Introduction
The notion that walking and thinking are linked goes back to the Peripatetic school of philosophy in ancient Greece and throughout history, there have been anecdotal claims that walking helps people think, solve problems and come up with creative ideas. Over the past couple of decades, some empirical evidence for these claims has been obtained.
Creativity has been proposed to involve two main processes: divergent and convergent thinking (Guilford,
1967). Divergent thinking, which is the focus of this study, is the ability to come up with new ideas or solutions to a single problem. Convergent thinking, on the other hand, involves the generation of a single novel solution by bringing together different concepts or problems. Two of the most commonly used tests are Guilford’s alternate uses test or AUT (Guilford,
1967) for divergent thinking and Mednick’s remote association test or RAT (Mednick,
1962) for convergent thinking. In the AUT, participants are asked to come up with alternate uses for everyday objects and in the RAT, they are required to find a commonality between three given words. Although these tests assess specific processes, they have been said to be good indicators for creative potential (Runco & Acar,
2012). With regard to the AUT, there are several sub-scores, such as fluency, flexibility and originality. In this study we use fluency and flexibility sub-scores of the AUT, which is defined by the number of valid responses generated and the number of categories the responses can be assigned to, respectively, to characterize divergent thinking.
Concerning large body movements, walking has been experimentally associated with better performance in divergent thinking (Kuo & Yeh,
2016; Leung et al.,
2012; Oppezzo & Schwartz,
2014; Zhou et al.,
2017). For instance, Oppezzo and Schwartz (
2014) found that people performed better in a divergent thinking task while walking as opposed to sitting. The same authors also found that there was a residual effect of walking by showing that performance continued to be enhanced when people performed the sitting condition after the walking condition.
Specifically, not just walking, but free walking has been shown to have the most benefit for divergent thinking. Kuo and Yeh (
2016) showed that performance in the AUT improved during unrestrained or free walking as opposed to walking in a prescribed rectangular path. In the same study, the authors tested whether the effect was due to the path itself by including a condition, where participants had to walk in a path which was generated during another participant’s free walking condition. They found that performance improved only during the actual free walking and thus concluded that it is the freedom to move and not the characteristics of the path that has an influence.
Although unrestricted walking in particular seems to have an effect, to our knowledge, no study has tested if this effect pertains to motor restriction in general. However, studies have suggested that free walking as well as other fluid movements can form an association of bodily states and abstract concepts and thereby improve divergent thinking (Kuo & Yeh,
2016; Leung et al.,
2012; Slepian & Ambady,
2012,
2014). Slepian and Ambady (
2012), for instance, found that AUT fluency scores were higher when participants made fluid movement with the hand while drawing as opposed to when they made non-fluid movements (by drawing straight lines). The authors argue that fluid movements could be linked to fluid thought processes. A similar argument was made by Kuo and Yeh (
2016), namely, that walking/moving freely could activate the idea of mind wandering.
Albeit less, there also is evidence for a link between creativity and eye-related movements, namely, blinks. Eye blinks are of three types: reflexive, spontaneous or voluntary. Reflexive blinks occur as automatic responses to startling external stimuli, such as an air puff. Voluntary blinks occur when someone is explicitly asked to make a blink. Spontaneous blinking, which is the focus of our study and has been found to be linked with divergent thinking (Akbari Chermahini & Hommel,
2010; Ueda et al.,
2016), is blinking that occurs unconsciously and not as a reflexive response. These blinks occur about 10–15 times a minute (Burr,
2005; Doughty,
2001; Kaminer et al.,
2011). Although they serve to moisten the eyes, their frequency is much higher than what is required just for that purpose (Kaminer et al.,
2011). The purpose of these surplus blinks is not completely decoded, but they have been shown to reflect cognitive processes (Brych & Händel,
2020; Brych et al.,
2020,
2021; Fogarty & Stern,
1989; Goldstein et al.,
1992). One study by Akbari Chermahini and Hommel (
2010) found that participants who had a comparably moderate baseline eye blink rate, as identified as the centre of the distribution of blink rates for this specific study, had higher scores on the AUT compared to subjects showing blink rates at the left and right side of the distribution. Another study, specifically focussing on blinking during the task as opposed to during a baseline period, found a positive correlation between blink rate and AUT scores (Ueda et al.,
2016).
Despite this sparse evidence from directly testing for creativity, there is rather strong experimental indication that cognitive aspects that might be applied during creative thinking (Madore et al.
