Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies 4/2022

Open Access 07-07-2021 | Original Paper

Parenting and Adolescents’ Academic Achievement: The Mediating Role of Goal Engagement and Disengagement

Auteurs: Priscilla S. Yau, Yongwon Cho, Jacob Shane, Joseph Kay, Jutta Heckhausen

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies | Uitgave 4/2022

Abstract

Prior research has investigated parental behavior and students’ motivation separately as predictors of adolescents’ academic achievement. The current study jointly examined the associations between parental behavior, adolescents’ motivation, and academic achievement. Using data collected from participants in youth programs (N = 220), we investigated whether students’ academic goal engagement and disengagement mediates the association between adolescents’ relationships with their parents and their academic achievement. Findings from regression-based mediation models indicated that adolescents’ perceived maternal support was positively associated with their academic achievement, and that this association was mediated by students’ engagement with academic goals. Perceived maternal psychological control was negatively associated with students’ academic success, mediated by students’ academic goal disengagement. Supplementary analyses examining components of perceived parental support and psychological control showed that maternal warmth may be most beneficial for adolescents’ academic success, whereas maternal devaluation may be most detrimental. Perceived paternal psychological control was only associated with lower academic achievement, but was not related to adolescents’ goal disengagement.
Opmerkingen
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Research has widely shown that parents play an important role in adolescents’ academic performance (Froiland & Worrell, 2017; Pinquart, 2016; Pinquart & Ebling, 2020; Wang et al., 2018), yet less is known about the mechanisms through which this occurs. The purpose of our study is to examine: (1) which parenting characteristics are related to adolescents’ academic achievement, and (2) whether adolescents’ motivational academic goal engagement and disengagement mediate the association between parenting and academic achievement.

Positive Parenting and Academic Success

Parents’ expectations, beliefs, and behaviors are strongly associated with students’ academic achievement (Abar et al., 2009; Boonk et al., 2018; Dornbusch et al., 1987; Englund et al., 2004; Gordon & Cui, 2012; Hill et al., 2004; Roksa & Potter, 2011). More specifically, prior research has found that parenting practices with supportive characteristics such as being highly responsive and setting up clear rules are the most effective in facilitating children’s academic achievement (Aunola et al., 2000; Baumrind, 1966; Brown & Iyengar, 2008; Woolley et al., 2009). Many studies also have shown that supportive parenting characteristics, including warmth (i.e., expressing love towards the child), involvement (i.e., spending time with the child), and autonomy-support (i.e., guiding the child towards independence), are positively linked to academic success (Pinquart, 2016; Gonida & Cortina, 2014; Froiland & Worrell, 2017; Henry & Plunkett et al., 2011; Topor et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2014; Wang & Sheikh‐Khalil, 2014). Combining these findings, Pinquart (2016) conducted a meta-analysis and showed wide-spread evidence for significant associations between supportive parenting characteristics and adolescents’ academic achievement. Our study will focus on the combination of parental warmth, involvement, and autonomy-support, referred to as parental support. Building on the evidence linking parental support with academic achievement, we examine motivational mechanisms as potential mediators between supportive parenting and children’s academic achievement.

Negative Parenting and Academic Difficulties

In addition to positive supportive parenting, negative parenting characteristics are also associated with academic and developmental outcomes in children and adolescents (e.g., Garcia & Serra, 2019; Sangawi et al., 2018). Negative parenting is not defined merely as an absence of parental support, but rather as different parenting strategies that have potentially detrimental consequences for adolescents (e.g., Xiang et al., 2017). One of the potentially harmful strategies is parental psychological control, which refers to parents’ attempts to manipulate and control their children’s thoughts and behavior (Barber, 1996). Constructs of parental psychological control consist of multiple components, including devaluation (i.e., invalidating the child’s thoughts and expressions) and guilt induction (i.e., causing the child to feel guilty) (Tian et al., 2019). Some parents use psychological control so children will remain emotionally dependent on them (Symeou & Georgiou, 2017).
In the context of academic achievement, psychological control has been found to be negatively associated with academic success (e.g., Aunola & Nurmi, 2004; Pinquart, 2016). Psychological control can be especially harmful for adolescents because as they approach the transition into adulthood they are in the process of developing their self-concept of competence and identity, consequently feeling more vulnerable to social influences that may undermine their sense of autonomy and self-worth (Symeou & Georgiou, 2017). For example, Lu et al. (2017) found that adolescents who experienced strong parental attempts for psychological control had low academic self-concept. Numerous studies have also shown a negative association between parental psychological control and adolescents’ academic self-efficacy and academic performance (Desjardins & Leadbeater, 2017; Xu et al., 2020). Building on this research that has revealed associations between parental psychological control and adolescents’ academic outcomes, we will further investigate the mechanisms through which this effect occurs.

Goal Engagement, Goal Disengagement, and Academic Achievement

One of the crucial processes that are involved in adolescents’ educational pursuit is goal engagement (Hall et al., 2006; Hamm et al., 2013). According to the motivational theory of lifespan development (Heckhausen et al., 2010), individuals actively contribute to their development through the coordination of both motivational engagement and disengagement. Goal engagement is defined as the behavioral strategy of investing time and effort into achieving a goal and the use of cognitive strategies of intentional goal commitment that includes avoiding distraction, enhancing perceptions of control, and imagining positive affect with goal attainment. By enhancing and maintaining their goal commitment, individuals are able to persist in their pursuit of desired goals, such as outstanding achievement in school (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). Investing time and effort into schoolwork is one of the main factors that determine academic success (Hall et al., 2006), and internally maintaining goal focus, especially under challenging conditions (Hamm et al., 2013) or severely negative life events (Poulin & Heckhausen, 2007), facilitates the behavioral strategies needed to achieve in school.
Conversely, when individuals feel they are not in control of their goal attainment or may not be able to attain their goals, they may disengage from pursuing them (Heckhausen et al., 2010). While goal disengagement may be a beneficial strategy in circumstances in which the goal is truly unattainable (Rothermund & Brandstädter, 2003; Wrosch & Scheier, 2020), individuals experience detrimental effects when they disengage from goals pre-maturely. When individuals disengage from goals too early, they are not investing substantial effort, persisting in the face of setbacks, or remaining optimistic about attainability of the goal (Freund & Baltes, 1998; Komarraju & Nadler, 2013; Monzani et al., 2015). This may be particularly damaging for the pursuit of academic goals during high school, as high school academic achievement strongly predicts postsecondary educational attainment (Galla et al., 2019). To promote goal engagement and prevent adolescents from pre-maturely disengaging from their academic goals, we aim to understand factors that facilitate adolescents’ academic goal engagement and disengagement.

Goal Engagement and Disengagement as Mediators Between Parenting and Academic Achievement

Prior studies have examined the indirect relation between parenting and academic achievement through a few motivational factors as mediators (e.g., Guay et al., 2013; Pinquart & Ebling, 2020). For example, Lowe and Dotterer (2013) found that parental warmth was positively associated with adolescents’ academic motivation, resulting in better academic outcomes. Other motivational factors such as students’ perception of control, academic self-concept, and intrinsic motivation have been shown to mediate the relation between parenting and academic achievement (Fulton & Turner, 2008; Guay et al., 2013; Pinquart & Ebeling, 2020). On the other hand, adolescents with controlling parents are less likely to maintain cognitive engagement with academic goals, resulting in lower academic achievement (Markazi & Badrigargari, 2011). Despite the findings, no known studies have assessed the association between parenting characteristics and adolescents’ academic goal engagement and disengagement, which are crucial for academic outcomes. Hence, our study aims to fill this gap by examining how adolescents’ use of motivational academic engagement and disengagement may explain the mechanism through which parenting and academic achievement are associated.

