Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2008

01-01-2008 | Original Article

Multiple temporal references in sensorimotor synchronization with metrical auditory sequences

Auteur: Bruno H. Repp

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A local phase perturbation in an auditory sequence during synchronized finger tapping elicits an automatic phase correction response (PCR). The stimulus for the PCR is usually considered to be the most recent tap-tone asynchrony. In this study, participants tapped on target tones (“beats”) of isochronous tone sequences consisting of beats and subdivisions (1:n tapping). A phase perturbation was introduced either on a beat or on a subdivision. Both types of perturbation elicited a PCR, even though there was no asynchrony associated with a subdivision. Moreover, the PCR to a perturbed beat was smaller when an unperturbed subdivision followed than when there was no subdivision. The relative size of the PCRs to perturbed beats and subdivisions depended on tempo, on whether the subdivision was local or present throughout the sequence, and on whether or not participants engaged in mental subdivision, but not on whether or not taps were made on the subdivision level. The results show that phase correction in synchronization depends not merely on asynchronies but on perceptual monitoring of multiple temporal references within a metrical hierarchy.
Voetnoten
1
Large et al. (2002) call the events at the lowest metrical level (IOI = 400 ms) beats and thus do not talk about subdivisions. However, the perceived main beat (tactus) was probably either at the 1,600-ms level (because of the modular structure of the sequences) or at the 800-ms level (because at this rate the beat salience is maximal), so that the events at the lowest level functioned as subdivisions.
 
2
The experiment also contained sequences with much smaller, mostly subliminal EOSs (± 10 ms). The results, while generally consistent with those for the larger EOSs, were too variable to reach statistical significance and therefore are not reported.
 
3
All intervals are reported here as specified or recorded by the MAX software. It is known from acoustic measurements that the real-time temporal intervals generated or recorded by MAX in this configuration were shorter by about 2.4%.
 
4
Double standard errors are slightly smaller than two-tailed but slightly larger than one-tailed 95% confidence intervals. A one-tailed test against zero is justified here because mean PCRs are expected to be in the same direction as the EOS.
 
5
Relative times of occurrence are defined as the asynchronies of taps relative to an isochronous 560-ms time grid initiated by the first beat-level tone.
 
6
One type of sequence was missing in this design, namely sub EOS no beat with EOS = 0. During preparation of the materials it was overlooked that this sequence is not identical with the EOS = 0 versions of sub EOS + beat and sub + beat EOS.
 