2015) are linked to eye movements. When people engage in imagination and memory retrieval, they tend to make more saccades and blinks (Salvi & Bowden,
2016). Although eye blinks are not specifically investigated, a number of studies have found a link between eye movements in general and memory retrieval (Damiano & Walther,
2019; Johansson & Johansson,
2014; Johansson et al.,
2012; Lenoble et al.,
2019). In fact, not allowing any eye movements worsens performance in these tasks (Damiano & Walther,
2019; Johansson & Johansson,
2014). Moreover, Johansson and Johanssonn (
2014) also showed that asking participants to make eye movements that were incongruent with the position of the objects during a visuospatial memory task, worsened performance as opposed to congruent movements. Apart from visual memory, a study by Lenoble et al. (
2019) showed that eye movements could also aid in retrieving autobiographical memories. These authors found memories retrieved while making free eye movements were more detailed and faster than those retrieved during fixation. Moreover, Salvi and Bowden (
2016) in their paper, mention how it has been anecdotally suggested that closing one’s eyes is thought to reflect the process of disengaging from the outside world and concentrating on inner thoughts. Indeed, we recently showed that blinks could directly reflect cognitive processes independent of external sensory input (Brych & Händel,
2020; Brych et al.,
2021; Murali & Händel,
2021).
Therefore, while there is strong evidence that eye movements are related to cognitive processes and some evidence that they are linked to creativity, it is not clear if eye movements play a role in cognition or if they are linked via a common process. Akbari Chermahini and Hommel (
2010) suggested that the link between eye blinks and divergent thinking performance could be mediated by dopamine levels. Dopamine has been shown to play a role during divergent thinking (Kulisevsky et al.,
2009; Zabelina et al.,
2016) and at the same time is correlated with eye blink rates (Bologna et al.,
2012; Karson,
1983; Taylor et al.,
1999). Blink rate has been shown to increase following the administration of dopamine agonists (Karson et al.,
1984) and decrease following dopamine antagonists (Blin et al.,
1990; Strakowski & Sax,
1998; Strakowski et al.,
1996). However, a few recent studies have found a lack of evidence for the positive relationship between dopamine and eye blinks in healthy human adults (Dang et al.,
2017; Sescousse et al.,
2018). Overall, when it comes to eye blinks, dopamine and creativity, the relationship is hard to interpret, since there are several neuronal processes modulated by dopamine, which could influence blinking as well as creativity. For example, a link might be realized via the default mode network. Blinks activate the default mode network (Nakano et al.,
2013), which has been shown to play a role in creativity (Beaty et al.,
2014; Kühn et al.,
2014) as well as to be linked to dopamine (Dang et al.,
2012; Nagano-Saito et al.,
2009).
Using three experiments, we tested the influence of body movement and motor restriction on the AUT and additionally tested the relationship between blinks and performance during the different conditions and the interaction between blinks and other body movements. In experiment 1, we focused on the interaction between walking and blinking. When investigating specific movements, it is important to consider that many body movements interact. As shown by us and by others, spontaneous blink rate increases can be linked to movements of the mouth during speech (Brych et al.,
2020; von Cramon & Schuri,
1980). In addition, walking goes hand in hand with an increased blink rate (Cao et al.,
2020). Importantly, this association is not dependent on the visual input, as it persists even during absolute darkness, suggesting a link deeply integrated into the system. We, therefore, set out to better understand the influence of different movement states on creativity, while additionally assessing the interactions between large movements and blinks. The purpose of experiment 1 was to (a) replicate previous findings on walking and blinking on divergent thinking, (b) to differentiate the effects for the different phases of the creativity task (baseline, thinking, responding), and (c) to understand if effects of blinking and walking are additive or independent from each other.
The aim of experiment 2 was to understand if the mere execution of motor output is the relevant factor or if it rather is the specific instruction/ body state, which introduces the effect. As detailed earlier, particularly walking without restriction to the path, has been shown to improve divergent thinking. Free eye movements, although not been studied in the context of creativity, have been shown to benefit memory. We wanted to understand if the positive effect on cognition extends to other movement states and hence, compared performance in the AUT not only during free and restricted walking, but additionally during free and restricted sitting. The aim of experiment 3 was to replicate the findings of experiment 2 and importantly test if eye blinks show a link to fluency depending on the level of restriction.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.