individual variability as random effects, to estimate subject-specific effects while accounting for school-level clustering and potential within-individual correlations (Gelman & Hill,
2006). Finally, we decomposed the effects into comparisons against reference categories to obtain the marginal treatment effect at each time point.
To answer our first question (What is the effect of a family-based poverty reduction intervention on family functioning, as perceived and reported by children?) we ran multilevel mixed-effects models as specified below:
$${{\rm{Y}}}_{{\rm{it}}}={{\rm{\alpha }}}_{0}+{{\rm{\beta }}}_{1}{{\rm{I}}}_{{\rm{i}}}+{{\rm{\beta }}}_{2}{{\rm{T}}}_{{\rm{it}}}+{{\rm{\beta }}}_{3}\left({{\rm{I}}}_{{\rm{i}}}* {{\rm{T}}}_{{\rm{it}}}\right)+{{\rm{u}}}_{{\rm{s}}}+{{\rm{e}}}_{{\rm{t}}}+{{\rm{z}}}_{{\rm{i}}}$$
(1)
Here, Y
it was the continuous outcome for the
i-th observations (i = 1, 2, ….1410) at time t (t = 1, 2…5); I was treatment (I = 0 for control group; D = 1 for treatment group); T was time (T = 1 at baseline; T = 2 at 12 months; T = 3 at 24 months; T = 4 at 36 months; and T = 5 at 48 months); u
s was the level 1 error (i.e. differences between the expected and observed values of outcome at school level); e
t was the level 2 error (i.e. difference between the expected and observed values of outcome at time level); and z
i was the level 3 error (i.e. difference between the expected and observed values of outcome at individual level). Robust standard errors were adjusted for clustering within schools to estimate subject-specific effects accounting for school-level clustering (Hox et al.,
2017). Treatment effects were reported as time-within-group simple effect comparisons (i.e., treatment arm vs. control arm at each time point) obtained via multiple pairwise comparison analyses. We used Sidak’s adjustment method (Abdi,
2007), that mitigates the false discovery risk by offering p-value corrections for multiple comparisons.
To answer our second question (Does the effect of a family-based poverty reduction intervention on family functioning vary by child’s gender and orphanhood status?), we conducted two separate moderator analyses. These analyses examined whether the child’s gender and orphanhood status moderate (i.e., shape the direction and/or strength of) the intervention’s effect on family functioning. To assess these potential moderation effects, we ran two regression models, each incorporating a different three-way interaction term to the model described in Eq. (
1): the group-by-time-by-child’s gender interaction and the group-by-time-by-child’s orphanhood status interaction. This approach allowed us to independently assess the moderating effects of child’s gender and orphanhood status. We assessed the moderation effects through joint tests of these three-way interactions, which demonstrate the interaction between time and group on the slopes of gender and orphanhood status. Specifically, these tests assessed whether the intervention’s effect over time differed by gender (boys versus girls) and across different orphanhood categories. The orphanhood categories were double orphans (children who have lost both parents to AIDS), single paternal orphans (children who have lost their fathers to AIDS but have a living mother), and single maternal orphans (children who have lost their mothers to AIDS but have a living father). To further explore these three-way interactions and understand the intervention’s effect on family functioning within each group, we conducted simple slope analyses using the “margins” command in Stata 16. This method allowed us to examine the effect of the intervention within each subgroup defined by gender and orphanhood category without cross-group comparisons. In these simple slope analyses, at each time point, participants in the control group within the same subgroup served as the reference group for comparison. For example, within the “single paternal orphans” category, we compared participants in the treatment arm to those in the control arm at each time point, without comparing them to participants in other orphanhood categories. This approach provided a clearer understanding of how the intervention affected family functioning within each subgroup, highlighting the nuances of its impact based on gender and orphanhood status.
Results
Table
1 describes baseline characteristics of our sample.
