Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 19-03-2020 | ORIGINAL PAPER

Modification of Attentional Bias to Emotional Faces Following Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy in People with a Current Depression

Auteurs: Paweł Holas, Izabela Krejtz, Katarzyna Wisiecka, Marzena Rusanowska, John B. Nezlek

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 6/2020

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Objectives

Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) is an evidence-based treatment to prevent relapse in individuals with recurrent major depressive disorder (MDD). It is not clear if MBCT is an effective therapy for current depression, and it is not clear what mechanisms are responsible for the effectiveness of MBCT. Theoretically, MBCT is believed to modify the processing of emotional information and reduce cognitive vulnerability to depression; however, it is not clear if MBCT leads to normalization of attentional biases in depressed individuals. The aim of the current study was to determine if MBCT can modify some of the attentional biases underlying depression in MDD.

Methods

Participants were 53 individuals with diagnosis of current MDD. They were randomly assigned to either MBCT (n = 25) or wait list control group (n = 28) condition. Before and after the 8-week MBCT intervention participants completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CESD), they viewed slides presenting sad, angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions, and their eye movements were recorded during the viewing task.

Results

As expected, compared to participants in the control group, the CESD scores of participants who received MBCT decreased following treatment, their attention to sad faces decreased, and their attention to happy faces increased. Moreover, cross-lagged analysis suggested a causal link from changes in attentional bias to changes in depression.

Conclusions

We found that MBCT can modify the attentional processing of emotional facial stimuli and that attentional bias modification may translate into clinical improvement in currently depressed individuals.
Opmerkingen

