Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2015

01-09-2015 | Original Article

Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation

Auteurs: Stefanie Schuch, Iring Koch

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Conflict adaptation can be measured by the “congruency sequence effect”, denoting the reduction of congruency effects after incongruent trials (where response conflict occurs) relative to congruent trials (without response conflict). Recently, it has been reported that conflict adaptation is larger in negative mood than in positive mood (van Steenbergen et al., Psychological Science 21:1629–1634, 2010). We conducted two experiments further investigating this important finding. Two different interference paradigms were applied to measure conflict adaptation: Experiment 1 was a Flanker task, Experiment 2 was a Stroop-like task. To get as pure a measure of conflict adaptation as possible, we minimized the influence of trial-to-trial priming effects by excluding all kinds of stimulus repetitions. Mood states were induced by presenting film clips with emotional content prior to the interference task. Three mood states were manipulated between subjects: amused, anxious, and sad. Across both interference paradigms, we consistently found conflict adaptation in negative, but not in positive mood. Taken together with van Steenbergen et al. (Psychological Science 21:1629–1634, 2010) findings, the results suggest that the negative-mood-triggered increase in conflict adaptation is a general phenomenon that occurs independently of the particular mood-induction procedure and interference paradigm involved.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The questionnaire data were also analyzed in 3 × 3 ANOVAs with Group (amused, anxious, sad) and time point (before mood induction, after mood induction, after RT experiment) as independent variables. For the STAI state scale, the ANOVA yielded a significant interaction, F(4,114) = 6.73, p < 0.01, as well as a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 17.62, p < 0.01, and no effect of mood group, F < 1. A preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” revealed a significant interaction, F(2,57) = 10.34, p < 0.01. The respective ANOVA on the PANAS negative affect scale also yielded a significant interaction, F(4,114) = 3.37, p = 0.01, as well as a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 8.88, p < 0.01, and no main effect of mood group, F < 1. The preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” revealed a significant interaction, F(2,57) = 5.02, p = 0.01. As to the PANAS positive affect scale, there was also a significant interaction, F(4,114) = 3.06, p = 0.02, a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 7.63, p < 0.01, and no effect of mood group, F < 1. The preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” did not reveal a significant interaction, F(2,57) = 1.45, p = 0.24.
 
