Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2016

22-10-2015 | S.I. : Discourse and conversation analytic approaches to the study of ASD

Parents as a Team: Mother, Father, a Child with Autism Spectrum Disorder, and a Spinning Toy

Auteurs: Douglas W. Maynard, T. A. McDonald, Trini Stickle

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 2/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This paper is a single case study involving a visit to a diagnostic clinic for autism spectrum disorder. A young boy finds a toy that he can hold with one hand and spin with another. In order to retrieve the toy and leave it in the clinic, the parents engage in a team effort. We describe this achievement in terms of two styles of practice or interactional routines with differing participation frameworks. We examine not only how the parents work as a team using these styles, but also how they improvise to extract the spinning toy from their son’s grasp with minimal protest on his part.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
For specific measures and findings, see Smith et al. (2012) regarding health symptoms and Seltzer et al. (2010) regarding cortisol levels deriving from dealing with adolescents and adults with disabilities including ASD. Also see research (Smith et al. 2010) that examines the effects of the family environment, especially “expressed emotion,” on ASD and its abatement as opposed to the escalation of behavior problems over time.
 
2
Literature on parental team work is sparse. In families with typically developing children, there are studies on non-overlapping shift work as a kind of “tag team” effort (Hattery 2001), co-parenting teams in the context of stepfamily households (Braithwaite et al. 2003), and team work involving parents with school staff (Comer and Haynes 1991). Regarding children with chronic health care problems, researchers (McNeill et al. 2014:1117–1118) refer to symmetry, whereby “parenting roles are shared and the mother and father work together as a ‘tag team,” with considerable overlap of what each partner does to look after their child” in day-to-day care. Other research (Bonsall 2014a), based on reviews of books by fathers of children with disabilities, turns up three methods whereby parents distribute responsibilities: cooperation, negotiation, and differentiation. As Bonsall (2014a:514) suggests in the context of discussing the role of fathers, research on what we call team work involving the parents or caretakers in the home as it occurs in real time is needed, a point to which we return in the Conclusion.
 
3
Both Goodwin (1990) and Goodwin (1997) have drawn on notion of the situated activity system. Goodwin (1990:8) proposes that the goal is to “formulate a unit of analysis which emphasizes the interactive meshing of the actions of separate participants into joint social projects.”
 
4
The study was funded by a seed grant from the University of Wisconsin Graduate School, and by a grant from the U.S. National Science Foundation, entitled “The Sociology of Testing and Diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders.”
 
5
For another single case analysis of family interactions involving a child with ASD, see Sterponi and Fasulo (2010). The child in their study is an older, more verbal child with ASD.
 
6
Figures that have no accompanying transcript will be labeled with letters (A, B, etc.), whereas figures that have an accompanying transcript will be numbered (1, 2, etc.) so that their labels correspond to the number of the transcript.
 
7
See Demuth (1986) on prompting routines in the language socialization of Basotho children.
 
8
The testing sequence is related to the well-known instructional sequence identified in the work of Sinclair and Coulthard (1975) and Mehan (1978, 1979), which is operative in classroom or other educational settings.
 
9
However, current work on the interactional dynamics of testing (Talkington, in progress) shows how formulations about “play” and “game” operate within and for official testing activities.
 
10
Unfortunately, the stacking toy is behind a chair, and Mother’s hand is not visible except faintly on the original frame (through the mesh on the back of the chair. The arrow here is meant to point to where the toy and mother’s hand are.
 
11
A device for more explicit elicitation is to direct the child with “Say X,” where X is the target word. See Demuth (1986:46–47) and extract Y below.
 
12
We follow the general use of the term “directive” to refer to task-related injunctions or utterances whereby one person asks or tells another to do some specific task. Drawing on Goodwin’s (1990) discussion, our approach is that the directive force of an utterance is dependent on the sequential and embodied context in which it occurs. Research in family contexts with typically developing children (e.g. Cekaite 2010; Craven and Potter 2010; Goodwin 2006), and research on children with ASD or other developmental challenges (e.g., Aldred et al. 2001; Bernard-Opitz 1982; Duchan 1983; Koegel and Mentis 1985; Mirenda and Donnellan 1986), inform our analyses.
 
