Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness 10/2020

Open Access 07-08-2020 | ORIGINAL PAPER

Mindfulness and Affect During Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Recurrent Depression: an Autoregressive Latent Trajectory Analysis

Auteurs: Marleen J. ter Avest, Corina U. Greven, Marloes J. Huijbers, Tom F. Wilderjans, Anne E. M. Speckens, Philip Spinhoven

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 10/2020

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Objectives

Gaining knowledge of dynamic processes of mechanisms underlying mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) for recurrent depression could help to improve treatment efficacy. The current study examined the overall course and week-to-week associations of mindfulness and positive/negative affect during MBCT for recurrent depression.

Methods

Using data from the MOMENT study, 235 patients with recurrent depression in (partial) remission allocated to MBCT were included. Prior to each MBCT session, self-reports were obtained on mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect.

Results

Autoregressive latent trajectory (ALT) modeling revealed that, across the MBCT course, larger increases in mindfulness were associated with larger increases in positive affect (r = .80, p < .050). Higher general levels of negative affect were associated with smaller increases in mindfulness over time (r = −.26, p < .001). Week-to-week effects showed no reciprocal cross-lagged effects between mindfulness and positive affect or negative affect, except for positive affect at session 2 which was positively associated with mindfulness at session 3 (r = .11, p < .050).