7
Evidently, this last instruction was difficult to follow. Three participants tapped so lightly with their left (subdivision) hand that the taps sometimes were not registered. This happened particularly when the subdivision hand tapped in phase with the beat hand. As this was the least interesting part of the data (the two hands were expected to show similar PCRs when tapping in phase with each other), no steps were taken to correct this problem. For each participant, there were still sufficient trials with complete data to compute an (albeit less reliable) estimate of the PCR in the subdivision hand.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.PubMedCrossRef Aschersleben, G. (2002). Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain and Cognition, 48, 66–79.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 305–317. Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1995). Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 305–317.
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1997). Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 35–46.PubMed Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1997). Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 35–46.PubMed
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization: Intersensory integration of action effects. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 227–244). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization: Intersensory integration of action effects. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 227–244). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.CrossRef Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Byblow, W. D., Chua, R., & Goodman, D. (1995). Asymmetries in coupling dynamics of perception and action. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 123–137.PubMedCrossRef Byblow, W. D., Chua, R., & Goodman, D. (1995). Asymmetries in coupling dynamics of perception and action. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 123–137.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, Y., Ding, M., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1997). Long memory processes (1/f a type) in human coordination. Physical Review Letters, 79, 4501–4504.CrossRef Chen, Y., Ding, M., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1997). Long memory processes (1/f a type) in human coordination. Physical Review Letters, 79, 4501–4504.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, Y., Ding, M., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2001). Origins of timing errors in human sensorimotor coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33, 3–8.PubMed Chen, Y., Ding, M., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2001). Origins of timing errors in human sensorimotor coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33, 3–8.PubMed
go back to reference Chen, Y., Repp, B. H., & Patel, A. D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.PubMedCrossRef Chen, Y., Repp, B. H., & Patel, A. D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Collyer, C. E., Boatright-Horowitz, S. S., & Hooper, S. (1997). A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 346–352. Collyer, C. E., Boatright-Horowitz, S. S., & Hooper, S. (1997). A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 346–352.
go back to reference Desain, P. (1992). A (de)composable theory of rhythm perception. Music Perception, 9, 439–454. Desain, P. (1992). A (de)composable theory of rhythm perception. Music Perception, 9, 439–454.
go back to reference Drake, C., & Palmer, C. (1993). Accent structures in music performance. Music Perception, 10, 343–378. Drake, C., & Palmer, C. (1993). Accent structures in music performance. Music Perception, 10, 343–378.
go back to reference Eck, D. (2002). Finding downbeats with a relaxation oscillator. Psychological Research, 66, 18–25.PubMedCrossRef Eck, D. (2002). Finding downbeats with a relaxation oscillator. Psychological Research, 66, 18–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engström, D. A., Kelso, J. A. S., & Holroyd, T. (1996). Reaction–anticipation transitions in human perception–action patterns. Human Movement Science, 15, 809–832.CrossRef Engström, D. A., Kelso, J. A. S., & Holroyd, T. (1996). Reaction–anticipation transitions in human perception–action patterns. Human Movement Science, 15, 809–832.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P., Oléron, G., & Paillard, J. (1958). Sur les repères sensoriels qui permettent de controler les mouvements d’accompagnement de stimuli périodiques. [On the sensory data that enable control of movements accompanying periodic stimuli.] L’Année Psychologique, 58, 322–338. Fraisse, P., Oléron, G., & Paillard, J. (1958). Sur les repères sensoriels qui permettent de controler les mouvements d’accompagnement de stimuli périodiques. [On the sensory data that enable control of movements accompanying periodic stimuli.] L’Année Psychologique, 58, 322–338.
go back to reference Franêk, M., Radil, T., & Indra, M. (1990). Subjective timing of rhythmical patterns during synchronization and reproduction of acoustic sequences. Studia Psychologica, 32, 203–209. Franêk, M., Radil, T., & Indra, M. (1990). Subjective timing of rhythmical patterns during synchronization and reproduction of acoustic sequences. Studia Psychologica, 32, 203–209.
go back to reference Franêk, M., Mates, J., Radil, T., Beck, K., & Pöppel, E. (1991). Sensorimotor synchronization: Motor responses to regular auditory patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 509–516. Franêk, M., Mates, J., Radil, T., Beck, K., & Pöppel, E. (1991). Sensorimotor synchronization: Motor responses to regular auditory patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 509–516.
go back to reference Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1995). Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 2524–2531.CrossRef Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1995). Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 2524–2531.CrossRef
go back to reference Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.PubMed Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.PubMed
go back to reference Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1987). Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source. Biological Cybernetics, 56, 305–311.PubMedCrossRef Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1987). Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source. Biological Cybernetics, 56, 305–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hibi, S. (1983). Rhythm perception in repetitive sound sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan (E), 4, 83–95. Hibi, S. (1983). Rhythm perception in repetitive sound sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan (E), 4, 83–95.
go back to reference Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2004). When two limbs are weaker than one: Sensorimotor syncopation with alternating hands. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1085–1101. Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2004). When two limbs are weaker than one: Sensorimotor syncopation with alternating hands. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1085–1101.