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of the study sample
Family cohesion (range: 0–24) | 17.8 | [16.9; 18.7] | 17.3 | [16.8; 17.9] | 17.5 | [17; 18] | 0.83 |
Frequency of family communication (0–48) | 15.8 | [15; 16.5] | 15.9 | [15.1; 16.8] | 15.9 | [15.3; 26.5] | 0.08 |
Comfort of family communication (0–48) | 11.6 | [10.9; 12.2] | 11.3 | [10.8; 11.8] | 11.4 | [11; 11.8] | 0.32 |
Child-caregiver relationship (Range: 0–44) | 32.7 | [31.8; 33.6] | 32.1 | [31.5; 32.7] | 32.3 | [31.8; 32.8] | 1.27 |
Child’s gender | | | | | | | 0.63 |
Male | 44.96 | [40.4; 49.6] | 43.54 | [39.9; 47.2] | 44.04 | [41.2; 46.9] | |
Female | 55.04 | [50.4; 59.6] | 56.46 | 52.8; 60.1] | 55.96 | [53.1; 58.8] | |
Child’s orphanhood status | | | | | | | 2.24 |
Double orphan | 22.8 | [19.9; 26] | 17.8 | [14.9; 21.2] | 19.6 | [17.3; 22.1] | |
Single paternal orphan | 22.8 | [17.3; 29.3] | 21.3 | [18.5; 24.5] | 21.8 | [19.1; 24.9] | |
Single maternal orphan | 54.4 | [49.2; 59.6] | 60.8 | [57.5; 64.1] | 58.6 | [55.6; 61.5] | |
At baseline, the majority of children were identified as single maternal orphans, while approximately one-fifth were single paternal orphans, and another one-fifth were double orphans.
Results (Table
2) show significant positive effect of the intervention on family communication, including both the frequency of communication and the comfort of communication, in the short term.
Table 2
Effect of the poverty reduction intervention on family functioning, over the course of 48 months
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm |
at 12 months | 0.26 | [−0.5; 1] | 1.58* | [0.4; 2.8] | 1.07* | [0.2; 1.9] | 0.18 | [−0.8; 1.1] |
at 24 months | 0.62 | [−0.2; 1.4] | 1.71* | [0.3; 3.1] | 0.89 | [−0.3; 2.1] | 0.95 | [−0.2; 2.1] |
at 36 months | 0.37 | [−0.5; 1.2] | 0.42 | [−1.1; 1.9] | 0.17 | [−0.8; 1.1] | 0.53 | [−0.5; 1.6] |
at 48 months | 0.02 | [−0.6; 0.6] | 0.37 | [−1; 1.8] | 0.62 | [−0.2; 1.4] | 0.39 | [−0.4; 1.2] |
Observations | 6,429 | | 6,408 | | 6,407 | | 6,425 | |
Number of groups | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | |
More specifically, at 12 months, participants in the treatment group reported a higher frequency of communication and greater comfort in communicating with their caregivers compared to their control group counterparts. The significant positive effect of the intervention on communication frequency persisted at the 24-month follow-up but eventually faded away. The results indicate no significant effect of the intervention on either family cohesion or the child-caregiver relationship.
Moderator analyses and joint three-way interaction tests (Table
3) indicate that the intervention’s effect on family functioning was not significantly moderated by the child’s gender. However, simple slope analyses (Table
3) show a significant positive effect of the intervention on family communication, including both the frequency of communication and comfort in communicating, for boys in the short term. In particular, compared to their counterparts in the control group, boys in the treatment group reported higher frequency of communication at the 12-month and 24-month follow ups. They also reported greater comfort in communicating, but only at the 12-month follow-up. The seeming inconsistency between these findings can be attributed to the distinct methodological approaches of the two types of analyses. Moderator analyses and three-way interaction tests are designed to evaluate whether the effect of the intervention varies across distinct levels of the moderator variable—here, the child’s gender—across the entire sample. Such tests may not yield statistically significant results if the overall moderating effect is weak or displays variability across time points. Conversely, simple slope analyses are focused on examining specific subgroups and discrete time points, which allows for the detection of significant effects within these more narrowly defined contexts. This approach can reveal a positive intervention effect on communication for boys at particular follow-up intervals, even in cases where the overall moderation effect is not statistically significant.