Differential Effects of Mothers and Fathers

While numerous studies have assessed parenting by combining the effects of mothers and fathers (e.g., Gonida & Cortina, 2014; Froiland & Worrell, 2017, Wang et al., 2014), other studies have highlighted differences between mothers’ and fathers’ influence on their children’s academic outcomes through examining the effects of each parent separately (e.g., Henry et al., 2011; Hill, 2015; Hsu et al., 2011; Lowe & Dotterer, 2013). For example, Hsu et al. (2011) found that among Taiwanese parents, mothers had greater involvement in adolescents’ education than fathers. In addition, mothers’ involvement was a stronger predictor of academic achievement than fathers’ involvement. Henry et al. (2011) found that among Latinx adolescents, their perception of mothers’ educational aspirations predicted their GPA, but fathers’ educational aspirations did not. On the other hand, meta-analyses have found a positive association between fathers’ involvement and various developmental outcomes (Jeynes, 2015), and have emphasized the need for studies to identify the distinct ways in which mothers and fathers contribute to their children’s academic achievement (Hill, 2015). To further investigate the role played by both mothers and fathers in adolescents’ educational pursuits, the present study examined parenting dimensions separately for mothers and fathers.

Current Study

Many studies have explored direct and indirect associations between parenting, adolescents’ motivation, and academic achievement (e.g., Lowe & Dotterer, 2013; Pinquart, 2016; Wang & Sheikh‐Khalil, 2014). However, studies have yet to examine whether adolescents’ academic goal engagement and disengagement may mediate the association between parenting and adolescents’ academic achievement; our study aims to do so. Based on prior research, we expect that adolescents’ academic goal engagement will positively mediate the relation between parental support and adolescents’ academic achievement for both mothers (Hypothesis 1a) and fathers (Hypothesis 1b). In contrast, adolescents’ academic goal disengagement is expected to mediate the negative relation between parental psychological control and adolescents’ academic achievement for both mothers (Hypothesis 2a) and fathers (Hypothesis 2b). To determine which parenting characteristic is driving the association between parenting and academic achievement via adolescents’ goal engagement and goal disengagement, sub-factors of parental support and psychological control will also be examined.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Data was collected from 244 high school students in grades 9–12 at four different afterschool programs and one in-school program in Southern California. A total of 220 participants were retained in the analyses after accounting for missing data and using multiple imputation for all variables of interest. The remaining missing data was from demographic variables, and 24 participants who did not have complete data on demographics were excluded from the study. Little’s MCAR test (Little, 1988) showed that data was missing completely at random (χ2 (100) = 102.06, p = 0.424), and multiple imputation was conducted to infer the missing values from existing observations in non-categorical variables (Li et al., 2015). Results did not change when analyses were run using the imputed data. Missing values occurred most frequently for paternal psychological control (11.1%), paternal support (9.4%), and parental education (9.8%). As 17% of participants lived with their mothers only, this may have contributed to a lack of responses for variables relating to fathers. However, results regarding paternal support did not change when analyses were conducted by excluding participants who did not live with their fathers. Participant demographics are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptives for parenting and demographic variables
 
N
Minimum
Maximum
Mean
Std. Deviation
Mothers’ Support
220
1.04
5.00
3.84
0.82432
Fathers’ Support
220
1.22
5.00
3.49
0.88054
Mothers’ Psychological Control
220
1.00
3.00
1.75
0.55573
Fathers’ Psychological Control
220
1.00
3.00
1.70
0.53541
Academic Achievement
220
1.00
6.00
4.24
1.5163
Goal Engagement
220
1.00
5.00
4.09
0.59830
Goal Disengagement
220
1.00
5.00
2.92
0.86312
(Covariates)
Grade
 Freshmen
65
    
 Sophomore
75
    
 Junior
34
    
 Senior
46
    
Gender
 Male
84
    
 Female
136
    
Ethnicity
 Whites
24
    
 Latinx
173
    
 Others
23
    
Parental Education
 Less than high school
78
    
 High school completion
70
    
 More than high school
72
    
All data was collected at one time point. Students and their parents received study information sheets, and those who were interested in participating completed the survey after the parent signed an informed consent form and the adolescent signed an assent form. The survey took approximately 45 minutes to complete, and participants were compensated with a $10 Amazon or Starbucks gift cards upon completion. All procedures were approved by the University of California, Irvine Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Parental Support

The Perception of Parenting-College Student Scale (POP-CSS) was used to measure perceived parental support (Niemiec et al., 2006). The POP-CSS was used instead of the original Perception of Parenting Scale because the latter scale has been mainly used and validated for children, even those as young as 8-years old (Grolnick et al., 1991). In noticing the similarity of items between the two scales, we used the one adapted for college students because it directly asks about their perceived relationship with their parents and the phrasing of the items is more specific. For example, one item in the original POP scale states, “some mothers always have enough time to talk to their children,” while the item adapted for college students states, “my mother seems to know how I feel about things.” Nevertheless, the use of the college scale will be mentioned in the Limitations section. The POP-CSS is a 42-item measure (21 items for mothers and 21 items for fathers), with a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all true” to “very true.” A 6-item subscale was used to measure parental warmth, with examples including that the mother or father “is typically happy to see me” or “accepts me and likes me as I am.” The internal consistency for both mothers’ warmth (α = 0.86) and fathers’ warmth (α = 0.85) was good. To measure parental involvement, a 6-item subscale was also used, with sample items including that the parent “puts time and energy into helping me” or “spends a lot of time with me.” Internal consistency for mothers’ involvement (α = 0.77) and fathers’ involvement (α = 0.75) was acceptable. To measure parental autonomy support, a 9-item subscale was used. A sample item is that the parent “helps me to choose my own direction,” and internal consistency for mothers’ (α = 0.85) and fathers’ autonomy support (α = 0.83) was also good. However, due to the high collinearity among the subscales (see Table 2), we combined the subscales for our main analyses and labeled them as “maternal support” and “paternal support”. When all three subscales were combined, the internal consistency was excellent for both maternal support (α = 0.92) and paternal support (α = 0.92). We also examined the contribution of each subscale on the outcome variables in the exploratory analysis section.
Table 2
Correlation matrix for all predictors, mediators, and outcome variables
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
1. Mothers’ Warmth
1.00
          
 
2. Fathers’ Warmth
0.44***
1.00
           
3. Mothers’ Involvement
0.77***
0.32***
1.00
          
4. Fathers’ Involvement
0.26***
0.74***
0.38***
1.00
         
5. Mothers’ Autonomy Support
0.80***
0.33***
0.70***
0.25***
1.00
        
6. Fathers’ Autonomy Support
0.35***
0.79***
0.26***
0.74***
0.39***
1.00
       
7. Mothers’ Devaluation
−0.62***
−0.28***
−0.54***
−0.26***
−0.69***
−0.32***
1.0
      
8. Fathers’ Devaluation
−0.35***
−0.54***
−0.32***
−0.43***
−0.30***
−0.54***
0.47***
1.00
     
9. Mothers’ Guilt Induction
−0.66***
−0.26***
−0.60***
−0.21***
−0.67***
−0.33***
0.62****
0.30***
1.00
    
10. Fathers’ Guilt Induction
−0.28***
−0.46***
−0.31***
−0.41***
−0.29***
−0.47***
0.29***
0.55***
0.51***
1.00
   
11. Goal Engagement
0.32***
0.22**
0.16*
0.14*
0.24***
0.18**
−0.20**
−0.07
−0.17*
−0.11
1.00
  
12. Goal Disengagement
−0.11
0.08
−0.09
0.04
−0.02
0.09
0.16*
0.10
0.08
−0.01
−0.07
1.00
 
13. Academic Achievement
0.22**
0.11
−0.09
0.09
0.14*
0.07
0-.18**
−0.11
−0.07***
−0.19**
0.35
−0.24***
1.00
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Parental Psychological Control

To measure psychological control, the Psychological Control Scale-Youth Report (PCS-YR) was used (Barber, 1996). This is an 8-item measure for both mothers and fathers, with a 3-point Likert scale ranging from “not like her/him” to “a lot like her/him.” Devaluation and guilt induction were included as two subscales of psychological control. Devaluation consisted of 6 items for both mothers (α = 0.82) and fathers (α = 0.83), while guilt induction consisted of 2 items for mothers (α = 0.66) and fathers (α = 0.71). A sample item of devaluation is that the mother or father “finishes my sentences whenever I talk,” and an example of guilt induction is the mother or father “blames me for other family members’ problems.” The subscales in our dataset indicated high collinearity, and we combined the subscales to label them as “maternal psychological control” and “paternal psychological control”. When these two subscales were combined, the internal consistency for maternal psychological control (α = 0.85) and maternal psychological control (α = 0.85) were high. In the exploratory analysis section, we examined the different roles of each subscale.