go back to reference Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2005). Staying offbeat: Sensorimotor syncopation with structured and unstructured auditory sequences. Psychological Research, 69, 292–309.PubMedCrossRef Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2005). Staying offbeat: Sensorimotor syncopation with structured and unstructured auditory sequences. Psychological Research, 69, 292–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S., DelColle, J. D., & Schöner, G. (1990). Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 139–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kelso, J. A. S., DelColle, J. D., & Schöner, G. (1990). Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 139–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Large, E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music. Human Movement Science, 19, 527–566.CrossRef Large, E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music. Human Movement Science, 19, 527–566.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W. (2001). Periodicity, pattern formation, and metric structure. Journal of New Music Research, 30, 173–185.CrossRef Large, E. W. (2001). Periodicity, pattern formation, and metric structure. Journal of New Music Research, 30, 173–185.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Kolen, J. F. (1994). Resonance and the perception of musical meter. Connection Science, 6, 177–208.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Kolen, J. F. (1994). Resonance and the perception of musical meter. Connection Science, 6, 177–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26, 1–37.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26, 1–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., Fink, P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychological Research, 66, 3–17.PubMedCrossRef Large, E. W., Fink, P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychological Research, 66, 3–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. I. Timing and error corrections. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 463–473.PubMedCrossRef Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. I. Timing and error corrections. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 463–473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. II. Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 475–484.PubMedCrossRef Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. II. Stability analysis, error estimation and simulations. Biological Cybernetics, 70, 475–484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mates, J., Radil, T., Müller, U., & Pöppel, E. (1994). Temporal integration in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 332–340.CrossRef Mates, J., Radil, T., Müller, U., & Pöppel, E. (1994). Temporal integration in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 332–340.CrossRef
go back to reference McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (2003). Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: A comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1102–1125.PubMedCrossRef McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (2003). Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: A comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1102–1125.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nagasaki, H. (1987a). Correlations of stress and timing in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 161–180.CrossRef Nagasaki, H. (1987a). Correlations of stress and timing in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 161–180.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagasaki, H. (1987b). Frequency dependence of rhythm in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 247–256.CrossRef Nagasaki, H. (1987b). Frequency dependence of rhythm in periodic tapping. Human Movement Science, 6, 247–256.CrossRef
go back to reference van Noorden, L., & Moelants, D. (1999). Resonance in the perception of musical pulse. Journal of New Music Research, 28, 43–66.CrossRef van Noorden, L., & Moelants, D. (1999). Resonance in the perception of musical pulse. Journal of New Music Research, 28, 43–66.CrossRef
go back to reference Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409–464. Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409–464.
go back to reference Penel, A., & Drake, C. (1998). Sources of timing variations in music performance: A psychological segmentation model. Psychological Research, 61, 12–32.CrossRef Penel, A., & Drake, C. (1998). Sources of timing variations in music performance: A psychological segmentation model. Psychological Research, 61, 12–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Penel, A., & Drake, C. (2004). Timing variations in music performance: Musical communication, perceptual compensation, and/or motor control? Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 545–562. Penel, A., & Drake, C. (2004). Timing variations in music performance: Musical communication, perceptual compensation, and/or motor control? Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 545–562.
go back to reference Peper, C. E., & Beek, P. J. (1998). Distinguishing between the effects of frequency and amplitude on interlimb coupling in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm. Experimental Brain Research, 118, 78–92.CrossRef Peper, C. E., & Beek, P. J. (1998). Distinguishing between the effects of frequency and amplitude on interlimb coupling in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm. Experimental Brain Research, 118, 78–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Peper, C. E., Beek, P. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1995). Coupling strength in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm. Human Movement Science, 14, 217–245.CrossRef Peper, C. E., Beek, P. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1995). Coupling strength in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm. Human Movement Science, 14, 217–245.CrossRef
go back to reference Peters, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychological Research, 51, 38–42.CrossRef Peters, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychological Research, 51, 38–42.CrossRef
go back to reference Pressing, J. (1998). Error correction processes in temporal pattern production. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 63–101.PubMedCrossRef Pressing, J. (1998). Error correction processes in temporal pattern production. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 63–101.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pressing, J. (1999). The referential dynamics of cognition and action. Psychological Review, 106, 714–747.CrossRef Pressing, J. (1999). The referential dynamics of cognition and action. Psychological Review, 106, 714–747.CrossRef
go back to reference Pressing, J., & Jolley-Rogers, G. (1997). Spectral properties of human cognition and skill. Biological Cybernetics, 76, 339–347.PubMedCrossRef Pressing, J., & Jolley-Rogers, G. (1997). Spectral properties of human cognition and skill. Biological Cybernetics, 76, 339–347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1997). Acoustics, perception, and production of legato articulation on a computer-controlled grand piano. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1878–1890.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (1997). Acoustics, perception, and production of legato articulation on a computer-controlled grand piano. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1878–1890.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1999a). Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 529–548. Repp, B. H. (1999a). Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 529–548.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1999b). Control of expressive and metronomic timing in pianists. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 145–164.PubMed Repp, B. H. (1999b). Control of expressive and metronomic timing in pianists. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 145–164.PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1999c). Relationships between performance timing, perception of timing perturbations, and perceptual-motor synchronization in two Chopin preludes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 188–203.CrossRef Repp, B. H. (1999c). Relationships between performance timing, perception of timing perturbations, and perceptual-motor synchronization in two Chopin preludes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 188–203.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2001a). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2001a). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2001b). Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Human Movement Science, 20, 277–312.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2001b). Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Human Movement Science, 20, 277–312.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002a). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410–430.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2002a). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410–430.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002b). Effects of metrical structure on phase resetting in sensorimotor synchronization. In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Eds.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 572–574). Sydney: Causal Productions (CD-ROM). Repp, B. H. (2002b). Effects of metrical structure on phase resetting in sensorimotor synchronization. In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Eds.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 572–574). Sydney: Causal Productions (CD-ROM).