13
In relation to Father’s mostly accommodative style, Joey can show spontaneously the sort of speech production that mother worked to elicit in a conditional way. For example, at one point fairly early in the session when the parents were answering questions from the pediatrician (00:13:58), Father observed Joey climb up on the mattress. He used a declarative yes–no inquiry, “You gonna come up?” that both notices and asks Joey about his activity. Joey confirms, “Up.” He thereby not only shows the competence required to participate in a question–answer sequence but also produces an unscripted and unconditioned instance of language use that the mother’s interactional style works more explicitly to induce through conditional kinds of sequences.
 
14
Couper-Kuhlen (2014) discusses “proposals” and “offers,” whose linguistic formatting relate to Mother’s and Father’s formulations here. Mother’s utterance is proposal-like, in that it implicates her in the activity, and Father’s utterance is offer-like in that it addresses only Joey’s want.
 
15
There is a substantial conversation analytic literature involving research on directive-response sequences in family contexts with typically developing children (e.g. Cekaite 2010; Craven and Potter 2010; Goodwin 2006) that is helpful for our study. A thorough review can be found in Craven and Potter (2010:419–422). Also see communication research on directives involving children with ASD or other developmental challenges (e.g., Aldred et al. 2001; Bernard-Opitz 1982; Duchan 1983; Koegel and Mentis 1985; Mirenda and Donnellan 1986).
 
16
See Schegloff (2007:30–31) for a discussion of blocking moves in relation to pre-sequence and sequence organization.
 
17
Although she has her back to the family as she types on a computer, Dr. Daley looks up and into the one-way mirror where she can see the family behind her, and smiles in a way suggesting she assumes they can see her reflected visage as well.
 
18
Depending on their context as much as their syntactic design (often having a “bald imperative” form), aggravated directives invoke some kind of stark social imposition (Goodwin 1990:67–70, 78–79).
 
19
In McCathren et al.’s (1995) terms, this would be a “re-directive,” which proposes a new topic or direction by attempting to distract a child from a current endeavor.
 
20
See Whalen et al. (2002: 241) on “improvisational choreography,” and how routines and sequences (along with technologies and artefacts) often afford “extemporaneous composition.” Also, with regard to parenting children with autism, see Solomon (2013:132), citing Daniel (1998), on the use of “fugitive” and “improvised parenting practices.”
 
21
See also Goffman’s (1974:99–103) discussion of both “paternal” constructions, which are “felt to be in the dupe’s best interests, but which he might reject… were he to discover what was really happening,” and “strategic” fabrications, which again are in the target’s best interests but do not morally contaminate the engineers. Accordingly, in Goffman’s framework, the mother’s and father’s efforts here are paternal (or we might say “parental”), strategic, and benign fabrications.
 
22
For example, the mother in the family studied by Sterponi and Fasulo’s (2010) seems to employ an accommodative style when responding to Aaron’s appendor questions.
 