Academic Goal Engagement and Goal Disengagement

Education-specific Optimization of Primary and Secondary Control Scale (OPS) was used to measure motivational goal engagement and goal disengagement strategies (Heckhausen and Tomasik, 2002). The 9-item scale contained 5 items reflecting behavioral engagement (e.g., I will put time and effort into my education whenever I can”) and 4 items reflecting cognitive engagement (e.g., “I often tell myself that I will be successful in reaching my educational goals”). Responses from the 5-point Likert scale ranged from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree,” and they were averaged to produce the composite goal engagement measure (α = 0.84). Four items relating to goal adjustment (e.g., “If I have trouble with homework, I will adjust my goals so they are more realistic”) and goal disengagement (e.g., “If I cannot attain my educational goals, I will let go of them”) were averaged to create the goal disengagement measure (α = 0.61), which has been validated and used in multiple studies (e.g., Heckhausen et al., 1998; Heckhausen and Tomasik, 2002). All items from the 5-point Likert scale included responses ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.”

Academic Achievement

Participants were asked to report their grades using a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from “Mostly lower than C’s” to “Mostly A’s.”

Demographics

Participants reported their gender (1 = Male, 2 = Female), parental education, and ethnicity. Mothers’ and fathers’ parental education were coded into three ordinal levels (1 = less than high school, 2 = high school completion, 3 = more than high school). The highest of the two education scores between the parents was used, while the existing score was used when either the mother’s or father’s scores was missing. Ethnicity was categorized into three groups (Whites, Latinx, and Other), with each group dummy-coded.

Analytical Plan

A regression-based mediation model was conducted using maternal support as a predictor, academic goal engagement as a mediator, and adolescents’ academic achievement as an outcome measure to test hypothesis 1a. Hypothesis 1b was tested by including paternal support as a predictor in the second regression-based mediation model. Hypothesis 2a was tested through a third regression-based mediation model to determine whether academic goal disengagement mediated the relation between maternal psychological control and academic achievement, and a fourth regression-based mediation model was conducted using paternal psychological control as a predictor to test hypothesis 2b.
For our supplementary analysis, we explored the role of parental support and parental psychological control sub-factors. We used an additional mediation model to examine the indirect effects of specific sub-factors of parental support (warmth, involvement, and autonomy support from mothers and fathers) and psychological control (devaluation and guilt induction from both mothers and fathers) on academic achievement mediated by academic goal engagement and academic goal disengagement.
Mediation testing procedures were conducted both by using the causal-steps approach, a four-step approach in which several OLS regression analyses are conducted with the significance of the coefficients examined at each step (Baron and Kenny, 1986), and by using the bootstrapping of indirect effects approach (Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The bootstrap sample was set at 5000 and the significant confidence interval was set at 95%. A bootstrapped 95% CI that did not contain zero meant that there was a significant indirect effect (Hayes, 2017). Mediation was only tested if each of the steps in the causal-steps approach were satisfied. In all models, participants’ age, gender, ethnicity and parental education were controlled as covariates. SPSS version 24.0 using PROCESS macro was used to test the mediation models (Hayes, 2017), and SPSS version 24.0 was also used for all other models.
Due to the limitation of determining directionality with a cross-sectional dataset, alternative models were tested to support the robustness of the model pertaining to the hypotheses. It is possible that the association between parenting and academic achievement is bidirectional and parents are responding to adolescents’ academic achievement (e.g., Silinskas et al., 2013). To address this issue, we tested whether academic achievement predicted parenting.

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among the variables of interest are shown in Table 1 and Table 2 (N = 220). The correlation patterns showed that the variables of interest were associated with the expected directions.

Goal Engagement Mediating the Association Between Parental Support and Academic Performance

Hypothesis 1a was tested using a series of regression models that assessed whether the relation between maternal support and academic achievement was mediated by academic goal engagement. Participants’ grade, gender, ethnicity, and parental education level were included as covariates in each regression model.
Following the four-step approach by Baron and Kenny (1986), the first step showed that maternal support was positively associated with academic achievement (C-path: β = 0.18, p = 0.007). In the second step, we found a positive association between mothers’ support and adolescents’ goal engagement (A-path: β = 0.29, p < 0.001). In the third step, goal engagement was positively associated with academic achievement (B-path: β = 0.34, p < 0.001). Maternal support was no longer directly associated with academic achievement when controlling for goal engagement (C’-path: β = 0.09, p = 0.196). According to the causal-steps approach (Baron and Kenny, 1986), this pattern of results suggests that goal engagement fully mediates the relation between maternal support and academic achievement. Mediation was next assessed using the bootstrapping of indirect effect approach (Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The standardized indirect effect from maternal support to academic achievement via goal engagement was 0.10. This indirect effect was tested for significance using a bootstrap approach that employed 95% bias corrected confidence intervals based on 5000 samples of the indirect effects. Results showed that the 95% confidence interval did not contain zero (95% CI [0.034, 0.172]), which indicated that the indirect effect from parental support to academic achievement via academic engagement was significant (Hayes, 2017). The regression-based mediation model explained 17.33% of the total variance in adolescents’ academic achievement (Miočević et al., 2018). The results are depicted in Table 3 and Figure 1, and support hypothesis 1a in that academic goal engagement mediated the relation between maternal support and academic achievement.
Table 3
Regression-based mediation model for association between maternal support and academic achievement via goal engagement
Variables
b
SE
β
t
p
Model 1 Outcome variable = Goal Engagement
(constant)
3.25
0.29
 
11.37
<0.001
Maternal Support
0.21
0.05
0.29
4.38
<0.001
 Grade
0.04
0.04
0.08
1.22
0.223
 Gender
0.06
0.08
0.05
0.83
0.409
Ethnicity
 White
0.16
0.13
0.09
1.23
0.219
 Other
−0.30
0.13
−0.15
−2.23
0.027
Parental Education
−0.07
0.05
−0.10
−1.35
0.177
 
F (6, 213) = 5.40, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.132
Model 2 Outcome variable = Academic Achievement
(constant)
−1.38
0.90
 
−1.54
0.126
Maternal Support
0.16
0.12
0.09
1.30
0.196
Goal Engagement
0.85
0.17
0.34
5.06
<0.001
Grade
0.11
0.09
08
1.24
0.216
Gender
0.42
0.20
0.14
2.14
0.033
Ethnicity
Whites
0.17
0.33
0.03
0.51
0.612
Others
0.14
0.33
0.03
0.41
0.679
Parental Education
0.27
0.13
0.15
20.03
0.043
 
F (7, 212) = 6.35, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.173
b, SE, β, t, and p respectively indicate unstandardized coefficient, standard error, standardized coefficient, t statistics, and probability statistics. When controlling for ethnicities as a covariate, Latinx was treated as the reference group. For gender, male was coded as “1” and female was coded as “2”
A total effect test was conducted for hypothesis 1b, and the first step of the four-step approach showed that paternal support was not significantly associated with academic achievement (C-path: β = 0.14, p = 0.055). Thus, we did not proceed to test whether adolescents’ goal engagement mediated the relation between paternal support and academic achievement using the causal steps approach.