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12671-020-01353-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most common and burdensome psychiatric disorders with a lifetime prevalence rate of approximately 16% (Kessler et al. 2009). According to the World Health Organization, MDD is currently the leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization 2017). Although many treatments have been proposed (Dimidjian et al. 2006), many of them have been found to have limited efficacy (Cuijpers et al. 2010), particularly in terms of preventing relapse.
MDD has a high relapse rate or recurrence after remission or recovery (Kessler et al. 2003). Up to 80% of individuals with a history of two episodes are likely to have another recurrence (Kupfer et al. 1996). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) was developed to reduce the risk of depressive relapse by attenuating the reactivation of dysfunctional thinking styles that represent one of the core mechanism by which CBT prevents depressive relapse (Segal et al. 2002). MBCT blends elements of cognitive behavioral therapy for depression (Teasdale et al. 2000) with mindfulness training (Segal et al. 2002), and MBCT has been found to reduce relapse in patients with recurrent MDD, defined as 3 or more previous episodes (e.g., Godfrin and van Heeringen 2010; Ma and Teasdale 2004; Teasdale et al. 2000). For a similar conclusion, see meta-analyses by Piet and Hougaard (2011) and Kuyken et al. (2016). Based on this evidence, MBCT has been recommended as a prophylactic treatment for recurrent MDD (i.e., National Institute for Clinical Excellence 2009); however, it is not clear if MBCT is effective in treating current MDD. Interestingly, despite the fact that MBCT was developed to prevent the recurrence of depression, patients who were currently depressed were excluded from the first seminal studies (Teasdale et al. 2000; Ma and Teasdale 2004).
Several researchers have raised concerns that depressive symptoms such as difficulty with concentration and intensity of negative thinking that may preclude the acquisition of attention regulation skills that are essential to MBCT (e.g., Segal et al. 2002). Regardless, a growing body of research has found that MBCT can reduce depressive symptoms per se (Finucane and Mercer 2006; Kenny and Williams 2007). Recently, several attempts have been made to use MBCT to treat the currently depressed (e.g., Van Aalderen et al. 2012), and this research suggests that MBCT could also be an effective treatment for current depression (Goldberg et al. 2018; Strauss et al. 2014).
MBCT was designed to target the cognitive reactivity (Segal et al. 2002), e.g., the ease with which dysphoric mood can reactivate constellation of negative thoughts and feelings that may lead to a depressive episode. Several laboratory studies have found that cognitive reactivity is a marker for vulnerability to relapse and recurrence of depression (Lau et al. 2004). For example, Segal et al. (2006) found that recovered depressed individuals tended to decline into depressogenic thinking patterns following the induction of sad moods, and those who exhibited greater reactivation of dysfunctional thinking styles in response to the sad mood induction were at the greatest risk of relapse over the following 18 months. Importantly for the current study, research has also found that attentional bias for sad faces is associated with significantly greater impairments in mood recovery following mood induction among individuals with MDD than it is among nonclinical subjects (Clasen et al. 2013). These findings suggest that change in depressive attentional bias may be linked to mechanisms that are the focus of MBCT.
Cognitive theories assume that attentional biases to negative emotional stimuli play important roles in the development and maintenance of depression (e.g., Clark and Beck 1999; Williams et al. 1988), and this assumption has been supported by a wealth of research (Armstrong and Olatunji 2012; Foland-Ross and Gotlib 2012). For example, eye tracking studies have consistently found that depression is associated with prolonged maintenance of gaze on negative information (Caseras et al. 2007; Eizenman et al. 2003; Kellough et al. 2009), and greater neglect of positive information (e.g., De Raedt and Koster 2010, see Peckham et al. 2010). Prolonged processing of negative information leads to more persistent sad mood (e.g., Disner et al. 2017) and has been found to be related to greater trait rumination (Donaldson et al. 2007; Owens and Gibb 2017) and increased daily rumination in depressed individuals (Holas et al. 2019).
“Mindfulness is defined as awareness that emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by moment” (Kabat-Zinn 2003, p. 145). Mindfulness thus involves awareness of the direction of attention and the ability to switch attention between thoughts, feelings, or bodily sensations and present stimuli (Bishop et al. 2004). Learning to stabilize attention through mindfulness exercises could be a way for people to recognize their cognitive reactivity and provide a basis for decentering and avoiding negative patterns of thinking (Williams and Kabat-Zinn 2011). In MBCT, this is achieved by instructing people to redirect their attention to the present moment, usually their own breathing. In turn, this can increase the voluntary deployment of attention (Segal et al. 2002).
Research on MBCT has found that decreases in dysfunctional thinking (rumination and worry) mediated treatment outcomes and relationships between cognitive reactivity and relapse risk (for meta-analysis see Gu et al. 2015 and van der Velden et al. 2015). Nevertheless, modifying attention to emotional stimuli as a therapeutic mechanism of MBCT as a treatment for current depression has not been examined that thoroughly (Alsubaie et al. 2017). Biases in attentional processing are a hallmark characteristic of depression and a vulnerability factor of depression (Joormann and Gotlib 2007; Koster et al. 2011), and so research on modifying attention within the confines of MBCT is clearly warranted.
Some research has been done on the impact of mindfulness training on attentional processes. This research has focused on processing at the level of specific components of attention (alerting, orienting, and executive attention), general attentional processing following short-term (like MBCT), and long-term meditation training in healthy participants (e.g., Anderson et al. 2007; Chambers et al. 2008; Chan and Woollacott 2007; Jha et al. 2007; Moore and Malinowski 2009; Slagter et al. 2007; Van den Hurk et al. 2010). Only a few studies have examined the effects of MBCT on attention in clinically depressed patients (e.g., De Raedt et al. 2012; Van den Hurk et al. 2012; Verhoeven et al. 2014). In addition, recent research suggests that mindful attention can lead to decentering by disengaging the self from imagined situations so that negative affective reactions do not develop (Lebois et al. 2015).
To our knowledge, only two published studies have evaluated changes in attentional biases to emotional information following MBCT (De Raedt et al. 2012; Verhoeven et al. 2014). These studies examined attentional processes in people who were formerly depressed but were in remission, and they measured reaction times manually. The majority of research on attentional biases in emotional disorders has measured attention using reaction time (RT) (e.g., Bar-Haim et al. 2007 for meta-analysis).
Although studies using RT to measure attention are informative, as discussed by Armstrong and Olatunji (2012), RT-based measures of attention are limited in potentially significant ways. For example, when measuring attention using RT, there is a possible confound between the effects of a stimulus and the time it takes to press a key to record a response. Another limitation is that RT is based on measures of individual points in time (“a snapshot”), which makes it problematic to capture the dynamics of attention.
Eye tracking is an alternative to RT that measures attention dynamically and provides accurate measures for assessing the time course of visual attention with no delay between attending to a stimulus and the measurement of attention. Because of these advantages, the use of eye-tracking to measure attention in studies of relationships between attentional processes and emotions has increased in recent years (e.g., Armstrong and Olatunji 2012). Given these advantages, in the present research, we measured attentional processes using eye-tracking.
The aims of the current study were to determine if MBCT can reduce depressive symptoms and if it can modify attentional biases to negative information underlying depression in currently depressed individuals. In addition, we wanted to examine possible causal relationships between the severity of depressive symptoms severity and attentional biases. We expected that MBCT would reduce depressive symptoms in MDD individuals and lead to normalization of attentional biases. Specifically, we expected a reduced attention (gaze duration) to sad and angry faces, and increased gaze duration to happy faces after MBCT, and no such effects in our control group. Furthermore, we hypothesized that modification of attentional bias to negative faces precludes decrease in depressive scores. We expected therefore, that gaze duration at pre-test would be related to CESD scores at post-test but that CESD scores at pre-test would not be related to gaze duration at post-test.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited via open calls posted on popular Internet portals. The calls invited people to participate in a study that involved MBCT training for 8 weeks and concerned how people feel and how they process information in their daily lives. A total of 692 people completed an online screening that consisted of background data and the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CESD, Radloff 1977). A total of 249 individuals were excluded on the basis following criteria: their CESD scores were below 20 (n = 66), or that they were younger than 25 (n = 150) or that they were taking psychiatric medications (n = 73) (some of them had multiply criteria). We then phone interviewed 273 people in order to qualify them in terms of 2 major criteria for depression (sadness or anhedonia for at least 2 weeks) to establish higher likelihood of meeting DSM criteria for current major depressive episode. Based on the phone interview, 154 people were excluded if they were currently receiving psychotherapy (n = 14); if they were suffering from a current or lifetime psychotic disorder, bipolar disorder, substance abuse problem; if they reported current suicidal tendencies (n = 15); or if they did not meet criteria for major depression (n = 63). Sixty-two were not interested in participation. We qualified 119 people who were then invited for a clinical interview in the laboratory. A total of 84 people came to a structured clinical interview (the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) version 5.0.0; Sheehan et al. 1998) conducted by a trained clinician. Three people were excluded from the study because they had comorbid diagnoses based on the MINI. Details how the subjects flowed through the study are presented in the CONSORT flow chart (Fig. 1).
The final sample consisted of 81 participants meeting DSM-IV criteria for MDD. In terms of ethnicity, all participants were white, which was not unusual given the demographics of the country in which the study was conducted. Participants were assigned to either MBCT group or wait list control condition. Twenty-four participants dropped out of the study after the pre-testing. The final sample consisted of 25 participants in the MBCT group (20 Female, Mage = 34.96, SD = 8.01) and 28 in control condition (19 Female, Mage = 34.25, SD = 8.75). The groups did not differ in terms of mean age and gender (ps = .76, .32, respectively). Summary statistics of previous episodes of depression and of comorbidity are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Description of previous episodes of depression and comorbidities
 