Table 3
Effect of the poverty reduction intervention on family functioning by child’s gender
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm for Boys |
at 12 months | 0.16 | [−0.8; 1.1] | 1.52* | [0.2; 2.9] | 1.13* | [0.1; 2.2] | −0.02 | [−1.5; 1.4] |
at 24 months | −0.02 | [−1.1; 1] | 1.81* | [0.3; 3.3] | 0.80 | [−0.8; 2.4] | −0.13 | [−1.7; 1.5] |
at 36 months | 0.27 | [−0.8; 1.3] | 0.66 | [−1.3; 2.6] | 0.07 | [−1.5; 1.6] | 0.49 | [−1; 1.9] |
at 48 months | −0.08 | [−0.8; 0.7] | −0.43 | [−2.4; 1.5] | 1.31 | [−0.4; 3] | 0.51 | [−0.9; 1.9] |
Observations | 2,905 | | 2,892 | | 2,891 | | 2,903 | |
Number of groups | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | |
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm for Girls |
at 12 months | 0.35 | [−0.6; 1.3] | 1.51 | [−0.2; 3.2] | 0.96 | [−0.2; 2.1] | 0.41 | [−0.9; 1.7] |
at 24 months | 1.17* | [0.2; 2.1] | 1.50 | [−0.4; 3.4] | 0.87 | [−0.3; 2.1] | 1.92* | [0.4; 3.4] |
at 36 months | 0.48 | [−0.7; 1.6] | 0.13 | [−1.6; 1.8] | 0.19 | [−0.9; 1.3] | 0.63 | [−0.7; 2] |
at 48 months | 0.14 | [−0.7; 1] | 1.00 | [−0.8; 2.8] | 0.01 | [−0.8; 0.8] | 0.36 | [−0.9; 1.6] |
Observations | 3,524 | | 3,516 | | 3,516 | | 3,522 | |
Number of groups | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | |
Girls, on the other hand, experienced significant positive effects of the intervention on family cohesion and the child-caregiver relationship. More specifically, at the 24-month follow-up, girls in the treatment group reported greater family cohesion and better child-caregiver relationship than girls in the control group.
Moderator analyses and three-way interaction tests (Table
4) show that a child’s orphanhood status moderates only the intervention’s effect on the child-caregiver relationship. Simple slope analyses (Table
3) show a significant and lasting positive effect (at 24, 36, and 48 months) of the intervention on this outcome for paternal orphans only. We found no significant intervention effect on this outcome for other orphanhood categories. For single maternal orphans, the results show a significant positive effect of the intervention on family communication, including both the frequency of communication and comfort of communicating. Compared to their counterparts in the control group, single maternal orphans in the treatment group reported a higher frequency of communication at the 12-month and 24-month follow ups. They also reported greater comfort in communicating at 12, 24, 36, and 48 months following the baseline assessment.
Table 4
Effect of the poverty reduction intervention on family functioning by child’s orphanhood status
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm for Double Orphans |
at 12 months | −0.36 | [−1.4; 0.7] | 1.47 | [−0.5; 3.4] | 0.04 | [−1.5; 1.6] | −1.49 | [−3.5; 0.5] |
at 24 months | 0.38 | [−1.1; 1.9] | 0.96 | [−1.5; 3.4] | −0.41 | [−2.7; 1.9] | −0.30 | [−2.6; 2.0] |
at 36 months | −0.19 | [−1.8; 1.5] | −1.04 | [−3.4; 1.3] | −1.93* | [−3.7; −0.2] | −0.83 | [−3.1; 1.4] |
at 48 months | 0.46 | [−0.9; 1.8] | 0.44 | [−1.6; 2.5] | −0.50 | [−2.7; 1.7] | −0.20 | [−2.6; 2.2] |
Observations | 1,248 | | 1,243 | | 1,243 | | 1,247 | |
Number of groups | 47 | | 47 | | 47 | | 47 | |
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm for Single Paternal Orphans |
at 12 months | −0.35 | [−1.6; 0.9] | −0.06 | [−3; 2.9] | 0.48 | [−1.1; 2.1] | 0.81 | [−1.4; 3] |
at 24 months | 0.98 | [−0.5; 2.5] | 1.31 | [−1; 3.6] | 0.12 | [−1.7; 1.9] | 2.6* | [0.5; 4.7] |
at 36 months | 1.34 | [−0.2; 2.9] | 0.82 | [−1.2; 2.8] | −0.83 | [−2.9; 1.2] | 2.54* | [0.5; 4.5] |
at 48 months | 0.44 | [−0.9; 1.7] | −0.05 | [−2.9; 2.8] | 0.33 | [−1.3; 2.0] | 2.69* | [0.6; 4.8] |
Observations | 1,397 | | 1,394 | | 1,393 | | 1,395 | |
Number of groups | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | |
Treatment Arm vs. Control Arm for Single Maternal Orphans |
at 12 months | 0.73 | [−0.2; 1.7] | 2.13** | [0.7; 3.6] | 1.61** | [0.4; 2.8] | 0.35 | [−0.8; 1.5] |
at 24 months | 0.48 | [−0.6; 1.6] | 2.09* | [0.03; 4.2] | 1.74** | [0.5; 2.9] | 0.59 | [−1.0; 2.1] |
at 36 months | 0.14 | [−0.8; 1.1] | 0.71 | [−1.2; 2.6] | 1.39** | [0.4; 2.4] | 0.09 | [−1.1; 1.2] |
at 48 months | −0.29 | [−1.1; 0.5] | 0.46 | [−1.4; 2.3] | 1.22* | [0.2; 2.3] | −0.54 | [−1.7; 0.6] |
Observations | 3,784 | | 3,771 | | 3,771 | | 3,783 | |
Number of groups | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | | 48 | |
For double orphans, the results show no positive effect of the intervention on any of the family functioning outcomes. On the contrary, the results suggest that at 36 months, double orphans in the treatment group reported lower levels of communication comfort than their counterparts in the control group.