Conclusions

The current study supports a positive association in strength of increase between mindfulness and positive affect, while higher general levels of negative affect might be associated with smaller increases of mindfulness during MBCT for recurrent depression. For future research, experience sampling methods (ESMs) are recommended to capture dynamics on a smaller time scale. ALT modeling techniques are advised to be better able to interpret the processes of stability and change during MBCT for recurrent depression.
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a common psychiatric disorder in which emotion regulation plays an important role (Joormann and Stanton 2016; World Health Organization 2017). Along with the diminished experience of positive emotions, the frequent experience of negative emotions is a key characteristic of depressive symptomatology (American Psychiatric Association 2013). MDD often runs a chronic and recurrent course, and many patients experience residual depressive symptoms (Nierenberg 2015; Richards 2011). Currently, MDD is a leading cause of disease burden worldwide (World Health Organization 2017).
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT; Segal et al. 2002) has been shown to be effective for relapse prevention in recurrent depression (Kuyken et al. 2016). MBCT consists of 8 weekly group sessions of 2.5 h and a silent day (Segal et al. 2012). The intervention includes mindfulness practices and elements from cognitive therapy for depression (Beck et al. 1979). Mindfulness, in the context of mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs), is most often referred to as paying attention, on purpose, in the present moment and non-judgmentally (Kabat-Zinn 1994) and is considered a skill that can be trained (Segal et al. 2012; Tang et al. 2015).
In order to improve the treatment efficacy of MBCT for recurrent depression, a better understanding of the processes which bring about therapeutic change is needed (Dimidjian and Segal 2015; Kazdin 2007). Research to date proposes various possible mediators for the effect of MBIs on mental health outcomes. A systematic review and meta-analysis of 20 studies, of which 15 randomized controlled trials (RCTs), aimed to identify potential psychological mediating mechanisms underlying the effects of MBIs in mixed samples (Gu et al. 2015). The study found moderate evidence that an increase in mindfulness and a reduction of rumination and worry mediated treatment outcome. Evidence for reduction of cognitive and emotional reactivity and for increases in self-compassion and psychological flexibility mediating treatment outcome was preliminary but insufficient. Another systematic review of 23 studies on MBCT for recurrent MDD, of which there were 20 RCTs, provided evidence for increases in mindfulness, self-compassion, and meta-awareness and decreases in rumination and worry to mediate treatment outcome (van der Velden et al. 2015). In addition, the study found preliminary evidence indicating that changes in attention, memory specificity, self-discrepancy, emotional reactivity, and momentary positive affect and negative affect might be possible mediating mechanisms of change. Despite accumulating evidence for mindfulness and emotion (regulation) as possible mediators for the effect of MBCT on mental health outcomes, findings have been largely based on pre-post assessments of mediators so far, and most studies did not take the temporal order of mediator and outcome variables into account. This makes it difficult to identify causal links between the intervention, the outcome(s), and possible mediators (Kazdin 2007). More rigorous designs are needed to disentangle causal and mutual associations of possibly mediating factors during MBCT.
According to the mindfulness-to-meaning theory (MMT), mindfulness has been suggested to have a negative reciprocal relationship with negative affect and a positive reciprocal relationship with positive affect (Garland et al. 2015). As residual depressive symptoms (e.g., negative affect) are a predictor for relapse/recurrence depression (Buckman et al. 2018; Watson et al. 1988) and positive affect is suggested to improve well-being and reduce depressive symptoms and risk of relapse/recurrence depression (Bolier et al. 2013; Khazanov and Ruscio 2016; Sin and Lyubomirsky 2009), developing one’s level of mindfulness could lead to decreases in negative affect and increases in positive affect, further leading to better outcomes in (recurrent) depressed patients.
Research to date with more advanced methodological approaches, such as experience sampling methods (ESMs; Larson and Csikszentmihalyi 1983), suggests these positive associations between mindfulness and positive affect and, to a lesser extent, negative associations between mindfulness and negative affect during MBCT in recurrently depressed patients. For example, an open-label RCT of MBCT for adults with residual depressive symptoms (N = 120) used experience sampling methodology during 6 consecutive days before and after MBCT or waitlist control period. The results showed that MBCT was associated with increased experience of momentary positive affect compared to waitlist control (Geschwind et al. 2011). Another ESM study of mainly patients with recurrent MDD (N = 29) showed that a mindful walking intervention resulted in an overall increase of both levels of mindfulness and positive affect and a decrease in negative affect. In addition, time-specific moment-to-moment effects showed that increased mindfulness was associated with subsequent increases in positive affect and decreases in negative affect the next moment during the day and vice versa (Gotink et al. 2016).
In short, overall increases in mindfulness appear to be associated with overall increases in positive affect and decreases in negative affect. However, knowledge of the dynamic process and temporal order of these changes in recurrently depressed patients is still scant. The dynamic interplay between levels of mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect during MBCT for recurrent depression has not been investigated yet. The present study expands the previous literature by investigating the course and mutual associations of mindfulness and both positive affect and negative affect in patients with recurrent depression following MBCT based on a large sample originating from two multicenter RCTs (Huijbers et al. 2012). Moreover, assessments of mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect took place prior to each MBCT session. Finally, the current study uses autoregressive latent trajectory (ALT) modeling to analyze the data, which permits a distinction between overall trajectories across the entire intervention and week-to-week effects of mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect. This makes it highly suitable for analyzing dynamic processes. Over the course of the MBCT, it is expected that mindfulness and positive affect increase, while negative affect decreases. In addition, the strength of increase in mindfulness is expected to be associated with the strength of increase in positive affect and decrease in negative affect. Finally, reciprocal week-to-week associations are expected between mindfulness and affect. Specifically, higher levels of mindfulness at a certain week are expected to precede higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect in the subsequent week. Meanwhile, higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect at a certain week are expected to precede higher levels of mindfulness in the subsequent week.