MBCT, N = 25
Control, N = 28
Age of first episode
N = 19, M = 23.5 (SD = 8), range = 12–41
N = 24, M = 23 (SD = 9), range 12–42
Previous episodes
  1
1
2
  2–5
7
14
  > 5
17
12
Diagnoses
  1 additional diagnosis
5 (20%)
7 (25%)
  2 or more additional diagnoses
10 (40%)
12 (43%)
  Dysthymia
5
1
  GAD
5
12
  Social phobia
8
6
  Panic attacks
3
2
  Specific phobia
2
3
  OCD
2
3
  Agoraphobia
3
0

Procedure

The study consisted of the following components: a laboratory session during which participants performed the eye-tracking task, followed by the maintenance of an online diary for 7 consecutive days (these data are not relevant to the current hypotheses and are not discussed in this paper), 8 weeks of MBCT training for experimental group or a waiting period for the control group, the maintenance of an online diary for 7 consecutive days (these data are not relevant to the current hypotheses and are not discussed in this paper), and then followed by a laboratory session during which post-training measures were collected, including the eye tracking task and the CESD.
Eye Tracking Task
Four-face task. In this task, based on Leyman et al. (2011), a series of 12 slides with four facial expressions (neutral, sad, angry, and happy) was shown to participants. We used expressions of 6 female and 6 male faces taken from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database (Goeleven et al. 2008). The four facial expressions of the same model were simultaneously presented for 10 s in one of 4 quadrants of the slide on a black background (see Fig. 2). The positions of expressions within each slide were randomized and fixed for all participants, and the order of the slides was random for each participant. Each trial began with a fixation cross in the middle of the screen for 1000 ms. Participants were told to look at each of the slides.
Intervention
We used a MBCT protocol developed by Segal et al. (2002). It consisted of 8 weekly sessions of 2.5 h each, with a 5-h day-long retreat between the 6th and 7th sessions. The MBCT training was provided by two trainers with extensive experience in meditation and MBCT. One was a psychiatrist and cognitive behavioral therapist with 8 years of experience guiding MBCT and MBSR groups. The second was a psychologist with 8 years of mindfulness-based intervention experience. Training groups consisted of up to 16 participants.