23
For a notable exception, see the “ethnography of autism” project (Ochs and Solomon 2004).
 
24
See Bonsall’s (2014b:297) discussion of how members construct families through actions and participation, and how researchers can locate disabilities “on a map of family relations” and in “routines and interactions.”
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aldred, A., Pollard, C., Phillips, R., & Adams, C. (2001). Multidisciplinary social communication intervention for children with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Educational and Child Psychology, 18(2), 76–84. Aldred, A., Pollard, C., Phillips, R., & Adams, C. (2001). Multidisciplinary social communication intervention for children with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Educational and Child Psychology, 18(2), 76–84.
go back to reference Aronsson, K., & Cekaite, A. (2011). Activity contracts and directives in everyday family politics. Discourse and Society, 22, 1–18.CrossRef Aronsson, K., & Cekaite, A. (2011). Activity contracts and directives in everyday family politics. Discourse and Society, 22, 1–18.CrossRef
go back to reference Bernard-Opitz, V. (1982). Pragmatic analysis of the communicative behavior of an autistic child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 99–109.CrossRefPubMed Bernard-Opitz, V. (1982). Pragmatic analysis of the communicative behavior of an autistic child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 99–109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bonsall, A. (2014a). Fathering occupations: An analysis of narrative accounts of fathering children with special needs. Journal of Occupational Science, 21(4), 504–518.CrossRef Bonsall, A. (2014a). Fathering occupations: An analysis of narrative accounts of fathering children with special needs. Journal of Occupational Science, 21(4), 504–518.CrossRef
go back to reference Bonsall, A. (2014b). ‘This is what we do’: Constructing postmodern families through occupations. Journal of Occupational Science, 21(3), 296–308.CrossRef Bonsall, A. (2014b). ‘This is what we do’: Constructing postmodern families through occupations. Journal of Occupational Science, 21(3), 296–308.CrossRef
go back to reference Braithwaite, D. O., McBride, M. C., & Schrodt, P. (2003). ‘Parent teams’ and the everyday interactions of co‐parenting in stepfamilies. Communication Reports, 16(2), 93–111. Braithwaite, D. O., McBride, M. C., & Schrodt, P. (2003). ‘Parent teams’ and the everyday interactions of co‐parenting in stepfamilies. Communication Reports, 16(2), 93–111.
go back to reference Cekaite, A. (2010). Shepherding the child: Embodied directive sequences in parent–child interactions. Text & Talk, 30, 1–25.CrossRef Cekaite, A. (2010). Shepherding the child: Embodied directive sequences in parent–child interactions. Text & Talk, 30, 1–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Comer, J. P., & Haynes, N. M. (1991). Parent involvement in schools: An ecological approach. The Elementary School Journal, 91(3), 271–277.CrossRef Comer, J. P., & Haynes, N. M. (1991). Parent involvement in schools: An ecological approach. The Elementary School Journal, 91(3), 271–277.CrossRef
go back to reference Couper-Kuhlen, E. (2014). What does grammar tell us about action? Pragmatics, 24(3), 623–647.CrossRef Couper-Kuhlen, E. (2014). What does grammar tell us about action? Pragmatics, 24(3), 623–647.CrossRef
go back to reference Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Ford, C. E. (Eds.). (2004). Sound patterns in interaction. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Ford, C. E. (Eds.). (2004). Sound patterns in interaction. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
go back to reference Craven, A., & Potter, J. (2010). Directives: Entitlement and contingency in action. Discourse Studies, 12(4), 419–442.CrossRef Craven, A., & Potter, J. (2010). Directives: Entitlement and contingency in action. Discourse Studies, 12(4), 419–442.CrossRef
go back to reference Daniel, E. V. (1998). The limits of culture. In N. B. Dirks (Ed.), In near ruins (pp. 67–91). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Daniel, E. V. (1998). The limits of culture. In N. B. Dirks (Ed.), In near ruins (pp. 67–91). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
go back to reference Demuth, K. (1986). Prompting routines in the language socialization of basotho children. In B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 51–79). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Demuth, K. (1986). Prompting routines in the language socialization of basotho children. In B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 51–79). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Dolev, S., Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., & Yirmiya, N. (2009). Emotional availability in mother–child interaction: The case of children with autism spectrum disorder. Parenting: Science and Practice, 9(3–4), 183–197. doi:10.1080/15295190902844332.CrossRef Dolev, S., Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., & Yirmiya, N. (2009). Emotional availability in mother–child interaction: The case of children with autism spectrum disorder. Parenting: Science and Practice, 9(3–4), 183–197. doi:10.​1080/​1529519090284433​2.CrossRef