Method

Participants

Adult patients were recruited via direct referral from mental health professionals and media advertisements between September 2009 and January 2012 at 12 different secondary and tertiary psychiatric outpatient clinics across the Netherlands. Native Dutch-speaking patients with recurrent depression (≥ 3 prior episodes) according to the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association 2000) using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; κ = .70, p = .001, 95% CI = .46–.94; First et al. 1996), who were on a stable dose of antidepressant medication (≥ 6 months) and currently in (partial) remission, were included after written informed consent had been obtained. Exclusion criteria were as follows: bipolar disorder, psychotic disorder, neurological disorder, somatic disorder, current alcohol and/or drug dependency as assessed by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al. 1998), use of a high dose of benzodiazepines, electric convulsive therapy ≤ 3 months, previous mindfulness training or considerable meditation experience, current psychological treatment (≥ 1×/3 weeks), and visual hearing/cognitive impairments impeding full participation. Patients were included only after written informed consent had been obtained. Participants from both multicenter trials who received an adequate dose of the intervention, i.e., at least 4 MBCT sessions (Kuyken et al. 2010; Teasdale et al. 2000), were included in the current study (N = 235). Table 1 contains a detailed description of demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants of the current study. Participants participated, on average, 7.16 sessions (range = 4–8, SD = 1.08) and performed 58% (range = 0–1, SD = .23) of the given formal homework exercises.
Table 1
Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with recurrent depression receiving mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (N = 235)
Variable
N (%)
Female
157 (67)
Marital status
  Single
55 (24)
  Married or cohabiting
137 (60)
  Divorced or widowed
38 (16)
  Missing
5 (2)
Level of education
  Low
19 (8)
  Middle
65 (29)
  High
144 (63)
  Missing
7 (3)
Employed
155 (66)
Previous CBT
137 (58)
Type of mADM
  SSRI
162 (69)
  TCA
46 (20)
  Othera
27 (11)
Remission
  Full (IDS-C30 ≤ 11)
125 (53)
  Partial (IDS-C30 > 11)
110 (47)
Suicide attempt (lifetime)
43 (18)
Variable
Mean (SD)
Age in years
50.70 (10.81)
Depressive symptoms (IDS-C30)
12.58 (9.74)
Previous MDEs
5.91 (5.72)
Age at MDD onsetb
25.89 (11.84)
CBT cognitive behavioral therapy, mADM maintenance antidepressant medication, SSRI selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, TCA tricyclic antidepressant, IDS-C30 Dutch Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology-Clinician rated, MDEs major depressive episodes, MDD major depressive disorder
aIncluding serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, monoamine oxidase-inhibitors, and mirtazapine
bBased on self-report

Procedures

The current study used data from the MOMENT study which consists of two related multicenter RCTs. The first RCT was a non-inferiority trial that compared the combination of MBCT and maintenance of antidepressant medication (ADM) with MBCT alone (Huijbers et al. 2016). The second RCT was a superiority trial that compared the combination of MBCT and ADM with ADM alone (Huijbers et al. 2015). For study details, participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics, and outcomes of both RCTs, see the respective papers (Huijbers et al. 2015, 2016).
MBCT was delivered in a real-life setting, from university hospitals to community mental health centers across the Netherlands. Participants attended MBCT classes together with non-trial participants, i.e., patients with recurrent depression from regular clinical practice. The MBCT was largely based on the protocol of Segal et al. (2002) with some adaptations (2.5-h instead of 2-h sessions and additionally 1 silent day between the 6th and 7th sessions). Adding the silent day has been suggested in the most recent version of the MBCT protocol (Segal et al. 2012). Groups consisted of 8 to 12 patients during 8 weekly sessions. MBCT included formal meditation exercises, such as the body scan, sitting meditation, walking meditation, and mindful movement as well as informal exercises, such as bringing present-moment awareness to everyday activities. Cognitive behavioral techniques included education, monitoring and scheduling of activities, identification of negative automatic thoughts, and devising a relapse prevention plan. Participants were encouraged to practice meditation at home for about an hour a day using CDs. At the start of each weekly session, questionnaires were filled out by participants. A total number of 21 teachers recruited from regular clinical practice participated in both trials. Videotapes were available for 15 primary teachers and examined with the Mindfulness-Based Interventions: Teaching Assessment Criteria (MBI:TAC; Crane et al. 2013). Teacher ratings were “proficient” (n = 3), “competent” (n = 4), “advanced beginner” (n = 6), and “beginner” (n = 2). The mean teacher competency score was 3.53 (SD = .92, range = 2.00–5.15). Seven of the 15 teachers met the advanced criteria of the association of mindfulness-based teachers in the Netherland and Flanders (www.​vmbn.​nl), which are in accordance with the UK good practice guidelines (UK Network of Mindfulness-Based Teachers 2015).