Measures

Eye Tracking
Presentation of the stimuli was controlled by SMI Experiment Center software with an eye-tracking device. The stimuli were presented on a 19′ monitor at a resolution of 1680 × 1050. Participants’ eye movements were recorded with SMI RED 120 Hz eye tracker, with a conventional 9-point calibration before each task. BeGaze SMI software was used to calculate standard eye movement indices. Fixations were defined as stable eye movements, within 1 degree of visual angle, lasting for at least 100 ms. Areas of interest were drawn around each face in each trial. For each of the face, the total gaze duration (GD), the dwell time spent fixating on the face, was calculated.
Self-Report of Depressive Symptoms
Before and after training, participants completed the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Radloff 1977). The CESD is a 20-item scale measuring symptoms of depression. Using a 4-point scale, with end points anchored 0 (rarely or none of the time, less than 1 day) and 3 (most of the time, 5–7 days) participants reported how often they experienced each of the symptoms during the previous week. The reliabilities for the scale were satisfactory at both pre- and post-testing (αs = .82 and .94 respectively).
Copies of all measures and the raw data are available at: https://​osf.​io/​dq64j/​

Data Analysis

Recording eye movements during the four-face task allowed us to examine if MBCT training modified participants’ attention to positive and negative faces. Our general expectation was that MBCT training would lead to a reduction of gaze toward negative faces and an increase in gaze duration to positive faces. We carried out a series of 2 × 2 × 4 mixed ANOVAs with training (MBCT vs. control) as a between-subject factor, testing session (pre-training vs. post-training) and face type (angry vs. happy vs. neutral vs. sad) as within-subject factors.
We also expected to find significant reduction in depression symptoms. We examined such changes using a 2 × 2 ANOVA with training (MBCT vs. control) as a between-subject factor and testing session (pre-training vs. post-training) as a within-subject factor. When appropriate, post-hoc tests of differences were performed using Bonferroni correction, and the results are reported with Greenhouse-Geisser correction whenever the sphericity assumption was violated.
Finally, we tested cross-lagged relationships between gaze duration and depressive symptoms. We examined if relative gaze duration at pre-training session predicted depressive symptoms measured at time post-training and if depressive symptoms at pre-training predicted relative gaze duration at the post-training. If one such lagged relationship is significant and the other is not, this suggests that one construct is a cause and the other is an effect. We expected that gaze duration at pre-test would be related to CESD scores at post-test but that CESD scores at pre-test would not be related to gaze duration at post-test.

Results

Eye-Tracking Measures of Focus on Emotional Facial Expressions

We measured visual attention using gaze duration (i.e., how long, in total, a person dwelled on a face) separately for angry, happy, neutral, and sad faces. Before the analyses, we examined distributions of raw gaze durations. As a result, we excluded the data from five participants from the analyses. The mean gaze durations of two participants from the MBCT group and two participants from the control group were 3SD or more above their group’s mean, and the mean gaze duration of one participant from the control group was 3SD below the group mean.
Similar to Owens and Gibb (2017) and Krejtz et al. (2018), we measured attentional bias by calculating gaze duration for specific facial expressions controlling for total gaze duration for all facial expressions. We used the relative score not an absolute gaze duration because it is a more direct measure of an attentional bias reflecting the difference between processing of happy, sad, angry, and neutral faces, and it is currently the standard procedure in studying attentional biases. We calculated relative scores for each of the facial expressions using a standard formula (e.g., Everaert et al. 2014; Owens and Gibb 2017). The total gaze duration for each type of face (target face in the formula below) was divided by the total viewing time for all facial expressions.
$$ {\mathrm{GD}}_{\mathrm{relative}}=\frac{{\mathrm{Gaze}\ \mathrm{Duration}}_{\mathrm{target}\ \mathrm{face}}}{{\mathrm{GD}}_{\mathrm{positive}}\kern0.5em +{\mathrm{GD}}_{\mathrm{angry}}+{\mathrm{GD}}_{\mathrm{neutral}}\kern0.5em +{\mathrm{GD}}_{\mathrm{sad}}} $$
This adjusted the gaze duration for a specific type of face for total of gaze durations on all four types of faces. A score of .25 indicates equal gaze distribution between the target face and other faces, scores above .25 indicate a preference for the target face (longer total viewing time), and scores below .25 indicate less attention toward the target face relative to other facial expressions.