go back to reference Duchan, J. F. (1983). Autistic children are noninteractive: Or so we say. Seminars in Speech and Language, 4, 53–61. Duchan, J. F. (1983). Autistic children are noninteractive: Or so we say. Seminars in Speech and Language, 4, 53–61.
go back to reference Elder, J. H., Donaldson, S. O., Kairalla, J., Valcante, G., Bendixen, R., Ferdig, R., & Serrano, M. (2011). In-home training for fathers of children with autism: A follow up study and evaluation of four individual training components. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20(3), 263–271. doi:10.10077/s10826-010-9387-2.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Elder, J. H., Donaldson, S. O., Kairalla, J., Valcante, G., Bendixen, R., Ferdig, R., & Serrano, M. (2011). In-home training for fathers of children with autism: A follow up study and evaluation of four individual training components. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20(3), 263–271. doi:10.​10077/​s10826-010-9387-2.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ellis Weismer, S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Stronach, S., Karasinski, C., Eernisse, E. R., Venker, C. E., & Sindberg, H. (2010a). Lexical and grammatical skills in toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to late talking toddlers. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 41(8), 1065–1075.CrossRef Ellis Weismer, S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Stronach, S., Karasinski, C., Eernisse, E. R., Venker, C. E., & Sindberg, H. (2010a). Lexical and grammatical skills in toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to late talking toddlers. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 41(8), 1065–1075.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellis Weismer, S., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010b). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 40, 1259–1273.CrossRef Ellis Weismer, S., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010b). Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 40, 1259–1273.CrossRef
go back to reference Ford, C. E. (2004). Conversation and phonetics: Essential connections. In E. Couper-Kuhlen & C. E. Ford (Eds.), Sound patterns in interaction (pp. 3–25). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRef Ford, C. E. (2004). Conversation and phonetics: Essential connections. In E. Couper-Kuhlen & C. E. Ford (Eds.), Sound patterns in interaction (pp. 3–25). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRef
go back to reference Geils, C., & Knoetze, J. (2008). Conversations with Barney: A conversation analysis of interactions with a child with autism. South African Journal of Psychology, 38(1), 200–224.CrossRef Geils, C., & Knoetze, J. (2008). Conversations with Barney: A conversation analysis of interactions with a child with autism. South African Journal of Psychology, 38(1), 200–224.CrossRef
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1961). Encounters. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill. Goffman, E. (1961). Encounters. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. New York: Harper and Row. Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. New York: Harper and Row.
go back to reference Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order. American Sociological Review, 48, 1–17.CrossRef Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order. American Sociological Review, 48, 1–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633.CrossRef Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C. (1995). Co-constructing meaning in conversations with an aphasic man. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28, 233–260.CrossRef Goodwin, C. (1995). Co-constructing meaning in conversations with an aphasic man. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28, 233–260.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C. (1997). The blackness of black: Color categories as situated practice. In L. B. Resnick, R. Säljö, C. Pontecorvo, & B. Burge (Eds.), Discourse, tools and reasoning: Essays on situated cognition (pp. 111–140). New York: Springer.CrossRef Goodwin, C. (1997). The blackness of black: Color categories as situated practice. In L. B. Resnick, R. Säljö, C. Pontecorvo, & B. Burge (Eds.), Discourse, tools and reasoning: Essays on situated cognition (pp. 111–140). New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1489–1522.CrossRef Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1489–1522.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C. (2007). Participation, stance, and affect in the organization of activities. Discourse & Society, 18, 53–73.CrossRef Goodwin, C. (2007). Participation, stance, and affect in the organization of activities. Discourse & Society, 18, 53–73.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (1992). Assessments and the construction of context. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 147–190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (1992). Assessments and the construction of context. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 147–190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