Discussion
Our research is among the few studies in the field that employ an experimental design to rigorously evaluate the effects of an asset-based poverty reduction intervention on family functioning. This contribution is significant, given the well-documented role of family functioning as a protective factor against adverse experiences and its importance as a predictor of positive health outcomes for vulnerable children and adolescents worldwide (WHO,
2016). A robust evaluation of poverty reduction interventions’ impact on family functioning is crucial, as these interventions have substantial potential to mitigate the long-term effects of poverty—such as economic stress and family dysfunction—on adverse childhood experiences and subsequent physical and mental health outcomes in adulthood (Chen et al.,
2017; Choi et al.,
2019).
Our findings present a nuanced perspective that partially aligns with the hypotheses proposed by asset theory (McKernan & Sherraden,
2008; Sherraden,
1991,
2016). We found significant intervention effects on family communication, including the frequency and comfort of communication. However, these effects were not sustained beyond the 24-month intervention period. The mentorship component, which included sessions focused on developing stronger communication skills with caregivers and family members, likely contributed to the observed improvements in family communication during the 24-month intervention period. Once the intervention was concluded, its significant positive effects on family communication dissipated. Furthermore, on average, we found no significant intervention effects on family cohesion or the child-caregiver relationship.
Poverty reduction interventions that enhance a family’s financial resources address only one of the many protective factors within a family system that contribute to the family’s resilience (Henry et al.,
2015). Other aspects of family functioning, such as family cohesion and the quality of child-caregiver relationships, might be more deeply rooted in the emotional and psychological structure of the family, requiring additional resilience-promoting interventions (Black & Lobo,
2008). Family strengthening interventions aimed at improving intra-family relationships and fostering a supportive family environment can effectively complement economic support initiatives. Recent evidence (Karimli et al.,
2023a) suggests that combining asset accumulation interventions with multifamily group (MFG) therapy, which focuses on therapeutic processes like child management techniques and emotional regulation, significantly enhances family cohesion and the quality of child-caregiver relationships. This aligns with earlier research (Ismayilova & Karimli,
2020) indicating that combination of poverty reduction intervention with family coaching centered around family communication on child protection issues can substantially improve child-caregiver relationships and parenting practices in low-income settings. These examples emphasize the importance of an integrated, comprehensive approach that combines economic assistance with psychosocial and behavioral interventions to address the multifaceted nature of family dynamics. Further research is essential to explore and refine integrated models for supporting low-income families and to establish a comprehensive framework for enhancing family functioning among economically disadvantaged families.
We found no evidence that the child’s gender moderated the effect of the intervention on family functioning. In contrast, the child’s orphanhood status significantly moderated the effect of the intervention on the child-caregiver relationship. Additional simple slope analyses, conducted for each category of the moderator variables (i.e., child’s gender and orphanhood status), revealed significant variations in intervention effects, suggesting potential nuances within specific subpopulations. It is important to interpret these subgroup differences cautiously and avoid overstating their significance, especially given the absence of a significant overall moderation effect. However, the findings are intriguing and offer valuable insights for further exploration. The emergence of significant results within certain subgroups, despite a non-significant three-way test of moderation, may be due to the increased sensitivity of simple slope analyses in detecting variations within specific categories. The three-way interaction test evaluates whether the relationship between the intervention and outcomes differs consistently across all combinations of the moderator variables. A non-significant result in this test does not rule out the possibility of significant effects within individual subgroups. Subgroup analyses can uncover differential intervention effects that are contextually dependent and may not be evident in broader interaction tests. Consequently, these findings, although preliminary, provide valuable insights into the complex interplay between intervention effects and specific characteristics, warranting further investigation into these nuanced dynamics.
