Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2008

01-07-2008 | Original Article

Multiple response codes play specific roles in response selection and inhibition under task switching

Auteurs: Ronald Hübner, Michel D. Druey

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Several task-switch studies show that response category repetition is favorable on task repetition trials, but disadvantageous on task switch trials. In the present study we investigated how this interaction depends on the type and number of involved response categories. In a dual-task number-categorization experiment, subjects had to respond to tasks T1 and T2 with one of the two fingers of their left and right hand, respectively. For one group of participants, the use of spatial response categories, and for another group the use of finger-type categories was induced. It turned out that the interaction between task switching and response category repetition was clearly related to the induced response categories, but at the same time, the spatial categories nevertheless also affected response selection in the finger-type group. However, these two effects were additive. This shows that multiple response codes can simultaneously be involved in response selection, but that they affect performance differentially.
Voetnoten
1
Clearly, this conclusion is based on non-significant interactions, and, thus, strongly depends on the statistical power of the applied tests. However, given F-values of 0.084 (P = .775), and 0.045 (P = 0.834), it seems reasonable not to expect a different conclusion if the statistical power would be increased.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Campbell K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362.CrossRef Campbell K. C., & Proctor, R. W. (1993). Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1345–1362.CrossRef
go back to reference De Jong R., Liang C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef De Jong R., Liang C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene S., Bossini S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene S., Bossini S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Druey, M. D., & Hübner, R. (2007a). How stimulus features determine the type of response categories involved in response selection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (in press). Druey, M. D., & Hübner, R. (2007a). How stimulus features determine the type of response categories involved in response selection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Druey, M. D., & Hübner, R. (2007b). Response inhibition under task switching: Its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation. Psychological Research, under revision. Druey, M. D., & Hübner, R. (2007b). Response inhibition under task switching: Its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation. Psychological Research, under revision.
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more than the sum of its parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 216–228.PubMedCrossRef Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more than the sum of its parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 216–228.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In: B. Hommel, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility 28 (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In: B. Hommel, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility 28 (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., & Druey, M. D. (2006). Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? Psychological Research, 70, 245–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640–653.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hübner, R., & Lehle, C. (2007). Strategies of flanker co-processing in single and dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 103–123.PubMedCrossRef Hübner, R., & Lehle, C. (2007). Strategies of flanker co-processing in single and dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 103–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.CrossRef Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1260–1280.PubMedCrossRef Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1260–1280.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712.PubMedCrossRef Lien, M. C., Schweickert, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 692–712.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362–372.PubMedCrossRef Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362–372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 149–165.PubMedCrossRef Miller, J., & Alderton, M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 149–165.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 358–377.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Wang, D.-Y. D., & Pick, D. F. (2004). Stimulus-response compatibility with wheel-rotation responses: Will an incompatible response coding be used when a compatible coding is possible? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 841–847. Proctor, R. W., Wang, D.-Y. D., & Pick, D. F. (2004). Stimulus-response compatibility with wheel-rotation responses: Will an incompatible response coding be used when a compatible coding is possible? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 841–847.
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (1997). A dual-route processing architecture for stimulus-response correspondence effects. In: B. Hommel, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility 11 (pp. 119–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Ridderinkhof, K. R. (1997). A dual-route processing architecture for stimulus-response correspondence effects. In: B. Hommel, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility 11 (pp. 119–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response–response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response–response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2006). Response-based strengthening in task-shifting: Evidence from shift effects produced by errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 517–534.PubMedCrossRef Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2006). Response-based strengthening in task-shifting: Evidence from shift effects produced by errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 517–534.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMedCrossRef Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wenke, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2005). The influence of task instruction on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception and Performance, 31, 803–819.CrossRef Wenke, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2005). The influence of task instruction on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception and Performance, 31, 803–819.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Multiple response codes play specific roles in response selection and inhibition under task switching
Auteurs
Ronald Hübner
Michel D. Druey
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0118-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2008

Psychological Research 4/2008 Naar de uitgave