go back to reference Goodwin, M. H. (2006). Participation, affect, and trajectory in family directive/response sequences. Text & Talk, 26, 515–543.CrossRef Goodwin, M. H. (2006). Participation, affect, and trajectory in family directive/response sequences. Text & Talk, 26, 515–543.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, M. H., & Cekaite, A. (2014). Orchestrating directive trajectories in communicative projects in family interaction. In P. Drew & E. Couper-Kuhlen (Eds.), Requesting in social interaction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Goodwin, M. H., & Cekaite, A. (2014). Orchestrating directive trajectories in communicative projects in family interaction. In P. Drew & E. Couper-Kuhlen (Eds.), Requesting in social interaction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
go back to reference Grandin, T. (1984). My experiences as an autistic child and review of selected literature. Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 13, 144–175. Grandin, T. (1984). My experiences as an autistic child and review of selected literature. Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 13, 144–175.
go back to reference Grinker, R. R. (2007). Unstrange minds: Remapping the world of autism. New York: Basic Books. Grinker, R. R. (2007). Unstrange minds: Remapping the world of autism. New York: Basic Books.
go back to reference Hattery, A. J. (2001). Tag-team parenting, costs and benefits of utilizing nonoverlapping shift work in families with young children. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Social Services, 82(4), 419–427.CrossRef Hattery, A. J. (2001). Tag-team parenting, costs and benefits of utilizing nonoverlapping shift work in families with young children. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Social Services, 82(4), 419–427.CrossRef
go back to reference Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., Krauss, M. W., Sayer, A., & Upshur, C. C. (2001). Children with disabilities: A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(3), 1–126.CrossRef Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., Krauss, M. W., Sayer, A., & Upshur, C. C. (2001). Children with disabilities: A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(3), 1–126.CrossRef
go back to reference Heritage, J. (1984). A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 299–345). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Heritage, J. (1984). A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 299–345). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Hirstein, W., Iversen, P., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2001). Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 268, 1883–1888.CrossRef Hirstein, W., Iversen, P., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2001). Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 268, 1883–1888.CrossRef
go back to reference Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
go back to reference Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference King, G. A., Zwaigenbaum, L., King, S., Baxter, D., Rosenbaum, P., & Bates, A. (2006). A qualitative investigation of changes in the belief systems of families of children with autism or down syndrome. Child: Care, Health and Development, 32(3), 353–369. King, G. A., Zwaigenbaum, L., King, S., Baxter, D., Rosenbaum, P., & Bates, A. (2006). A qualitative investigation of changes in the belief systems of families of children with autism or down syndrome. Child: Care, Health and Development, 32(3), 353–369.
go back to reference Koegel, R. L., & Mentis, M. (1985). Motivation in childhood autism: Can they or won’t they? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 185–191.CrossRefPubMed Koegel, R. L., & Mentis, M. (1985). Motivation in childhood autism: Can they or won’t they? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 185–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Marlaire, C. L., & Maynard, D. W. (1990). Standardized testing as an interactional phenomenon. Sociology of Education, 63, 83–101.CrossRef Marlaire, C. L., & Maynard, D. W. (1990). Standardized testing as an interactional phenomenon. Sociology of Education, 63, 83–101.CrossRef
go back to reference Maynard, D. W., & Marlaire, C. L. (1992). Good reasons for bad testing performance: The interactional substrate of educational exams. Qualitative Sociology, 15, 177–202.CrossRef Maynard, D. W., & Marlaire, C. L. (1992). Good reasons for bad testing performance: The interactional substrate of educational exams. Qualitative Sociology, 15, 177–202.CrossRef
go back to reference McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1995). The role of directives in early language intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 19(2), 91–101.CrossRef McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1995). The role of directives in early language intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 19(2), 91–101.CrossRef
go back to reference McNeill, T., Nicholas, D., Beaton, J., Montgomery, G., MacCulloch, R., Gearing, R., & Selkirk, E. (2014). The coconstruction of couples’ roles in parenting children with a chronic health condition. Qualitative Health Research, 24(8), 1114–1125.CrossRefPubMed McNeill, T., Nicholas, D., Beaton, J., Montgomery, G., MacCulloch, R., Gearing, R., & Selkirk, E. (2014). The coconstruction of couples’ roles in parenting children with a chronic health condition. Qualitative Health Research, 24(8), 1114–1125.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mehan, H. (1978). Structuring school structure. Harvard Educational Review, 48, 32–64.CrossRef Mehan, H. (1978). Structuring school structure. Harvard Educational Review, 48, 32–64.CrossRef