More specifically, we found improved family communication (including the frequency and comfort of communication) for boys, and improved family cohesion and quality of the child-caregiver relationship for girls. Similarly, single maternal orphans (i.e., children who lost their mothers) in the treatment groups reported improved family communication, whereas single paternal orphans (i.e., children who lost their fathers) in the treatment group reported improved child-caregiver relationship. This finding aligns with previous studies that suggest significant gender variations in family functioning outcomes and processes (Colarossi & Eccles,
2003; Feldman et al.,
2018; Figlio et al.,
2019; Raley & Bianchi,
2006; Rueger et al.,
2008; Van Polanen et al.,
2017). Our results can be contextualized within the framework of gender schema theory (Bem,
1983; Starr & Zurbriggen,
2017), gender socialization theory (Leaper & Friedman,
2007), and the hypothesis that children develop closer relationships with the same-gender caregivers(Van Polanen et al.,
2017). Previous research (Karimli et al.,
2012) has highlighted the predominant role of female caregivers for children in our sample. Consequently, considering the proposition that girls might develop more secure attachment relationships with female caregivers and boys with male caregivers (Van Polanen et al.,
2017), it is possible that the intervention specifically improved the quality of relationships with primarily female caregivers and fostered a greater sense of family cohesion among girls, but not among boys. Moreover, gender-based socialization patterns can contribute to variations in how children perceive and respond to interventions, as well as the support they receive from their families (Endendijk et al.,
2016). In Uganda, traditional gender norms are deeply rooted in societal expectations and cultural practices. Women are often expected to fulfill nurturing roles within the household, focusing on caregiving and maintaining family cohesion, while men are typically associated with providing and less involved in emotional caregiving (Ninsiima et al.,
2018). This division of gender roles can influence how children are socialized from a young age, reinforcing the idea that emotional expression and communication are more acceptable for girls than boys (Nalukwago et al.,
2019; Vu et al.,
2017). These traditional gender norms and practices extend to parental communication styles with their children, as parents and caregivers often reinforce traditional gender stereotypes by showing a greater inclination to discuss emotions with daughters compared to sons (Aznar & Tenenbaum,
2015; Chaplin et al.,
2005; Fivush et al.,
2019). These gender-related dynamics may help understand the observed intervention effects, which showed improvement in family communication for boys, and in child-caregiver relationships and family cohesion for girls.
Limitations
This study has two key limitations to be considered when interpreting the findings. First, the absence of caregiver data limits our insight into family functioning, which is based solely on children’s accounts of family cohesion, the child-caregiver relationship, and communication within the family. The lack of caregiver-reported data, particularly regarding parenting stress, restricts our understanding of the caregiver’s perspective on these relationships. Furthermore, we have no data from various family members to assess the strengths and challenges of the family, as well as the communication patterns and capabilities that define the household’s dynamics. Without these broader measures and viewpoints, the study falls short of capturing the full complexity of the family environment.
Second, the study does not capture the nuances of intra-household dynamics, particularly the complexity of care arrangements for orphaned children. These children may experience various care structures offered by their extended families (Bryant & Beard,
2016; Heymann et al.,
2007; Karimli et al.,
2012). For instance, a maternal orphan—a child who lost their mother—might reside with their father but depend primarily on their grandmother for daily sustenance and emotional support. Our dataset does not specify the caregivers’ roles and contributions, leaving a gap in our comprehension of who provides critical forms of care such as emotional support, supervision, and basic needs. Without this detailed understanding, we cannot fully appreciate the intricate fabric of family functioning in these households, nor can we properly acknowledge the contributions of extended family members who may play pivotal roles in the lives of these children. Despite its limitations, our study is an important contribution to the robust examination of effects of poverty reduction interventions on family functioning, providing valuable insights from children’s perspectives.
Implications
Assessing the impact of poverty reduction interventions on family functioning is important, with economic strengthening serving as a means to the broader objective of emotional and social well-being of a family. When asset accumulation and poverty reduction alone is insufficient, additional resources need be invested in complementary programs to ensure more lasting and robust effects on family functioning. Our findings indicate that asset accumulation and economic strengthening alone may not substantially improve family functioning, emphasizing the need for a comprehensive, multifaceted approach to fostering family resilience. This may contribute to a shift in policy and intervention design towards an integrated approach, addressing both economic and psychosocial indicators of family functioning and family well-being.
Further research is needed to identify resilience-strengthening interventions that effectively complement poverty reduction and asset accumulation initiatives, while considering the diverse cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds of populations. Gathering comprehensive data from family members to fully capture the complexity of family functioning will enhance this field.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.