Goal Disengagement Mediating the Association Between Psychological Control and Academic Achievement

To test hypothesis 2a and 2b, we conducted a series of regressions to test whether goal disengagement mediated the association between perceived parental psychological control and adolescent academic achievement. Participants’ grade, gender, ethnicity, and parental education level were included as covariates in each regression model.
The total effect test between maternal psychological control and academic achievement was conducted as step one of the casual steps approach (Baron and Kenney, 1986). Results showed that maternal psychological control was negatively associated with academic achievement (C-path: β = −0.19, p = 0.005). Step two showed that maternal psychological control was positively associated with goal disengagement (A-path: β = 0.18, p = 0.008). In step three, we found that goal disengagement negatively predicted academic achievement (B-path: β = −0.20, p = 0.003). Maternal psychological control remained positively associated of academic achievement when goal disengagement was controlled in the model (C’-path: β = −0.16, p = 0.021). According to the causal steps approach (Baron and Kenny, 1986), this pattern of results indicates that the relation between parental psychological control and adolescent academic achievement was partially mediated by academic goal disengagement. Mediation was further examined using the bootstrapping of indirect effect approach (Shrout & Bulger, 2002). The standardized indirect effect from maternal psychological control to adolescent academic achievement via goal disengagement was −0.04. Standardized indirect effects were computed for each of 5000 bootstrapped samples to form a 95% confidence interval of the estimate. The 95% confidence interval did not contain zero (95% CI [−0.078, −0.005]), indicating that the indirect effect was statistically significant (Hayes, 2017). The regression-based mediation model explained 11.41% of the total variance in adolescents’ academic achievement (Miočević et al., 2018). The results are presented in Table 4 and Figure 2, and collectively support hypothesis 2a in suggesting that goal disengagement at least partly mediates the relation between maternal psychological control and lower academic achievement.
Table 4
Regression-based mediation model for association between maternal psychological control and academic achievement via goal disengagement
Variables
b
SE
β
t
p
Model 1 Outcome variable = Goal Disengagement
(constant)
2.87
0.34
 
8.53
< 0.001
Maternal Psychological Control
0.28
0.10
0.18
2.69
0.008
Grade
−0.06
0.05
−0.07
−1.08
0.282
Gender
−0.29
0.12
−0.17
−2.44
0.015
Ethnicity
Whites
−0.03
0.20
−0.01
−0.15
0.884
Others
−0.02
0.20
−0.01
−0.10
0.918
Parental Education
0.08
0.08
0.08
1.00
0.320
 
F (6, 213) = 2.57, p = 0.020, R2 = 0.068
Model 2 Outcome variable = Academic Achievement
(constant)
4.65
0.67
 
6.95
< 0.001
Maternal Psychological Control
−0.42
0.18
−0.16
−2.33
0.021
Goal Disengagement
−0.35
0.12
−0.20
−2.98
0.003
Grade
0.11
0.09
0.08
1.24
0.215
Gender
0.38
0.21
0.12
1.86
0.064
Ethnicity
Whites
0.36
0.34
0.07
10.06
0.201
Others
−0.12
0.34
−0.02
−0.35
0.728
Parental Education
0.23
0.14
0.12
1.65
0.101
 
F (7, 212) = 3.90, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.114
b, SE, β, t, and p respectively indicate unstandardized coefficient, standard error, standardized coefficient, t statistics, and probability statistics. When controlling for ethnicities as a covariate, Latnix was treated as the reference group. For gender, male was coded as “1” and female was coded as “2”
For hypothesis 2b, the first step of the causal steps approach showed that paternal psychological control was negatively associated with academic achievement (B-path: β = −0.18, p = 0.011). However, step two showed that paternal psychological control did not significantly predict adolescents’ goal disengagement (A-path: β = 0.11, p = 0.109). Thus, we did not proceed to test whether adolescents’ goal disengagement mediated the relation between fathers’ psychological control and academic achievement, and hypothesis 2b was not supported.

Exploratory Analysis of Subscales of Parental Support and Psychological Control

To better understand why goal engagement may mediate the association between parental support and academic achievement, we conducted a series of mediation analyses using the subscales that measured parental support (i.e., warmth, involvement, and autonomy support) as predictors, academic achievement as an outcome variable, and goal engagement as a mediator for mother and fathers. As step one of the casual steps approach of mediation testing, maternal support subscales were entered as predictors with academic achievement as the outcome (C-path) (Baron and Kenney, 1986). We found that only mothers’ warmth was positively associated with academic achievement (β = 0.32, p = 0.012), while mothers’ involvement (β = −0.18, p = 0.095), and mothers’ autonomy support (β = 0.05, p = 0.658) were not significantly associated with academic achievement. Therefore, only mother’s warmth was entered as a predictor in the second step. Step two results showed that mothers’ warmth was positively associated with adolescents’ goal engagement (A-path: β = 0.33, p < 0.001), and adolescents’ goal engagement was positively associated with academic achievement (B-path: β = 0.33, p < 0.001). Mothers’ warmth no longer predicted academic achievement when goal engagement was controlled in the model (C’-path: β = 0.11, p = 0.101), indicating the relation was fully mediated. The standardized indirect effect via the mediation was 0.11 and significant (95% CI [0.044, 0.188]) (Hayes, 2017). In addition to the statistically significant effect size of 0.11, the mediation-based regression model explained 10.94% of the total variance in adolescents’ academic achievement (Miočević et al., 2018). On the other hand, no paternal support subscales were tested to be significantly associated with academic achievement (fathers’ warmth: β = 0.12, p = 0.294; fathers’ involvement: β = 0.05, p = 0.647; fathers’ autonomy support: β = −0.03, p = 0.817). Therefore, we did not proceed further to the mediation analysis for paternal support subscales.
We also conducted mediation analysis to investigate the mechanism between parental psychological control and academic achievement via goal disengagement by exploring the unique variances of the outcome variable associated with each subscale of parental psychological control scale (i.e., devaluation and guilt induction), separately for mothers and fathers. In step one of the causal steps approach (Baron and Kenny, 1986), the regression model using mothers’ devaluation and mothers’ guilt induction as predictors and academic achievement as the outcome showed that only mothers’ devaluation was negatively associated with academic achievement (C-path: β = −0.26, p = 0.003), while mothers’ guilt induction was not (C-path: β = 0.07, p = 0.379). Therefore, only mothers’ devaluation was used in the second step. Step two analysis showed that mothers’ devaluation was positively associated with adolescents’ goal disengagement (A-path: β = 0.20, p = 0.004), and adolescents’ goal disengagement was negatively associated with academic achievement (B-path: β = −0.19, p = 0.004). Mothers’ devaluation was also negatively associated with academic achievement when goal disengagement was controlled in the model (C’-path: β = −0.19, p = 0.010), indicating the relation was partially mediated by goal disengagement. The standardized indirect effect via the mediation was −0.04 and significant (95% CI [−0.081, −0.006]) (Hayes, 2017). In addition to the statistically significant effect size of −0.04, the final regression model explained 11.98% of the total variance in adolescents’ academic achievement (Miočević et al., 2018).
In the total effect test on the association between subscales of paternal psychological control and academic control, fathers’ guilt induction was negatively associated with academic achievement (C-path: β = −0.18, p = 0.031), while fathers’ devaluation was not (C-path: β = −0.04, p = 0.599). The second step was conducted including only fathers’ guilt induction, and the results showed that fathers’ guilt induction was not significantly associated with goal disengagement (A-path: β = −0.00, p = 0.944). Therefore, we did not proceed further to the next steps of mediation testing. Collectively, these results suggest that the association between mothers’ devaluation and academic achievement is partially mediated by a significant indirect pathway via goal disengagement. On the other hand, goal disengagement was not a significant mediator of the relation between fathers’ guilt induction and academic achievement.

Alternative Direction Model

Given the cross-sectional nature of the study, we conducted additional analyses to test whether the association among the variables of interests would be bidirectional. We tested multivariate mediation using goal engagement and goal disengagement as predictors, academic achievement as a mediator, and maternal support and maternal psychological control as outcome variables. The results showed that neither the standardized indirect effect from goal engagement to maternal support through academic achievement (β = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.002, 0.115]) nor the standardized indirect effect from goal disengagement to maternal psychological control through academic achievement (β = 0.03, 95% CI [−0.006, 0.061]) were significant (Hayes, 2017). Therefore, the alternative model test revealed the relative robustness of the main model of the study.

Discussion

The significance of academic achievement during high school in determining higher education attainment has been rising (Galla et al., 2019; Geiser & Santelices, 2007; Westrick et al., 2015), making it increasingly crucial that adolescents receive guidance and support from trustworthy and invested others such as parents (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Previous studies have demonstrated the role that parents play in supporting adolescents’ education (Froiland & Worrell, 2017; Veas et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2020). However, less is known about how academic goal engagement and disengagement mediate the relation between parenting and academic achievement. Our study contributes to the literature by revealing how parenting is indirectly associated with academic achievement via adolescents’ own engagement and disengagement with school-related goals.