Relative Gaze Duration

Descriptive statistics for gaze duration and relative gaze are presented in Table 2. We should note that analyses of absolute gaze duration produced results that were functionally equivalent to analyses of relative gaze duration. We present the results of analyses of relative gaze duration because the relative measure is reported more often than the absolute measure.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for gaze duration and relative gaze duration depending on face type and group
 
Gaze duration (ms)
Relative gaze duration
Face type/group
Pre-test mean (SE)
Post-test
Pre-test
Post-test
Happy
  MBCT
2243.25 (147.90)
2965.22 (220.75)
.274 (.015)
.349 (.017)
  Control
2065.04 (139.75)
2142.52 (208.59)
.250 (.014)
.254 (.017)
Sad
  MBCT
1981.24 (96.21)
1573.05 (117.20)
.235 (.009)
.198 (.012)
  Control
1932.33 (90.91)
1901.27 (110.74)
.243 (.010)
.250 (.011)
Angry
  MBCT
2059.63 (114.45)
1624.89 (106.82)
.239 (.011)
.207 (.011)
  Control
1730.12 (108.15)
1599.71 (100.94)
.217 (.010)
.214 (.010)
Neutral
  MBCT
2107.81 (139.67)
2157.09 (139.81)
.252 (.015)
.247 (.008)
  Control
2335.74 (131.98)
2184.23 (132.10)
.291 (.014)
.282 (.008)
The analyses of relative gaze duration produced a significant interaction effect among training, testing session, and face type (F(3, 138) = 4.02, p = .009, ɳp2 = .08). This interaction can be understood in two ways (see Table 2). First, the interaction can be interpreted as a result of the fact that for participants in the MBCT group the relative gaze time at happy faces significantly increased from pre- to post-training (F(1, 46) = 13.36, p < .001, ɳp2 = .225), whereas the relative time spent looking at sad and angry faces significantly decreased from pre- to post-training (F(1, 46) = 4.79, p = .034, ɳp2 = .094; F(1, 46) = 7.35 p = .009, ɳp2 = .138, respectively). In contrast, for the control group, there were no significant differences between pre- and post-training sessions for these measures (ps > .5).
Alternatively, the interaction can be interpreted as the result of the fact that for the MBCT group, at the pre-training session, there were no significant differences between time spent on different faces (F(3, 44) = 1.05, p = .38), whereas there were significant differences at post-training (F(3, 44) = 10.51, p < .001, ɳp2 = .417). After MBCT training, participants looked longer at happy faces compared to angry faces (p < .001), sad faces (p = .001), and neutral faces (p = .02). They also gazed longer at neutral faces compared to angry faces (p = .046), and sad faces (i = .02). In contrast, participants in the control group looked longer at neutral than angry faces at both pre-test (p = .009, F(3, 44) = 4.13, p = .012, ɳp2 = .220) and at post-test (p < .001, F(3, 44) = 8.62, p < .001, ɳp2 = .37).
Finally, we compared group differences at the pre-post training sessions. At the pre-training session, there were no group differences in relative gaze duration on different faces (for angry: F(1, 46) = 2.14, p = .15, ɳp2 = .044, sad: F(1, 46) < 1, happy: F(1, 46) = 1.46, p = .23, ɳp2 = .031, neutral: F(1, 46) = 3.58, p = .07, ɳp2 = .072). At the post-training session, MBCT group looked significantly longer at happy faces (F(1, 46) = 15.23, p < .001, ɳp2 = .249) and significantly less at sad faces and neutral faces than participants in the control condition (F(1, 46) = 10.04, p = .003, ɳp2 = .179; F(1, 46) = 10.35, p = .002, ɳp2 = .184; respectively).

Other Effects

These analyses also produced other significant effects (e.g., a main effect for face types). Given that these effects did neither test nor were they relevant to any of our hypotheses and many of the effects were qualified by the triple interaction, these effects are not described. They are presented in the online, supplemental materials.

Depressive Symptoms

The analysis of variance of CESD scores produced significant training × testing session interaction (F (1, 50) = 10.04, p = .003, ɳp2 = .167). At the pre-training session, there was no significant difference between the MBCT and the control group (M = 43.00, SE = 1.58, M = 40.67, SE = 1.52, respectively; F (1, 50) = 1.14, p = .292, ɳp2 = .022). In contrast, and as expected, at the post-training session the difference between the MBCT and the control group was statistically significant (F (1, 50) = 8.61, p = .005, ɳp2 = .147). The MBCT group had significantly lower CESD score (M = 18.52, SE = 2.43) than the control group (M = 28.41, SE = 2.34).