2
For the STAI state scale, the 3 × 3 ANOVA yielded a significant interaction, F(4,114) = 7.11, p < 0.01, as well as a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 22.62, p < 0.01, and no effect of mood group, F < 1. A preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” revealed a significant interaction, F(2,57) = 9.53, p < 0.01. The respective ANOVA on the PANAS negative affect scale did not yield a significant interaction, F(4,114) = 1.61, p = 0.18; there was a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 14.40, p < 0.01, and no main effect of mood group, F < 1. The preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” did not reveal a significant interaction with mood group, F(2,57) = 1.53, p = 0.23. As to the PANAS positive affect scale, the 3 × 3 interaction was not significant, F(4,114) = 1.79, p = 0.14; there was a main effect of time point, F(2,114) = 4.68, p = 0.01, and no effect of mood group, F < 1. The preplanned contrast comparing the time points “before mood induction” and “after mood induction” revealed a significant interaction, F(2,57) = 5.36, p < 0.01.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.CrossRefPubMed Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Becker, M. W., & Leinenger, M. (2011). Attentional selection is biased toward mood-congruent stimuli. Emotion, 11, 1248–1254.CrossRefPubMed Becker, M. W., & Leinenger, M. (2011). Attentional selection is biased toward mood-congruent stimuli. Emotion, 11, 1248–1254.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulated function. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 356–366.CrossRef Botvinick, M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulated function. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 356–366.CrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.CrossRefPubMed Botvinick, M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.CrossRefPubMed Botvinick, M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Braem, S., Verguts, T., Roggeman, C., & Notebaert, W. (2012). Reward modulates adaptation to conflict. Cognition, 125, 324–332.CrossRefPubMed Braem, S., Verguts, T., Roggeman, C., & Notebaert, W. (2012). Reward modulates adaptation to conflict. Cognition, 125, 324–332.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (Eds.). (2007). The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (Eds.). (2007). The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012b). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 72, 94–98.CrossRef Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012b). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 72, 94–98.CrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 380–390.CrossRef Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 380–390.CrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.CrossRefPubMed Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.CrossRefPubMed Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective states trigger processes of assimilation and accommodation. In L. L. Martin & G. S. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: a user’s guidebook (pp. 85–98). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective states trigger processes of assimilation and accommodation. In L. L. Martin & G. S. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: a user’s guidebook (pp. 85–98). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Forgas, J. P. (Ed.). (2000). Feeling and thinking: the role of affect in social cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press. Forgas, J. P. (Ed.). (2000). Feeling and thinking: the role of affect in social cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General, 130, 436–452.CrossRef Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General, 130, 436–452.CrossRef
go back to reference Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 46–48.CrossRef Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 46–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Hengstler, M., Holland, R. W., van Steenbergen, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (2014). The influence of approach-avoidance motivational orientation on conflict adaptation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,. doi:10.3758/s13415-014-0295-6. (online first). Hengstler, M., Holland, R. W., van Steenbergen, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (2014). The influence of approach-avoidance motivational orientation on conflict adaptation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,. doi:10.​3758/​s13415-014-0295-6. (online first).
go back to reference Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2005). A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1095–1109.CrossRef Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2005). A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1095–1109.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hübner, R., Steinhauser, M., & Lehle, C. (2010). A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. Psychological Review, 117, 759–784.CrossRefPubMed Hübner, R., Steinhauser, M., & Lehle, C. (2010). A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. Psychological Review, 117, 759–784.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 260–280.CrossRefPubMed Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 260–280.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C. W., & Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Form der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42, 139–156. Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C. W., & Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Form der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42, 139–156.
go back to reference Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C.D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Angstinventar: Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisungen. (The State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory: Theoretical and Practical Issues) Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH. Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C.D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Angstinventar: Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisungen. (The State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory: Theoretical and Practical Issues) Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
go back to reference Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.PubMed Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.PubMed
go back to reference Melcher, T., Obst, K., Mann, A., Paulus, C., & Gruber, O. (2012). Antagonistic modulatory influences of negative affect on cognitive control: reduced and enhanced interference resolution capability after the induction of fear and sadness. Acta Psychologica, 139, 507–514.CrossRefPubMed Melcher, T., Obst, K., Mann, A., Paulus, C., & Gruber, O. (2012). Antagonistic modulatory influences of negative affect on cognitive control: reduced and enhanced interference resolution capability after the induction of fear and sadness. Acta Psychologica, 139, 507–514.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mitchell, R. L. C., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 617–629.CrossRefPubMed Mitchell, R. L. C., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 617–629.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increased shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time-lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3218–3227.CrossRefPubMed Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increased shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time-lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3218–3227.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. R., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York: Oxford University Press. Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. R., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Schuch, S., Werheid, K., & Koch, I. (2012). Flexible and inflexible tasks: asymmetric interference when switching between emotional expression, sex, and age classification of perceived faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 994–1005.CrossRefPubMed Schuch, S., Werheid, K., & Koch, I. (2012). Flexible and inflexible tasks: asymmetric interference when switching between emotional expression, sex, and age classification of perceived faces. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 994–1005.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
go back to reference Van Steenbergen, H. (2014). Affective modulation of cognitive control: a biobehavioral perspective. In G. Gendolla, M. Tops, S. Koole (Eds.), Biobehavioral Foundations of Self-Regulation. Heidelberg: Springer. Van Steenbergen, H. (2014). Affective modulation of cognitive control: a biobehavioral perspective. In G. Gendolla, M. Tops, S. Koole (Eds.), Biobehavioral Foundations of Self-Regulation. Heidelberg: Springer.
go back to reference Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2009). Reward counteracts conflict adaptation. Psychological Science, 20, 1473–1477.CrossRefPubMed Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2009). Reward counteracts conflict adaptation. Psychological Science, 20, 1473–1477.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2010). In the mood for adaptation: how affect regulates conflict-driven control. Psychological Science, 21, 1629–1634.CrossRefPubMed Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2010). In the mood for adaptation: how affect regulates conflict-driven control. Psychological Science, 21, 1629–1634.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., Hommel, B., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2014). Hedonic hotspots regulate cingulate-driven adaptation to cognitive demands. Cerebral Cortex,. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht416. (online first).PubMed Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., Hommel, B., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2014). Hedonic hotspots regulate cingulate-driven adaptation to cognitive demands. Cerebral Cortex,. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bht416. (online first).PubMed
go back to reference Van Steenbergen, H., Booij, L., Band, G. P. H., Hommel, B., & van der Does, A. J. W. (2012). Affective regulation of conflict-driven control in remitted depressive patients after acute tryptophan depletion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 280–286.CrossRef Van Steenbergen, H., Booij, L., Band, G. P. H., Hommel, B., & van der Does, A. J. W. (2012). Affective regulation of conflict-driven control in remitted depressive patients after acute tryptophan depletion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 280–286.CrossRef
go back to reference Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of Positive and Negative Affect: the PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.CrossRefPubMed Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of Positive and Negative Affect: the PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–580.CrossRef Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–580.CrossRef
go back to reference Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: a review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 3–47.CrossRef Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: a review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 3–47.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation
Auteurs
Stefanie Schuch
Iring Koch
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0602-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2015

Psychological Research 5/2015 Naar de uitgave