Mediating Role of Goal Engagement for Maternal Support

Supporting Hypothesis 1a, our mediation analyses showed that adolescents’ goal engagement mediates the association between perceived maternal support and adolescents’ academic performance. Goal engagement consists of investing significant time and energy to achieve a goal, while using self-motivating skills of reminding oneself of the value of a goal or imagining positive consequences of attaining a goal (Heckhausen et al., 2010). Our results show that the degree to which adolescents reported using goal engagement strategies was associated with maternal support. Furthermore, similar to previous studies (Hall et al., 2006; Hamm et al., 2013) our results demonstrate that the active use of goal engagement was positively associated with greater academic success as indicated by overall grades. Hence, our results suggest that maternal support is associated with higher academic achievement because this support encourages adolescents to use behavioral and cognitive motivational strategies that reflect high academic goal engagement. It is also notable that we found a full mediation pattern, which suggests the possibility that the change in the degree of goal engagement may be the primary pathway of maternal supports’ association with academic achievement.
Our findings further indicate that while past studies have emphasized that various types of maternal support (i.e., showing warmth, spending time with the child, guiding the child towards independence) can benefit children and adolescents (e.g., Gordon & Cui, 2012; Inguglia et al., 2015; Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2005), maternal warmth may be particularly important for academic goal engagement and achievement. Results from supplementary analyses showed that only maternal warmth significantly predicted academic goal engagement, reflecting prior studies which have shown that when parents express warmth towards their adolescent child, the child may feel that they are capable of achieving academic success (e.g., Hill & Wang, 2015; Juang & Silbereisen, 2002). Hence, mothers’ warmth may provide adolescents with the self-efficacy beliefs that they need to become engaged with schoolwork and pursue their academic goals (Gurdal et al., 2016; Sun et al., 2020). Due to the high intercorrelations between mothers’ warmth, involvement, and autonomy support, however, findings regarding maternal support sub-factors need to be interpreted with caution. Future studies should further examine the unique contribution of each parenting characteristic to adolescents’ goal engagement and academic achievement.

Goal Disengagement and Maternal Psychological Control

We also found that goal disengagement strategies mediated the relation between maternal psychological control and academic achievement, which supported hypothesis 2a and is consistent with prior studies (Aunola & Nurmi, 2004; Pinquart, 2016). Recent findings have shown that parental psychological control is linked with adolescents’ uncertainty of their academic abilities (Lu et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2020). Adolescents who experience psychological control may develop low self-efficacy overtime in domains such as academics (e.g., Filippello et al., 2015). Our findings of the mediating role of adolescents’ goal disengagement may have resulted from the negative effects of maternal psychological control on their self-efficacy, which renders a higher chance of disengagement from academic goals. Another possible negative effect of maternal psychological control could be adolescents’ feelings of incompetence and lack of control, (Filippello et al., 2018), which may be associated with premature disengagement from their academic goals and ultimately a lower chance of achieving optimal academic performance.
Findings from the supplementary analysis on maternal psychological control sub-factors also suggest that the devaluation aspect of perceived maternal psychological control contributes to adolescents’ tendency to disengage from academic goals. One study found that devaluing the child by invalidating the child’s thoughts and expressions may lead to negative outcomes such as internalizing and externalizing behaviors (Symeou & Georgiou, 2017). Extending this finding to academics, devaluation from mothers may also contribute to adolescents’ low academic self-efficacy, which has been found to be associated with disengagement from their academic goals (Xu et al., 2020). When adolescents do not consider their own thoughts and behavior as valid and important, they may not believe that their actions lead to positive outcomes and consequently disengage from their academic goals, which may result in lower academic performance (Desjardins & Leadbeater, 2017; Mandara & Pikes, 2008). Regarding guilt induction, results showed that this component of maternal psychological control was not negatively associated with academic achievement. This finding suggests that though inducing guilt by bringing up past mistakes when criticizing the child or by blaming the child for others’ problems is harmful for their psychological well-being (e.g., Rakow et al., 2009), it may not necessarily contribute as much as devaluation to adolescents’ lack of persistence in pursuing and achieving their educational goals, especially when the sense of guilt was induced by mothers. Through examining the relation between different aspects of maternal psychological control and academics, our findings add to the existing literature by illustrating one possible process of how maternal psychological control is linked with students’ disengagement, and ultimately to lower academic achievement.

Differences in Mothers’ and Fathers’ Academic Influence

In contrast with the findings regarding mothers’ influence, paternal support and psychological control were only weakly associated with adolescents’ academic achievement. Our results showed that only maternal support and psychological control were associated with goal engagement and disengagement, which in turn was linked to academic achievement. These findings indicate that mothers continue to be a stronger influence on adolescents’ academic achievement than fathers. This is consistent with prior literature that has demonstrated greater academic-related involvement from mothers than from fathers (Hsu et al., 2011; Jeynes, 2015). However, these results may not necessarily suggest that fathers’ warmth or psychological devaluation do not affect adolescents’ developmental outcomes. Jeynes (2015) postulated that fathers may have a greater effect on youth’s behavioral outcomes, but a smaller effect on their academic achievement. This aligns with our finding which indicated that fathers’ guilt induction was significantly associated with lower academic achievements of adolescents, while mothers’ guilt induction was not. These findings suggest that mothers and fathers may negatively contribute to adolescents’ academic performance in different ways, and future studies should further examine potential mediating factors that explain the negative association between fathers’ guilt induction and adolescents’ academic achievement.

Limitations

There are several limitations in our study, the first being that this was a cross-sectional study. While the alternative model with reversed direction of association between parenting and academic achievement showed a weaker effect than the main model, we cannot confidently reject the possibility of a bidirectional relation between parenting and grades due to the lack of temporal precedence of the variables of interest. Second, our dataset did not contain information regarding prior academic achievement to be considered as a baseline measure. Future research should conduct longitudinal or experimental studies to confirm the direction of the associations between parenting, academic goal engagement and disengagement, and academic achievement. Third, as our sample size was relatively small with majority Latinx adolescents and only includes a specific point in time in a specific geographic location, the generalizability of our findings is potentially limited. Future studies with larger and more diverse samples would help establish the generalizability of our findings by examining whether demographic factors moderate the indirect effect of parenting on academic achievement. Fourth, the high amount of missing data for fathers’ variables may have affected the results on paternal support and psychological control. We expect that if future research is conducted with larger samples, the relatively unpronounced role of paternal support and psychological control may be better captured. Finally, we used a college-student version of the scale to measure perceived parenting and adolescents’ self-report of their experiences (i.e., perceived parental support, psychological control, grades). Having multiple observers assessing parent and child characteristics and multiple measures of academic achievement would help solidify the present findings.