Cross-Lagged Analyses

We examined possible causal relationships between depressive symptoms (CESD scores) and gaze duration using cross-lagged panel analyses. In these analyses, CESD scores at post-test were regressed on gaze duration at pre-test, while simultaneously, gaze duration at post-test was regressed on CESD scores at pre-test. The logic of these analyses is that temporal precedence is a necessary (although not sufficient) condition for causes: Causes precede effects. A causal link from gaze duration to depression would be suggested by a significant relationship between duration at pre-test and CESD scores at post-test, whereas a causal link from depression to gaze duration would be suggested by a significant relationship between CESD scores at pre-test and duration at post-test.
These analyses were limited to participants in the MBCT condition, and each measure of gaze duration was analyzed separately. The analyses were conducted using MPlus (Muthén and Muthén 1998-2012), and the model used for these analyses is presented in Fig. 3. As can be seen from this figure, each post-test measure was regressed on both pre-test measures, and the error covariances between the two measures at pre-test and at post-test were modeled.
The lagged coefficients from these analyses are summarized in Table 3. As can be seen from these coefficients, the lags from duration of gaze on sad faces to CED scores and from duration of gaze on happy faces to CESD scores were significant. In contrast, neither of the lags from CESD scores to gaze duration on sad faces and on happy faces was significant. Such a pattern of results is consistent with a casual sequence in which changes in attentional focus lead to changes in depressive symptoms. There was no evidence for a causal sequence in which changes in depressive symptoms lead to changes in attentional focus.
Table 3
Lagged relationships between depression and relative gaze to emotional expressions
Time 1
Time 2
Coeff.
Coeff/se
p
Depression
Gaze sad
.15
< 1
ns
Gaze sad
Depression
.36
2.15
.032
Depression
Gaze happy
.05
< 1
ns
Gaze happy
Depression
− .49
2.88
.004
Depression
Gaze neutral
.17
< 1
ns
Gaze neutral
Depression
.11
< 1
ns
Depression
Gaze angry
.08
< 1
ns
Gaze angry
Depression
− .15
< 1
ns
Coeff. represents standardized coefficients

Discussion

Attentional biases are believed to play a key role in the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of depression (e.g., De Raedt and Koster 2010). Research has documented that attentional biases in depression consist of greater attention to negative information such as sad faces and a reduction in attention to positive stimuli such as happy faces (Armstrong and Olatunji 2012). In the current study, we evaluated the effectiveness of an 8-week MBCT training on modification of attentional biases to emotional faces in a sample of currently depressed individuals.
As expected, we found a significant decrease in CESD scores in the MBCT group, whereas there were no changes in the control group. This finding is consistent with the results of several studies that have shown that individuals who are currently depressed can benefit from MBCT (e.g., Van Aalderen et al. 2012), and it is consistent with the conclusions of meta-analyses showing the effectiveness of mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) in treating current depression (Goldberg et al. 2018; Strauss et al. 2014). Further, as expected, we found reduced gaze duration to negative faces, measured by a decrease in time spent viewing sad and angry faces for the MBCT group, whereas there were no such changes in the control group. Moreover, relative gaze time at happy faces significantly increased from pre- to post-training in MBCT group, and following MBCT individuals spent more time looking at happy faces than at angry and neutral faces.
These findings are consistent with how mindfulness and MBCT have been conceptualized. Mindfulness training teaches people to rely less on preconceived ideas, beliefs, and biases and to rely more on paying attention to all available information in the present moment (Bishop 2002). The enhancement of selective attention to attend to only one particular component of the present moment (such as breathing sensations) and the enhancement of sustained attention have been proposed as central practices in MBCT (Teasdale et al. 1995). They have also been proposed as essential elements of why mindfulness training changes people’s thinking and increases their well-being (Holas and Jankowski 2013). MBCT emphasizes attentional training and a focus on participants’ moment-to-moment experiences irrespective of affective valence or the presence of physical discomfort (Kocovski et al. 2015). This may help people to develop the ability to decenter from mental events such as negative thoughts or memories (Teasdale et al. 2002).
Surprisingly, very little work has been done to determine if MBCT leads to the modification of attentional biases to dysphoric information in current depression. The only published study of which we are aware that examined the effects of mindfulness training on attention to emotional faces (De Raedt et al. 2012) found that after MBCT, previously depressed individuals exhibited reduced facilitation of attention for negative faces. These findings were interpreted as indicating of an open attention toward all emotional information (De Raedt et al. 2012).
The results of the present study suggest more specific changes than those discussed by De Raedt et al., i.e., a general decrease in attention to negative faces and an increase to positive faces following MBCT. Our findings are consistent with the results of Verhoeven et al. (2014) who found that response times on an emotional Stroop task decreased after MBCT in the previously depressed. Such results suggest information related to depression may attract less attention following MBCT.
In addition to finding that MBCT led to changes in means of gaze duration and depressive symptoms among participants who received MBCT, we also found lagged relationships from gaze duration to depressive symptoms, whereas there were no significant lagged relationships from symptoms to gaze duration. Such a pattern is consistent with the assumption that changes in attentional focus lead to changes in depressive symptoms, or more broadly, with the assumption that attentional focus is more of a cause of depression (as posited by numerous theories and models, (e.g., Clark and Beck 1999; Koster et al. 2011) rather than a result of it.
Attentional improvement has been suggested as an important reason why MBCT is effective in depression in addition to change in mindfulness, rumination, worry, and self-compassion which have been more frequently investigated for mediation effects (Alsubaie et al. 2017; van der Velden et al. 2015). Attentional improvement posits that improved abilities to regulate attention and to disengage from depressogenic cognition translate into better treatment outcomes (van der Velden et al. 2015). Within the context of MBCT, Van den Hurk et al. (2012) also intended to verify the assumption linking clinical improvements to improvements in the self-regulation of attention; however, they did not find support for this. One reason for this may be that Van den Hurk et al. examined changes in attentional processing to neutral, not emotional information, as we did in the current study, and a second reason may be the weaknesses of manual reaction time that Van den Hurk et al. used to measure attention.