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002c). Phase correction following a perturbation in sensorimotor synchronization depends on sensory information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 291–298.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (2002c). Phase correction following a perturbation in sensorimotor synchronization depends on sensory information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 291–298.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2002d). Phase correction in sensorimotor synchronization: Nonlinearities in voluntary and involuntary responses to perturbations. Human Movement Science, 21, 1–37.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2002d). Phase correction in sensorimotor synchronization: Nonlinearities in voluntary and involuntary responses to perturbations. Human Movement Science, 21, 1–37.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355–370.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355–370.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992. Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992.
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2004). Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization: Effects of intention, attention, and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 10. Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2004). Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization: Effects of intention, attention, and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 10.
go back to reference Schulze, H.-H. (1992). The error correction model for the tracking of a random metronome: Statistical properties and an empirical test. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition (pp. 275–286). Dordrecht: Kluwer. Schulze, H.-H. (1992). The error correction model for the tracking of a random metronome: Statistical properties and an empirical test. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition (pp. 275–286). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
go back to reference Schulze, H.-H., Cordes, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). Keeping synchrony while tempo changes: Accelerando and ritardando. Music Perception, 22, 461–477.CrossRef Schulze, H.-H., Cordes, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). Keeping synchrony while tempo changes: Accelerando and ritardando. Music Perception, 22, 461–477.CrossRef
go back to reference Semjen, A., Schulze, H.-H., & Vorberg, D. (1992). Temporal control in the coordination between repetitive tapping and periodic external stimuli. In C. Auxiette, C. Drake, & C. Gérard (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth Rhythm Workshop: Rhythm Perception and Production (pp. 73–78). Bourges, France: Imprimérie Municipale. Semjen, A., Schulze, H.-H., & Vorberg, D. (1992). Temporal control in the coordination between repetitive tapping and periodic external stimuli. In C. Auxiette, C. Drake, & C. Gérard (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth Rhythm Workshop: Rhythm Perception and Production (pp. 73–78). Bourges, France: Imprimérie Municipale.
go back to reference Semjen, A., Schulze, H.-H., & Vorberg, D. (2000). Timing precision in continuation and synchronization tapping. Psychological Research, 63, 137–147.PubMedCrossRef Semjen, A., Schulze, H.-H., & Vorberg, D. (2000). Timing precision in continuation and synchronization tapping. Psychological Research, 63, 137–147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thaut, M. H., Miller, R. A., & Schauer, L. M. (1998). Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: phase vs period correction. Biological Cybernetics, 79, 241–250.PubMedCrossRef Thaut, M. H., Miller, R. A., & Schauer, L. M. (1998). Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: phase vs period correction. Biological Cybernetics, 79, 241–250.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thaut, M. H., Tian, B., & Azimi-Sadjadi, M. R. (1998). Rhythmic finger tapping to cosine-wave modulated metronome sequences: Evidence of subliminal entrainment. Human Movement Science, 17, 839–863.CrossRef Thaut, M. H., Tian, B., & Azimi-Sadjadi, M. R. (1998). Rhythmic finger tapping to cosine-wave modulated metronome sequences: Evidence of subliminal entrainment. Human Movement Science, 17, 839–863.CrossRef
go back to reference Todd. N. P. McA., Lee, C. S., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2002). A sensorimotor theory of temporal tracking and beat induction. Psychological Research, 66, 26–39. Todd. N. P. McA., Lee, C. S., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2002). A sensorimotor theory of temporal tracking and beat induction. Psychological Research, 66, 26–39.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In J, Requin (Ed.), Attention and Performance VII (pp. 535–556). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In J, Requin (Ed.), Attention and Performance VII (pp. 535–556). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (pp. 390–406). New York: New York Academy of Sciences (Annals, Vol. 423). Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (pp. 390–406). New York: New York Academy of Sciences (Annals, Vol. 423).
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Schulze, H.-H. (2002). A two-level timing model for synchronization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 56–87.CrossRef Vorberg, D., & Schulze, H.-H. (2002). A two-level timing model for synchronization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 56–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, vol. 2 (pp. 181–262). London: Academic. Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action, vol. 2 (pp. 181–262). London: Academic.
go back to reference Vos, P. G., & Helsper, E. L. (1992). Tracking simple rhythms: In-phase versus anti-phase performance. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition: Towards bridging the gap (pp. 287–299). Dordrecht: Kluwer. Vos, P. G., & Helsper, E. L. (1992). Tracking simple rhythms: In-phase versus anti-phase performance. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition: Towards bridging the gap (pp. 287–299). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
go back to reference Wimmers, R. H., Beek, P. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1992). Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling. Human Movement Science, 11, 217–226.CrossRef Wimmers, R. H., Beek, P. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1992). Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling. Human Movement Science, 11, 217–226.CrossRef
go back to reference Wohlschläger, A., & Koch, R. (2000). Synchronization error: an error in time perception. In P. Desain & L. Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm perception and production (pp. 115–128). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. Wohlschläger, A., & Koch, R. (2000). Synchronization error: an error in time perception. In P. Desain & L. Windsor (Eds.), Rhythm perception and production (pp. 115–128). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Metagegevens
Titel
Multiple temporal references in sensorimotor synchronization with metrical auditory sequences
Auteur
Bruno H. Repp
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0067-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2008

Psychological Research 1/2008 Naar de uitgave