Conclusion

Our study builds on the literature about parenting and adolescents’ academic pursuits by examining ways in which parents may affect adolescents’ academic achievement through impacting their use of motivational academic goal engagement and disengagement strategies. While maternal support through the expression of warmth predicts adolescents’ engagement with academic goals that helps them attain better academic outcomes, maternal psychological control such as devaluation of the child is associated with adolescents’ disengagement from academic goals which diminishes academic performance. These findings highlight the important role that mothers play in supporting adolescents’ education by identifying ways to increase their academic success through developing their effort investment and persistence in school, and by helping them avoid pre-mature disengagement from their academic goals.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference Abar, B., Carter, K. L., & Winsler, A. (2009). The effects of maternal parenting style and religious commitment on self-regulation, academic achievement, and risk behavior among African-American parochial college students. Journal of Adolescence, 32(2), 259–273.PubMed Abar, B., Carter, K. L., & Winsler, A. (2009). The effects of maternal parenting style and religious commitment on self-regulation, academic achievement, and risk behavior among African-American parochial college students. Journal of Adolescence, 32(2), 259–273.PubMed
go back to reference Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2004). Maternal affection moderates the impact of psychological control on a child’s mathematical performance. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 965.PubMed Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2004). Maternal affection moderates the impact of psychological control on a child’s mathematical performance. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 965.PubMed
go back to reference Aunola, K., Stattin, H., & Nurmi, J. E. (2000). Parenting styles and adolescents’ achievement strategies. Journal of Adolescence, 23(2), 205–222.PubMed Aunola, K., Stattin, H., & Nurmi, J. E. (2000). Parenting styles and adolescents’ achievement strategies. Journal of Adolescence, 23(2), 205–222.PubMed
go back to reference Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 3296–3319.PubMed Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 3296–3319.PubMed
go back to reference Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173.PubMed Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173.PubMed
go back to reference Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child development, 887-907. Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child development, 887-907.
go back to reference Boonk, L., Gijselaers, H. J., Ritzen, H., & Brand-Gruwel, S. (2018). A review of the relationship between parental involvement indicators and academic achievement. Educational Research Review, 24, 10–30. Boonk, L., Gijselaers, H. J., Ritzen, H., & Brand-Gruwel, S. (2018). A review of the relationship between parental involvement indicators and academic achievement. Educational Research Review, 24, 10–30.
go back to reference Brown, L., & Iyengar, S. (2008). Parenting styles: the impact on student achievement. Marriage & Family Review, 43(1-2), 14–38. Brown, L., & Iyengar, S. (2008). Parenting styles: the impact on student achievement. Marriage & Family Review, 43(1-2), 14–38.
go back to reference Desjardins, T., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2017). Changes in parental emotional support and psychological control in early adulthood: direct and indirect associations with educational and occupational adjustment. Emerging Adulthood, 5(3), 177–190. Desjardins, T., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2017). Changes in parental emotional support and psychological control in early adulthood: direct and indirect associations with educational and occupational adjustment. Emerging Adulthood, 5(3), 177–190.
go back to reference Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, D. F., & Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance. Child Development, 1244–1257. Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, D. F., & Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance. Child Development, 1244–1257.
go back to reference Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2009). Schools, academic motivation, and stage‐environment fit. Handbook of adolescent psychology, 1. Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2009). Schools, academic motivation, and stage‐environment fit. Handbook of adolescent psychology, 1.
go back to reference Filippello, P., Harrington, N., Costa, S., Buzzai, C., & Sorrenti, L. (2018). Perceived parental psychological control and school learned helplessness: the role of frustration intolerance as a mediator factor. School Psychology International, 39(4), 360–377. Filippello, P., Harrington, N., Costa, S., Buzzai, C., & Sorrenti, L. (2018). Perceived parental psychological control and school learned helplessness: the role of frustration intolerance as a mediator factor. School Psychology International, 39(4), 360–377.
go back to reference Filippello, P., Sorrenti, L., Buzzai, C., & Costa, S. (2015). Perceived parental psychological control and learned helplessness: the role of school self-efficacy. School Mental Health, 7(4), 298–310. Filippello, P., Sorrenti, L., Buzzai, C., & Costa, S. (2015). Perceived parental psychological control and learned helplessness: the role of school self-efficacy. School Mental Health, 7(4), 298–310.
go back to reference Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: correlations with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 531.PubMed Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: correlations with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 531.PubMed
go back to reference Froiland, J. M., & Worrell, F. C. (2017). Parental autonomy support, community feeling and student expectations as contributors to later achievement among adolescents. Educational Psychology, 37(3), 261–271. Froiland, J. M., & Worrell, F. C. (2017). Parental autonomy support, community feeling and student expectations as contributors to later achievement among adolescents. Educational Psychology, 37(3), 261–271.
go back to reference Fulton, E., & Turner, L. A. (2008). Students’ academic motivation: relations with parental warmth, autonomy granting, and supervision. Educational Psychology, 28(5), 521–534. Fulton, E., & Turner, L. A. (2008). Students’ academic motivation: relations with parental warmth, autonomy granting, and supervision. Educational Psychology, 28(5), 521–534.
go back to reference Galla, B. M., Shulman, E. P., Plummer, B. D., Gardner, M., Hutt, S. J., Goyer, J. P., & Duckworth, A. L. (2019). Why high school grades are better predictors of on-time college graduation than are admissions test scores: the roles of self-regulation and cognitive ability. American Educational Research Journal, 56(6), 2077–2115. Galla, B. M., Shulman, E. P., Plummer, B. D., Gardner, M., Hutt, S. J., Goyer, J. P., & Duckworth, A. L. (2019). Why high school grades are better predictors of on-time college graduation than are admissions test scores: the roles of self-regulation and cognitive ability. American Educational Research Journal, 56(6), 2077–2115.
go back to reference Garcia, O. F., & Serra, E. (2019). Raising children with poor school performance: parenting styles and short-and long-term consequences for adolescent and adult development. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(7), 1089.PubMedCentral Garcia, O. F., & Serra, E. (2019). Raising children with poor school performance: parenting styles and short-and long-term consequences for adolescent and adult development. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(7), 1089.PubMedCentral
go back to reference Geiser, S., & Santelices, M. V. (2007). Validity of high-school grades in predicting student success beyond the freshman year: high-school record vs. standardized tests as indicators of four-year college outcomes. Geiser, S., & Santelices, M. V. (2007). Validity of high-school grades in predicting student success beyond the freshman year: high-school record vs. standardized tests as indicators of four-year college outcomes.
go back to reference Gonida, E. N., & Cortina, K. S. (2014). Parental involvement in homework: relations with parent and student achievement‐related motivational beliefs and achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 376–396. Gonida, E. N., & Cortina, K. S. (2014). Parental involvement in homework: relations with parent and student achievement‐related motivational beliefs and achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 376–396.
go back to reference Gordon, M. S., & Cui, M. (2012). The effect of school‐specific parenting processes on academic achievement in adolescence and young adulthood. Family Relations, 61(5), 728–741. Gordon, M. S., & Cui, M. (2012). The effect of school‐specific parenting processes on academic achievement in adolescence and young adulthood. Family Relations, 61(5), 728–741.
go back to reference Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources for school achievement: Motivational mediators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 508–517. Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources for school achievement: Motivational mediators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 508–517.
go back to reference Gurdal, S., Lansford, J. E., & Sorbring, E. (2016). Parental perceptions of children’s agency: Parental warmth, school achievement and adjustment. Early Child Development and Care, 186(8), 1203–1211.PubMed Gurdal, S., Lansford, J. E., & Sorbring, E. (2016). Parental perceptions of children’s agency: Parental warmth, school achievement and adjustment. Early Child Development and Care, 186(8), 1203–1211.PubMed
go back to reference Guay, F., Ratelle, C., Larose, S., Vallerand, R. J., & Vitaro, F. (2013). The number of autonomy-supportive relationships: are more relationships better for motivation, perceived competence, and achievement? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(4), 375–382. Guay, F., Ratelle, C., Larose, S., Vallerand, R. J., & Vitaro, F. (2013). The number of autonomy-supportive relationships: are more relationships better for motivation, perceived competence, and achievement? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(4), 375–382.
go back to reference Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, J. C., & Goetz, T. (2006). Primary and secondary control in academic development: gender-specific implications for stress and health in college students. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 19(2), 189–210. Hall, N. C., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, J. C., & Goetz, T. (2006). Primary and secondary control in academic development: gender-specific implications for stress and health in college students. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 19(2), 189–210.