Limitations and Future Directions

Although the present study generally met our expectations, there were some limitations. First, there was no follow-up assessment, so we cannot know if the changes we found were maintained over time. In addition, we examined a limited set of emotions. Although happiness and sadness are central to the experience of depression, it would be useful to study attention to other emotions such as fear. Finally, the causal relationship between attentional biases and symptoms should be examined in experimental settings that provide a better basis for drawing casual inferences and with larger samples that would provide more statistical power than we had.
Although MBCT has been shown to be effective, an important limitation of the majority of studies, including the present study, is the absence of an active control condition (ACC). The results of studies comparing MBCT to ACCs provide mixed support for the effectiveness of MBCT in terms of depressive relapse and related symptoms (e.g., Williams et al. 2014; Shallcross et al. 2015). In addition, several authors, including founders of MBCT (Segal et al. 2002), have warned that certain characteristics of depression, such as aversive content of depressive thoughts and feelings and ruminative processing, may make it difficult for depressed people to engaging in and benefit from mindfulness based interventions. Clearly, more randomized control trials with ACCs are needed to draw firmer conclusion about the efficacy of MBCT to treat current depression.
Current models of depression suggest that cognitive biases reflect regulatory deficits in attentional control (e.g., Joormann 2010; Koster et al. 2011), the ability to exert top-down control of attention. Future studies are needed to determine if MBCT not only modifies processing tendencies but also modifies dysfunctions in attentional control that may be responsible for negative interpretations of emotional information.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the institutional review board for research involving human subjects of SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities.
Informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Electronic supplementary material