Measures

The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) was administered to assess the characteristic of dispositional mindfulness, namely open or receptive awareness of and attention to what is taking place in the present (Brown and Ryan 2003). This questionnaire consists of 15 items formulated in a negative way (e.g., “I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present”). Items are rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “almost always” to “almost never”. Higher values represent higher levels of mindfulness. In this study, participants were instructed to choose the answers for each statement that best reflected their experiences of the past week, including the current day. The average scale score was used in this study. The Dutch version of the MAAS has shown the expected 1-factor structure, a good internal consistency, and theoretically coherent correlations with, e.g., well-being and stress among a Dutch non-clinical population (Schroevers et al. 2008). The internal consistency in the current study was excellent (α = .95).
Positive affect and negative affect were assessed with the International Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short-Form (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson 2007). This questionnaire consists of ten items of which 5 assess positive affective states (e.g., enthusiastic, excited) and 5 assess negative affective states (e.g., irritable, nervous) relating to the last week, including the day itself. The items are scored using a 5-point Likert scale anchored from “never” to “always”. Higher values indicate higher levels of positive/negative affective states. Thompson (2007) investigated the cross-sample stability, internal reliability, temporal stability, and convergent and criterion-related validities of the scale and found the scale to be psychometrically acceptable. The average scale score was used in the current study. The internal consistency in the current study was excellent for positive affect (α = .93) and good for negative affect (α = .89).

Data Analyses

The current study used the ALT modeling technique which combines a latent trajectory model (LTM) with an autoregressive (AR) model. The LTM allows each individual in the sample to have a different overall trajectory as marked by a different (subject-specific) intercept and slope when tracked over time. The intercept can be interpreted as a general level of a variable and is constant over time. Its variance represents differences in general levels between individuals. The slope can be interpreted as an overall rate of change (positive or negative) of a variable over the intervention period, and its variance represents inter-individual differences in the rate of change. However, reciprocal week-to-week associations between variables during the intervention period are not captured with a LTM. AR models can investigate reciprocal week-to-week associations between variables, so-called cross-lagged (CL) effects, while allowing the prior value of a variable to determine the current value of the same variable (AR effects). Although AR models allow investigating reciprocal CL effects, caution is warranted. Indeed, when not accounting for the (differences in) overall trajectories in the variable(s) over the intervention period, spurious CL effects might appear while they actually do not exist (Voelkle 2008). Therefore, combining both the AR model and LTM into an ALT model makes it possible to study reciprocal CL effects properly while controlling for overall trajectories. This enables a better interpretation of the processes of stability and change during MBCT for recurrent depression.
The analytical strategy followed Bollen and Curran’s (2004) recommendations. At first, univariate unconditional AR models, LTMs, and several ALT models were estimated separately for each variable —mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect— and compared to identify which model best represented the course of these variables separately during MBCT. Secondly, bivariate unconditional AR models, LTMs, and various ALT models were fitted and compared for both mindfulness and positive affect as well as negative affect to examine which model best represented the course and possible mutual associations of mindfulness and positive/negative affect during MBCT. For a detailed description of the model building steps of both the univariate and bivariate models, model fit, and data handling, see Supplementary Material 1. Descriptive analyses were performed with SPSS, version 22 (IBM Corporation 2013). All structural equation models (SEMs) were estimated by using IBM Amos SPSS, version 25.0 (Arbuckle 2017). As significance level, a two-sided alpha level of .050 was used.

Results

The descriptive statistics of and correlations between all studied variables are presented in Supplementary Material 2, Tables 2.1 and 2.2. Mindfulness and positive affect steadily increased over the intervention period, while negative affect showed a more irregular course. See Fig. 1 for a visual representation of the overall means of these variables per session. In addition, mindfulness had high autocorrelations between the weekly sessions, whereas these autocorrelations were moderate for positive affect and negative affect. At each session separately, mindfulness correlated positively with positive affect and negatively with negative affect. As the assumption of normality was violated for negative affect, a logarithmic transformation was applied to all measurement points of this variable before further analyses were performed. For a detailed description of the univariate and bivariate models that were fitted and the results of the final univariate ALT models, see Supplementary Material 2. To improve readability and interpretation, only the results of the final bivariate ALT model of both mindfulness and positive affect as well as negative affect are presented below.