go back to reference Hamm, J. M., Stewart, T. L., Perry, R. P., Clifton, R. A., Chipperfield, J. G., & Heckhausen, J. (2013). Sustaining primary control striving for achievement goals during challenging developmental transitions: the role of secondary control strategies. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(3), 286–297. Hamm, J. M., Stewart, T. L., Perry, R. P., Clifton, R. A., Chipperfield, J. G., & Heckhausen, J. (2013). Sustaining primary control striving for achievement goals during challenging developmental transitions: the role of secondary control strategies. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(3), 286–297.
go back to reference Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford publications. Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford publications.
go back to reference Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.PubMed Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.PubMed
go back to reference Heckhausen, J., Schulz, R., & Wrosch, C. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: optimization in primary and secondary control—a multiscale questionnaire (OPS-Scales). Technical report. Heckhausen, J., Schulz, R., & Wrosch, C. (1998). Developmental regulation in adulthood: optimization in primary and secondary control—a multiscale questionnaire (OPS-Scales). Technical report.
go back to reference Heckhausen, J., & Tomasik, M. J. (2002). Get an apprenticeship before school is out: how German adolescents adjust vocational aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 199–219. Heckhausen, J., & Tomasik, M. J. (2002). Get an apprenticeship before school is out: how German adolescents adjust vocational aspirations when getting close to a developmental deadline. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 199–219.
go back to reference Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117(1), 32.PubMedPubMedCentral Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117(1), 32.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Henry, C. S., Plunkett, S. W., & Sands, T. (2011). Family structure, parental involvement, and academic motivation in Latino adolescents. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 52(6), 370–390. Henry, C. S., Plunkett, S. W., & Sands, T. (2011). Family structure, parental involvement, and academic motivation in Latino adolescents. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 52(6), 370–390.
go back to reference Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: a meta-analysis of parental involvement and students’ academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(4), 919. Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: a meta-analysis of parental involvement and students’ academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(4), 919.
go back to reference Hill, N. E., Castellino, D. R., Lansford, J. E., Nowlin, P., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Parent academic involvement as related to school behavior, achievement, and aspirations: Demographic variations across adolescence. Child Development, 75(5), 1491–1509.PubMedPubMedCentral Hill, N. E., Castellino, D. R., Lansford, J. E., Nowlin, P., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Parent academic involvement as related to school behavior, achievement, and aspirations: Demographic variations across adolescence. Child Development, 75(5), 1491–1509.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hill, N. E., & Wang, M. T. (2015). From middle school to college: developing aspirations, promoting engagement, and indirect pathways from parenting to post high school enrollment. Developmental Psychology, 51(2), 224–235.PubMed Hill, N. E., & Wang, M. T. (2015). From middle school to college: developing aspirations, promoting engagement, and indirect pathways from parenting to post high school enrollment. Developmental Psychology, 51(2), 224–235.PubMed
go back to reference Hsu, H. Y., Zhang, D., Kwok, O. M., Li, Y., & Ju, S. (2011). Distinguishing the influences of father’s and mother’s involvement on adolescent academic achievement: Analyses of Taiwan Education Panel Survey data. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(5), 694–713. Hsu, H. Y., Zhang, D., Kwok, O. M., Li, Y., & Ju, S. (2011). Distinguishing the influences of father’s and mother’s involvement on adolescent academic achievement: Analyses of Taiwan Education Panel Survey data. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(5), 694–713.
go back to reference Inguglia, C., Ingoglia, S., Liga, F., Coco, A. L., & Cricchio, M. G. L. (2015). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Relationships with parental support and psychological distress. Journal of Adult Development, 22(1), 1–13. Inguglia, C., Ingoglia, S., Liga, F., Coco, A. L., & Cricchio, M. G. L. (2015). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Relationships with parental support and psychological distress. Journal of Adult Development, 22(1), 1–13.
go back to reference Jeynes, W. H. (2015). A meta-analysis: The relationship between father involvement and student academic achievement. Urban Education, 50(4), 387–423. Jeynes, W. H. (2015). A meta-analysis: The relationship between father involvement and student academic achievement. Urban Education, 50(4), 387–423.
go back to reference Juang, L. P., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2002). The relationship between adolescent academic capability beliefs, parenting and school grades. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 3–18.PubMed Juang, L. P., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2002). The relationship between adolescent academic capability beliefs, parenting and school grades. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 3–18.PubMed
go back to reference Komarraju, M., & Nadler, D. (2013). Self-efficacy and academic achievement: Why do implicit beliefs, goals, and effort regulation matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 67–72. Komarraju, M., & Nadler, D. (2013). Self-efficacy and academic achievement: Why do implicit beliefs, goals, and effort regulation matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 67–72.
go back to reference Li, P., Stuart, E. A., & Allison, D. B. (2015). Multiple imputation: a flexible tool for handling missing data. JAMA, 314(18), 1966–1967.PubMedPubMedCentral Li, P., Stuart, E. A., & Allison, D. B. (2015). Multiple imputation: a flexible tool for handling missing data. JAMA, 314(18), 1966–1967.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Little, R. J. (1998). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. Little, R. J. (1998). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202.
go back to reference Lowe, K., & Dotterer, A. M. (2013). Parental monitoring, parental warmth, and minority youths’ academic outcomes: exploring the integrative model of parenting. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(9), 1413–1425.PubMed Lowe, K., & Dotterer, A. M. (2013). Parental monitoring, parental warmth, and minority youths’ academic outcomes: exploring the integrative model of parenting. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(9), 1413–1425.PubMed
go back to reference Lu, M., Walsh, K., White, S., & Shield, P. (2017). The associations between perceived maternal psychological control and academic performance and academic self-concept in Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of basic psychological needs. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(5), 1285–1297. Lu, M., Walsh, K., White, S., & Shield, P. (2017). The associations between perceived maternal psychological control and academic performance and academic self-concept in Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of basic psychological needs. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(5), 1285–1297.
go back to reference Mandara, J., & Pikes, C. L. (2008). Guilt trips and love withdrawal: Does mothers’ use of psychological control predict depressive symptoms among African American adolescents? Family Relations, 57(5), 602–612. Mandara, J., & Pikes, C. L. (2008). Guilt trips and love withdrawal: Does mothers’ use of psychological control predict depressive symptoms among African American adolescents? Family Relations, 57(5), 602–612.
go back to reference Markazi, L., & Badrigargari, R. (2011). The role of parenting self-efficacy and parenting styles on self-regulation learning in adolescent girls of Tabriz. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 1758–1760. Markazi, L., & Badrigargari, R. (2011). The role of parenting self-efficacy and parenting styles on self-regulation learning in adolescent girls of Tabriz. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 1758–1760.
go back to reference Miočević, M., O’Rourke, H. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Brown, H. C. (2018). Statistical properties of four effect-size measures for mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 285–301.PubMed Miočević, M., O’Rourke, H. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Brown, H. C. (2018). Statistical properties of four effect-size measures for mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 285–301.PubMed
go back to reference Monzani, D., Steca, P., Greco, A., D’Addario, M., Pancani, L., & Cappelletti, E. (2015). Effective pursuit of personal goals: the fostering effect of dispositional optimism on goal commitment and goal progress. Personality and Individual Differences, 82, 203–214. Monzani, D., Steca, P., Greco, A., D’Addario, M., Pancani, L., & Cappelletti, E. (2015). Effective pursuit of personal goals: the fostering effect of dispositional optimism on goal commitment and goal progress. Personality and Individual Differences, 82, 203–214.
go back to reference Niemiec, C. P., Lynch, M. F., Vansteenkiste, M., Bernstein, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). The antecedents and consequences of autonomous self-regulation for college: a self-determination theory perspective on socialization. Journal of adolescence, 29(5), 761–775.PubMed Niemiec, C. P., Lynch, M. F., Vansteenkiste, M., Bernstein, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). The antecedents and consequences of autonomous self-regulation for college: a self-determination theory perspective on socialization. Journal of adolescence, 29(5), 761–775.PubMed
go back to reference Pinquart, M. (2016). Associations of parenting styles and dimensions with academic achievement in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 475–493. Pinquart, M. (2016). Associations of parenting styles and dimensions with academic achievement in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 475–493.