go back to reference Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Mirenda, P. L., & Donnellan, A. M. (1986). Effects of adult interaction style on conversational behavior in students with severe communication problems. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 126–141.CrossRef Mirenda, P. L., & Donnellan, A. M. (1986). Effects of adult interaction style on conversational behavior in students with severe communication problems. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 126–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmenal Disorders, 20(1), 115–128. doi:10.1007/BF02206861.CrossRef Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmenal Disorders, 20(1), 115–128. doi:10.​1007/​BF02206861.CrossRef
go back to reference Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., Sirota, K. G., & Solomon, O. (2004a). Autism and the social world: An anthropological perspective. Discourse Studies, 6, 147–183.CrossRef Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., Sirota, K. G., & Solomon, O. (2004a). Autism and the social world: An anthropological perspective. Discourse Studies, 6, 147–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Ochs, E., Sirota, K. G., & Solomon, O. (2004b). Autism and the social world: An anthropological perspective. Discourse Studies, 6(2), 399–419. Ochs, E., Sirota, K. G., & Solomon, O. (2004b). Autism and the social world: An anthropological perspective. Discourse Studies, 6(2), 399–419.
go back to reference Ochs, E., & Solomon, O. (2004). Introduction: Discourse and autism. Discourse Studies, 6, 139–146.CrossRef Ochs, E., & Solomon, O. (2004). Introduction: Discourse and autism. Discourse Studies, 6, 139–146.CrossRef
go back to reference Ochs, E., & Solomon, O. (2010). Autistic sociality. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 38, 69–92. Ochs, E., & Solomon, O. (2010). Autistic sociality. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 38, 69–92.
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism, 12(5), 457–472.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism, 12(5), 457–472.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Park, C. C. (1967). The siege: The first eight years of an autistic child. Boston: Little Brown. Park, C. C. (1967). The siege: The first eight years of an autistic child. Boston: Little Brown.
go back to reference Park, C. C. (2001). Exiting nirvana. Boston: Little, Brown & Co. Park, C. C. (2001). Exiting nirvana. Boston: Little, Brown & Co.
go back to reference Pelchat, D., Levert, M.-J., & Bourgeois-Guérin, V. (2009). How do mothers and fathers who have a child with a disability describe their adaptation/transformation process? Journal of Child Health Care, 13(3), 239–259.CrossRefPubMed Pelchat, D., Levert, M.-J., & Bourgeois-Guérin, V. (2009). How do mothers and fathers who have a child with a disability describe their adaptation/transformation process? Journal of Child Health Care, 13(3), 239–259.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Peters, A. M., & Boggs, S. T. (1986). Interactional routines as cultural influences upon language acquisition. In B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 80–96). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Peters, A. M., & Boggs, S. T. (1986). Interactional routines as cultural influences upon language acquisition. In B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 80–96). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Ray-Subramanian, C. E., Huai, N., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2011). Brief report: Adaptive behavior and cognitive skills for toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 41(5), 679–684. Ray-Subramanian, C. E., Huai, N., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2011). Brief report: Adaptive behavior and cognitive skills for toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 41(5), 679–684.
go back to reference Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735. Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.
go back to reference Schegloff, E. A. (1987). Analyzing single episodes of interaction: An exercise in conversation analysis. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50(2), 101–114.CrossRef Schegloff, E. A. (1987). Analyzing single episodes of interaction: An exercise in conversation analysis. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50(2), 101–114.CrossRef
go back to reference Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Hong, J., Smith, L. E., Almeida, D. M., Coe, C., & Stawski, R. S. (2010). Maternal cortisol levels and behavior problems in adolescents and adults with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 457–469.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Hong, J., Smith, L. E., Almeida, D. M., Coe, C., & Stawski, R. S. (2010). Maternal cortisol levels and behavior problems in adolescents and adults with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 457–469.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Orsmond, G. I., & Vestal, C. (2001). Families of adolescents and adults with autism: Uncharted territory. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 267–294.CrossRef Seltzer, M. M., Krauss, M. W., Orsmond, G. I., & Vestal, C. (2001). Families of adolescents and adults with autism: Uncharted territory. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 267–294.CrossRef
