Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2018

30-01-2017 | Original Article

Motor sequence learning and the effect of context on transfer from part-to-whole and from whole-to-part

Auteurs: Zipi Rhein, Eli Vakil

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present study attempts to characterize the contextual conditions (i.e., addition versus omission of elements) that enable or prevent transfer of an acquired skill. The effect of learning and transfer from part-to-whole and from whole-to-part was studied with the serial reaction time (SRT) task. In this study, two alternative sequences of the SRT task were utilized, a short (i.e., ‘part’) sequence consisting of six elements (ADBACD), and a long (i.e., ‘whole’) one consisting of 12 elements (BDCADBACDABC) in which the short sequence was embedded. Three groups participated in the study: one was trained with the ‘whole’ sequence and two with the ‘part’ sequence (differing in the number of initial training trials performed), for six blocks followed by a random block. Then, for an additional block, each group was divided into two subgroups, one which continued to practice the same sequence, while the other was transferred to the alternate sequence (i.e., ‘part-to-whole’ and ‘whole-to-part’). Results indicated that the group that first practiced the ‘whole’ and then the ‘part’ sequence showed full transfer, while the other group showed only partial transfer from the ‘part’ to ‘whole’ sequence. The findings of the present study are inconsistent with Thorndike’s principle of identical elements, and, instead, indicate that full transfer is enabled in spite of certain contextual changes (i.e., omissions), but only partial transfer is enabled when other changes are applied (i.e., additions).
Literatuur
go back to reference Abrahamse, E. L., Ruitenberg, M. F. L., De Kleine, E., & Verwey, W. B. (2013). Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–16.CrossRef Abrahamse, E. L., Ruitenberg, M. F. L., De Kleine, E., & Verwey, W. B. (2013). Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Abrahamse, E. L., & Verwey, W. B. (2008). Context dependent learning in the serial RT task. Psychological Research, 72, 397–404. Abrahamse, E. L., & Verwey, W. B. (2008). Context dependent learning in the serial RT task. Psychological Research, 72, 397–404.
go back to reference Adi-Japha, E., Karni, A., Parnes, A., Loewenschuss, I., & Vakil. E. (2008). A shift in task routines during the learning of a motor skill: group averaged data may mask critical phases in the individuals’ acquisition of skilled performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 1544–1551.CrossRef Adi-Japha, E., Karni, A., Parnes, A., Loewenschuss, I., & Vakil. E. (2008). A shift in task routines during the learning of a motor skill: group averaged data may mask critical phases in the individuals’ acquisition of skilled performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 1544–1551.CrossRef
go back to reference Ahlum-Heath, M. E., & Di Vesta, F. J. (1986). The effect of conscious controlled verbalization cognitive strategy on transfer in problem solving. Memory and cognition, 14(3), 281–285.CrossRefPubMed Ahlum-Heath, M. E., & Di Vesta, F. J. (1986). The effect of conscious controlled verbalization cognitive strategy on transfer in problem solving. Memory and cognition, 14(3), 281–285.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.CrossRef Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.CrossRef
go back to reference Averbeck, B. B., Sohn, J. W., & Lee, D. (2006). Activity in prefrontal cortex during dynamic selection of action sequences. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 276–282.CrossRefPubMed Averbeck, B. B., Sohn, J. W., & Lee, D. (2006). Activity in prefrontal cortex during dynamic selection of action sequences. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 276–282.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brown, R. M., & Robertson, E. M. (2007). Off-line processing: reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10468–10475.CrossRefPubMed Brown, R. M., & Robertson, E. M. (2007). Off-line processing: reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10468–10475.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Clawson, D. M., Healy, A. F., Ericsson, K. A., & Bourne, L. E. Jr. (2001). Retention and transfer of Morse code reception skill by novices: Part-whole training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 129–142.PubMed Clawson, D. M., Healy, A. F., Ericsson, K. A., & Bourne, L. E. Jr. (2001). Retention and transfer of Morse code reception skill by novices: Part-whole training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 129–142.PubMed
go back to reference Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual - motor skill learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of human learning. New York: Academic Press. Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual - motor skill learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of human learning. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Fontana, F. E., Mazzarado, O., Furtado, O., & Gallagher, J. D. (2009). Whole and part practice: A meta-analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109, 517–530.CrossRefPubMed Fontana, F. E., Mazzarado, O., Furtado, O., & Gallagher, J. D. (2009). Whole and part practice: A meta-analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109, 517–530.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jiménez, L. (2008). Taking patterns for chunks: is there any evidence of chunk learning in continuous serial reaction-time tasks? Psychological Research, 72, 387–396.CrossRef Jiménez, L. (2008). Taking patterns for chunks: is there any evidence of chunk learning in continuous serial reaction-time tasks? Psychological Research, 72, 387–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Jiménez, L., Méndez, A., Pasquali, A., Abrahamse, E. L., & Verwey, W. B. (2011). Chunking by colors: Assessing discrete learning in a continuous serial reaction-time task. Acta Psychologica, 137, 318–329.CrossRef Jiménez, L., Méndez, A., Pasquali, A., Abrahamse, E. L., & Verwey, W. B. (2011). Chunking by colors: Assessing discrete learning in a continuous serial reaction-time task. Acta Psychologica, 137, 318–329.CrossRef
go back to reference Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 861–868.CrossRef Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 861–868.CrossRef
go back to reference Korman, M., Raz, N., Flash, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 100, 12492–12497.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Korman, M., Raz, N., Flash, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 100, 12492–12497.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Mattoon, J. S. (1994). Designing instructional simulations: Effects of instructional control and type of training task on developing display-interpretation skills. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 4, 189–209.CrossRef Mattoon, J. S. (1994). Designing instructional simulations: Effects of instructional control and type of training task on developing display-interpretation skills. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 4, 189–209.CrossRef