go back to reference Pinquart, M., & Ebeling, M. (2020). Parental educational expectations and academic achievement in children and adolescents—a Meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 463–480. Pinquart, M., & Ebeling, M. (2020). Parental educational expectations and academic achievement in children and adolescents—a Meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 463–480.
go back to reference Poulin, M. J., & Heckhausen, J. (2007). Stressful events compromise control strivings during a major life transition. Motivation and Emotion, 31(4), 300–311. Poulin, M. J., & Heckhausen, J. (2007). Stressful events compromise control strivings during a major life transition. Motivation and Emotion, 31(4), 300–311.
go back to reference Rakow, A., Forehand, R., McKee, L., Coffelt, N., Champion, J., Fear, J., & Compas, B. (2009). The relation of parental guilt induction to child internalizing problems when a caregiver has a history ‘of depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18(4), 367–377.PubMedPubMedCentral Rakow, A., Forehand, R., McKee, L., Coffelt, N., Champion, J., Fear, J., & Compas, B. (2009). The relation of parental guilt induction to child internalizing problems when a caregiver has a history ‘of depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18(4), 367–377.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Roksa, J., & Potter, D. (2011). Parenting and academic achievement: intergenerational transmission of educational advantage. Sociology of Education, 84(4), 299–321. Roksa, J., & Potter, D. (2011). Parenting and academic achievement: intergenerational transmission of educational advantage. Sociology of Education, 84(4), 299–321.
go back to reference Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003). Coping with deficits and losses in later life: from compensatory action to accommodation. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 896.PubMed Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003). Coping with deficits and losses in later life: from compensatory action to accommodation. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 896.PubMed
go back to reference Sangawi, H., Adams, J., & Reissland, N. (2018). The impact of parenting styles on children developmental outcome: the role of academic self‐concept as a mediator. International Journal of Psychology, 53(5), 379–387.PubMed Sangawi, H., Adams, J., & Reissland, N. (2018). The impact of parenting styles on children developmental outcome: the role of academic self‐concept as a mediator. International Journal of Psychology, 53(5), 379–387.PubMed
go back to reference Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422–445.PubMed Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422–445.PubMed
go back to reference Silinskas, G., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M. K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2013). Children’s poor academic performance evokes parental homework assistance—but does it help? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(1), 44–56. Silinskas, G., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M. K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2013). Children’s poor academic performance evokes parental homework assistance—but does it help? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(1), 44–56.
go back to reference Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents and outcomes of self-determination in 3 life domains: the role of parents’ and teachers’ autonomy support. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(6), 589–604. Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents and outcomes of self-determination in 3 life domains: the role of parents’ and teachers’ autonomy support. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(6), 589–604.
go back to reference Sun, Y., Liu, R. D., Oei, T. P., Zhen, R., Ding, Y., & Jiang, R. (2020). Perceived parental warmth and adolescents’ math engagement in China: The mediating roles of need satisfaction and math self-efficacy. Learning and Individual Differences, 78, 101837. Sun, Y., Liu, R. D., Oei, T. P., Zhen, R., Ding, Y., & Jiang, R. (2020). Perceived parental warmth and adolescents’ math engagement in China: The mediating roles of need satisfaction and math self-efficacy. Learning and Individual Differences, 78, 101837.
go back to reference Symeou, M., & Georgiou, S. (2017). Externalizing and internalizing behaviours in adolescence, and the importance of parental behavioural and psychological control practices. Journal of Adolescence, 60, 104–113.PubMed Symeou, M., & Georgiou, S. (2017). Externalizing and internalizing behaviours in adolescence, and the importance of parental behavioural and psychological control practices. Journal of Adolescence, 60, 104–113.PubMed
go back to reference Tian, Y., Yu, C., Lin, S., Lu, J., Liu, Y., & Zhang, W. (2019). Parental psychological control and adolescent aggressive behavior: deviant peer affiliation as a mediator and school connectedness as a moderator. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 358.PubMedPubMedCentral Tian, Y., Yu, C., Lin, S., Lu, J., Liu, Y., & Zhang, W. (2019). Parental psychological control and adolescent aggressive behavior: deviant peer affiliation as a mediator and school connectedness as a moderator. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 358.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Topor, D. R., Keane, S. P., Shelton, T. L., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Parent involvement and student academic performance: a multiple mediational analysis. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 38(3), 183–197. Topor, D. R., Keane, S. P., Shelton, T. L., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Parent involvement and student academic performance: a multiple mediational analysis. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 38(3), 183–197.
go back to reference Veas, A., Castejón, J. L., Miñano, P., & Gilar‐Corbí, R. (2019). Relationship between parent involvement and academic achievement through metacognitive strategies: A multiple multilevel mediation analysis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 393–411. Veas, A., Castejón, J. L., Miñano, P., & Gilar‐Corbí, R. (2019). Relationship between parent involvement and academic achievement through metacognitive strategies: A multiple multilevel mediation analysis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 393–411.
go back to reference Wang, M. T., Hill, N. E., & Hofkens, T. (2014). Parental involvement and African American and European American adolescents’ academic, behavioral, and emotional development in secondary school. Child Development, 85(6), 2151–2168.PubMed Wang, M. T., Hill, N. E., & Hofkens, T. (2014). Parental involvement and African American and European American adolescents’ academic, behavioral, and emotional development in secondary school. Child Development, 85(6), 2151–2168.PubMed
go back to reference Wang, M., Deng, X., & Du, X. (2018). Harsh parenting and academic achievement in Chinese adolescents: Potential mediating roles of effortful control and classroom engagement. Journal of School Psychology, 67, 16–30.PubMed Wang, M., Deng, X., & Du, X. (2018). Harsh parenting and academic achievement in Chinese adolescents: Potential mediating roles of effortful control and classroom engagement. Journal of School Psychology, 67, 16–30.PubMed
go back to reference Wang, M. T., & Sheikh‐Khalil, S. (2014). Does parental involvement matter for student achievement and mental health in high school? Child Development, 85(2), 610–625.PubMed Wang, M. T., & Sheikh‐Khalil, S. (2014). Does parental involvement matter for student achievement and mental health in high school? Child Development, 85(2), 610–625.PubMed
go back to reference Westrick, P. A., Le, H., Robbins, S. B., Radunzel, J. M., & Schmidt, F. L. (2015). College performance and retention: a meta-analysis of the predictive validities of ACT® scores, high school grades, and SES. Educational Assessment, 20(1), 23–45. Westrick, P. A., Le, H., Robbins, S. B., Radunzel, J. M., & Schmidt, F. L. (2015). College performance and retention: a meta-analysis of the predictive validities of ACT® scores, high school grades, and SES. Educational Assessment, 20(1), 23–45.
go back to reference Woolley, M. E., Kol, K. L., & Bowen, G. L. (2009). The social context of school success for Latino middle school students: direct and indirect influences of teachers, family, and friends. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29(1), 43–70. Woolley, M. E., Kol, K. L., & Bowen, G. L. (2009). The social context of school success for Latino middle school students: direct and indirect influences of teachers, family, and friends. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29(1), 43–70.
go back to reference Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2020). Adaptive self-regulation, subjective well-being, and physical health: the importance of goal adjustment capacities. In Advances in Motivation Science (Vol. 7, pp. 199-238). Elsevier. Wrosch, C., & Scheier, M. F. (2020). Adaptive self-regulation, subjective well-being, and physical health: the importance of goal adjustment capacities. In Advances in Motivation Science (Vol. 7, pp. 199-238). Elsevier.
go back to reference Xiang, S., Liu, Y., & Bai, L. (2017). Parenting styles and adolescents’ school adjustment: investigating the mediating role of achievement goals within the 2× 2 framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1809.PubMedPubMedCentral Xiang, S., Liu, Y., & Bai, L. (2017). Parenting styles and adolescents’ school adjustment: investigating the mediating role of achievement goals within the 2× 2 framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1809.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Xu, X., Dai, D. Y., Liu, M., & Deng, C. (2020). Parental psychological control and academic functioning in Chinese high school students: a short‐term longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 90–10. Xu, X., Dai, D. Y., Liu, M., & Deng, C. (2020). Parental psychological control and academic functioning in Chinese high school students: a short‐term longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 90–10.
Metagegevens
Titel
Parenting and Adolescents’ Academic Achievement: The Mediating Role of Goal Engagement and Disengagement
Auteurs
Priscilla S. Yau
Yongwon Cho
Jacob Shane
Joseph Kay
Jutta Heckhausen
Publicatiedatum
07-07-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Child and Family Studies / Uitgave 4/2022
Print ISSN: 1062-1024
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2843
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-02007-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2022

Journal of Child and Family Studies 4/2022 Naar de uitgave