go back to reference Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), v-114. doi:10.1111/1540-5834.00001.CrossRef Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), v-114. doi:10.​1111/​1540-5834.​00001.CrossRef
go back to reference Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691–1704. doi:10.1037/a0013771.CrossRefPubMed Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691–1704. doi:10.​1037/​a0013771.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sinclair, J. M., & Couthard, R. M. (1975). Toward an analysis of discourse: English used by teachers and pupils. New York: Oxford University Press. Sinclair, J. M., & Couthard, R. M. (1975). Toward an analysis of discourse: English used by teachers and pupils. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Smith, L. E., Hong, J., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Almeida, D. M., & Bishop, S. L. (2010). Daily experiences among mothers of adolescents and adults with asd. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 167–178.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Smith, L. E., Hong, J., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Almeida, D. M., & Bishop, S. L. (2010). Daily experiences among mothers of adolescents and adults with asd. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 167–178.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smith, L. E., Seltzer, M. M., & Greenberg, J. S. (2012). Daily health symptoms of mothers of adolescents and adults with fragile × syndrome and mothers of adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1836–1846.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Smith, L. E., Seltzer, M. M., & Greenberg, J. S. (2012). Daily health symptoms of mothers of adolescents and adults with fragile × syndrome and mothers of adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1836–1846.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Solomon, O. (2013). Autism and affordances of achievement: Narrative genres and parenting practices. In N. Long & H. Moore (Eds.), The social life of achievement (pp. 120–138). Oxford, England: Berghahn. Solomon, O. (2013). Autism and affordances of achievement: Narrative genres and parenting practices. In N. Long & H. Moore (Eds.), The social life of achievement (pp. 120–138). Oxford, England: Berghahn.
go back to reference Sterponi, L., & Fasulo, A. (2010). “How to go on”: Intersubjectivity and progressivity in the communication of a child with autism. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 38, 116–142. Sterponi, L., & Fasulo, A. (2010). “How to go on”: Intersubjectivity and progressivity in the communication of a child with autism. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 38, 116–142.
go back to reference Stevanovic, M., & Peräkylä, A. (2012). Deontic authority in interaction: The right to announce, propose, and decide. Research on Language and Social Intgeraction, 45(3), 297–321.CrossRef Stevanovic, M., & Peräkylä, A. (2012). Deontic authority in interaction: The right to announce, propose, and decide. Research on Language and Social Intgeraction, 45(3), 297–321.CrossRef
go back to reference Suskind, R. (2014). Life animated: A story of sidekicks, heroes, and autism. New York: Kingswell. Suskind, R. (2014). Life animated: A story of sidekicks, heroes, and autism. New York: Kingswell.
go back to reference Talkington, A. (in progress). Inviting participation in testing by way of ‘play’ or ‘game’ formulations. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison. Talkington, A. (in progress). Inviting participation in testing by way of ‘play’ or ‘game’ formulations. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
go back to reference Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: Comparison with children with mental retardation with language delays, and with typical development. Child Development, 78(2), 597–608.CrossRefPubMed Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: Comparison with children with mental retardation with language delays, and with typical development. Child Development, 78(2), 597–608.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Whalen, J., Whalen, M., & Henderson, K. (2002). Improvisational choreography in teleservice work. British Journal of Sociology, 53(2), 239–258.CrossRefPubMed Whalen, J., Whalen, M., & Henderson, K. (2002). Improvisational choreography in teleservice work. British Journal of Sociology, 53(2), 239–258.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Parents as a Team: Mother, Father, a Child with Autism Spectrum Disorder, and a Spinning Toy
Auteurs
Douglas W. Maynard
T. A. McDonald
Trini Stickle
Publicatiedatum
22-10-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 2/2016
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2568-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2016

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2016 Naar de uitgave