Literatuur
go back to reference Alsubaie, M., Abbott, R., Dunn, B., Dickens, C., Keil, T. F., Henley, W., & Kuyken, W. (2017). Mechanisms of action in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in people with physical and/or psychological conditions: a systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 55, 74–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.008.CrossRefPubMed Alsubaie, M., Abbott, R., Dunn, B., Dickens, C., Keil, T. F., Henley, W., & Kuyken, W. (2017). Mechanisms of action in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in people with physical and/or psychological conditions: a systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 55, 74–91. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​cpr.​2017.​04.​008.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
go back to reference De Raedt, R., Baert, S., Demeyer, I., Goeleven, E., Raes, A., Visser, A., et al. (2012). Changes in attentional processing of emotional information following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in people with a history of depression: towards an open attention for all emotional experiences. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(6), 612–620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9411-x.CrossRef De Raedt, R., Baert, S., Demeyer, I., Goeleven, E., Raes, A., Visser, A., et al. (2012). Changes in attentional processing of emotional information following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in people with a history of depression: towards an open attention for all emotional experiences. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(6), 612–620. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10608-011-9411-x.CrossRef
go back to reference Depression, W. H. O. (2017). Other common mental disorders: global health estimates (pp. 1–24). Geneva: World Health Organization. Depression, W. H. O. (2017). Other common mental disorders: global health estimates (pp. 1–24). Geneva: World Health Organization.
go back to reference Dimidjian, S., Hollon, S. D., Dobson, K. S., Schmaling, K. B., Kohlenberg, R. J., Addis, M. E., et al. (2006). Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(4), 658. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.4.658.CrossRefPubMed Dimidjian, S., Hollon, S. D., Dobson, K. S., Schmaling, K. B., Kohlenberg, R. J., Addis, M. E., et al. (2006). Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(4), 658. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​0022-006X.​74.​4.​658.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Chatterji, S., Lee, S., Ormel, J., et al. (2009). The global burden of mental disorders: an update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 18(1), 23–33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Chatterji, S., Lee, S., Ormel, J., et al. (2009). The global burden of mental disorders: an update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 18(1), 23–33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Krejtz, I., Holas, P., Rusanowska, M., & Nezlek, J. B. (2018). Positive online attentional training as a means of modifying attentional and interpretational biases among the clinically depressed: an experimental study using eye tracking. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(9), 1594–1606. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22617.CrossRefPubMed Krejtz, I., Holas, P., Rusanowska, M., & Nezlek, J. B. (2018). Positive online attentional training as a means of modifying attentional and interpretational biases among the clinically depressed: an experimental study using eye tracking. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(9), 1594–1606. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​jclp.​22617.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kupfer, D. J., Frank, E., & Wamhoff, J. (1996). Mood disorders: update on prevention of recurrence. In C. Mundt, M. J. Goldstein, K. Hahlweg, P. Fiedler, et al. (Eds.), Interpersonal factors in the origin and course of affective disorders (pp. 289–302). Gaskell/Royal College of Psychiatrists. Kupfer, D. J., Frank, E., & Wamhoff, J. (1996). Mood disorders: update on prevention of recurrence. In C. Mundt, M. J. Goldstein, K. Hahlweg, P. Fiedler, et al. (Eds.), Interpersonal factors in the origin and course of affective disorders (pp. 289–302). Gaskell/Royal College of Psychiatrists.
go back to reference Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user’s guide: statistical analysis with latent variables (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user’s guide: statistical analysis with latent variables (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.
go back to reference Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22–57.PubMed Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22–57.PubMed
go back to reference Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and therapy, 33(1), 25–39.CrossRefPubMed Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and therapy, 33(1), 25–39.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275.CrossRefPubMed Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Aalderen, J. R., Donders, A. R. T., Giommi, F., Spinhoven, P., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2012). The efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in recurrent depressed patients with and without a current depressive episode: a randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42(5), 989–1001. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002054.CrossRefPubMed Van Aalderen, J. R., Donders, A. R. T., Giommi, F., Spinhoven, P., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2012). The efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in recurrent depressed patients with and without a current depressive episode: a randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42(5), 989–1001. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S003329171100205​4.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van den Hurk, P. A., van Aalderen, J. R., Giommi, F., Donders, R. A., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. (2012). An investigation of the role of attention in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for recurrently depressed patients. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(1), 103–120. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024811.CrossRef van den Hurk, P. A., van Aalderen, J. R., Giommi, F., Donders, R. A., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. (2012). An investigation of the role of attention in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for recurrently depressed patients. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(1), 103–120. https://​doi.​org/​10.​5127/​jep.​024811.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
go back to reference Williams, J. M. G., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Brennan, K., Duggan, D. S., Fennell, M. J., et al. (2014). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for preventing relapse in recurrent depression: a randomized dismantling trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(2), 275. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035036.CrossRefPubMed Williams, J. M. G., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Brennan, K., Duggan, D. S., Fennell, M. J., et al. (2014). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for preventing relapse in recurrent depression: a randomized dismantling trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(2), 275. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0035036.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Modification of Attentional Bias to Emotional Faces Following Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy in People with a Current Depression
Auteurs
Paweł Holas
Izabela Krejtz
Katarzyna Wisiecka
Marzena Rusanowska
John B. Nezlek
Publicatiedatum
19-03-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 6/2020
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01353-2