go back to reference Muller, B. (1999). Use specificity of cognitive skills: Evidence for production rules? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 191–207. Muller, B. (1999). Use specificity of cognitive skills: Evidence for production rules? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 191–207.
go back to reference Naylor, G., & Briggs, J. (1963). Effects of task complexity and task organization on the relative efficiency of part and whole training methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 217–224.CrossRefPubMed Naylor, G., & Briggs, J. (1963). Effects of task complexity and task organization on the relative efficiency of part and whole training methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 217–224.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Park, J. H., Wilde, H., & Shea, C. H. (2004). Part-whole practice of movement sequences. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 51–61.CrossRef Park, J. H., Wilde, H., & Shea, C. H. (2004). Part-whole practice of movement sequences. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 51–61.CrossRef
go back to reference Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., & Hallett, M. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annual Neurology, 34, 594–602.CrossRef Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., & Hallett, M. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annual Neurology, 34, 594–602.CrossRef
go back to reference Rand, M. K., Hikosaka, O., Miyachi, S., Lu X., Nakamura, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Shimo, Y. (2000). Characteristics of sequential movements during early learning periods in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 131, 293–304.CrossRefPubMed Rand, M. K., Hikosaka, O., Miyachi, S., Lu X., Nakamura, K., Kitaguchi, K., & Shimo, Y. (2000). Characteristics of sequential movements during early learning periods in monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 131, 293–304.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594. Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594.
go back to reference Rozanov, S., Keren, O., & Karni, A. (2010). The specificity of memory for a highly trained finger movement sequence: Change the ending, change all. Brain Research, 1331, 80–87.CrossRefPubMed Rozanov, S., Keren, O., & Karni, A. (2010). The specificity of memory for a highly trained finger movement sequence: Change the ending, change all. Brain Research, 1331, 80–87.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Abrahamse, E. L., De Kleine, E., & Verwey, W. B. (2012a). Context-dependent motor skill: Perceptual processing in memory-based sequence production. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 31–40. Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Abrahamse, E. L., De Kleine, E., & Verwey, W. B. (2012a). Context-dependent motor skill: Perceptual processing in memory-based sequence production. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 31–40.
go back to reference Ruitenberg, M. F. L., De Kleine, E., Van der Lubbe, R. H., Verwey, W. B., & Abrahamse, E. L. (2012b). Context-dependent motor skill and the role of practice. Psychological Research, 76, 812–820. Ruitenberg, M. F. L., De Kleine, E., Van der Lubbe, R. H., Verwey, W. B., & Abrahamse, E. L. (2012b). Context-dependent motor skill and the role of practice. Psychological Research, 76, 812–820.
go back to reference Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Verwey, W. B., & Abrahamse, E. L. (2015). What determines the impact of context on sequential action? Human Movement Science, 40, 298–314.CrossRef Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Verwey, W. B., & Abrahamse, E. L. (2015). What determines the impact of context on sequential action? Human Movement Science, 40, 298–314.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: a problem-based learning approach (4th edn.). Champaign: Human Kinetics. Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: a problem-based learning approach (4th edn.). Champaign: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Sosnik, R., Hauptmann, B., Karni, A., & Flash, T. (2004). When practice leads to co-articulation: The evolution of geometrically defined movement primitives. Experimental Brain Research, 156, 422–438.CrossRefPubMed Sosnik, R., Hauptmann, B., Karni, A., & Flash, T. (2004). When practice leads to co-articulation: The evolution of geometrically defined movement primitives. Experimental Brain Research, 156, 422–438.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Speelman, C. P., & Kirsner, K. (2001). Predicting transfer from training performance. Acta Psychological, 108, 247–281.CrossRef Speelman, C. P., & Kirsner, K. (2001). Predicting transfer from training performance. Acta Psychological, 108, 247–281.CrossRef
go back to reference Vakil, E., Karni, A., & Loewenschuss, I. (2002). Time delay and transfer in different stages of learning the tower of Hanoi. 43rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society. Vakil, E., Karni, A., & Loewenschuss, I. (2002). Time delay and transfer in different stages of learning the tower of Hanoi. 43rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society.
go back to reference Verwey, W. B. (2003). Effect of sequence length on the execution of familiar keying sequences: Lasting segmentations and preparation? Journal of Motor and Behavior, 35, 343–354.CrossRef Verwey, W. B. (2003). Effect of sequence length on the execution of familiar keying sequences: Lasting segmentations and preparation? Journal of Motor and Behavior, 35, 343–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Verwey, W. B. (2003a). Processing modes and parallel processors in producing familiar keying sequences. Psychological Research, 67, 106–122. Verwey, W. B. (2003a). Processing modes and parallel processors in producing familiar keying sequences. Psychological Research, 67, 106–122.
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., & Dronkert, Y. (1996). Practicing a structured continuous key-pressing task: Motor chunking or rhythm consolidation? Journal of Motor Behavior, 28, 71–79.CrossRef Verwey, W. B., & Dronkert, Y. (1996). Practicing a structured continuous key-pressing task: Motor chunking or rhythm consolidation? Journal of Motor Behavior, 28, 71–79.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1047–1060.PubMed Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1047–1060.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Motor sequence learning and the effect of context on transfer from part-to-whole and from whole-to-part
Auteurs
Zipi Rhein
Eli Vakil
Publicatiedatum
30-01-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0836-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2018

Psychological Research 3/2018 Naar de uitgave