Mindfulness and Positive Affect

The final bivariate ALT model showed an excellent model fit (χ2 = 83.51, df = 84, p = .495; comparative fit index (CFI) = 1.000; Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 1.000; root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) < .001). The significant parameter estimates are depicted in Fig. 2, with the (co)variance and correlation estimates being reported in Table 2. A positive covariance was observed between both intercepts (\( {\Psi}_{\alpha_{\mathrm{MFN}},{\alpha}_{\mathrm{PA}}}=.077 \), SE = .031, p = .013) and between both slopes (\( {\Psi}_{\beta_{\mathrm{MFN}},{\beta}_{\mathrm{PA}}}=.002 \), SE = .001, p = .047). This indicates that participants with higher general levels of mindfulness showed higher general levels of positive affect. In addition, participants with larger increases in mindfulness showed larger increases in positive affect. In addition to these general trajectories over the MBCT course, week-to-week effects were found. Both mindfulness (ρAR(1) = .327, SE = .051, p < .001) and positive affect (ρAR(1) = .187, SE = .048, p < .001) had a clear stable AR component. This indicates that higher levels of mindfulness respectively positive affect on a given session predicted higher levels of mindfulness respectively positive affect on the following session. The CL parameter estimates showed that the CL effect from positive affect at session 2 to mindfulness at session 3 was positive and significant (bPA(t2),MFN(t3) = .118, SE = .046, p = .011), whereas this effect was not significant for other sessions (−.044 < bPA(t − 1),MFN(t) < .057, p > .174). No CL effects were found from mindfulness to positive affect (bMFN(t − 1),PA(t) = .049, SE = .061, p = .423).
Table 2
Variances, covariances, and correlations between the first measurements (session 1) and the intercepts and slopes of mindfulness and positive affect (upper part), and of mindfulness and negative affect (lower part) based on the final bivariate ALT models
 
S1.MFN
S1.PA
Intercept MFN
Intercept PA
Slope MFN
Slope PA
S1.MFN
.423 (.040)***
.368
.773
.391
−.209
−.193
S1.PA
.163 (.032)***
.465 (.044)***
.330
.758
.091
.085
Intercept MFN
.189 (.032)***
.085 (.030)**
.142 (.034)***
.528
.100
−.344
Intercept PA
.099 (.034)**
.201 (.034)***
.077 (.031)*
.151 (.043)***
.200
.013
Slope MFN
−.006 (.004)ns
.003 (.005)ns
.002 (.003)ns
.003 (.004)ns
.002 (.001)ns
.800
Slope PA
−.007 (.005)ns
.003 (.005)ns
−.007 (.004)ns
<.001 (.005)ns
.002 (.001)*
.003 (.001)*
 
S1.MFN
S1.NA
Intercept MFN
Intercept NA
Slope MFN
 
S1.MFN
.423 (.040)***
−.272
.781
−.262
−.229
 
S1.NA
−.026 (.007)***
.021 (.002)***
−.238
.734
−.108
 
Intercept MFN
.201 (.030)***
−.014 (.005)**
.156 (.033)***
−.315
.040
 
Intercept NA
−.016 (.005)**
.010 (.001)***
−.012 (.004)**
.009 (.001)***
−.258
 
Slope MFN
−.007 (.003)ns
−.001 (.001)ns
.001 (.003)ns
−.001 (.001)*
.002 (.001)*
 
Variances are depicted on the diagonal, covariances below, and correlations above the diagonal. Standard errors are shown between parentheses. Level of significance is indicated for the covariances and variances only
S1 observed variable of the first session, MFN mindfulness, PA positive affect, NA negative affect (logarithmic transformed)
nsp ≥ .050; *p < .050; **p < .010; ***p < .001

Mindfulness and Negative Affect

The final bivariate ALT model showed a good model fit (χ2 = 106.67, df = 102, p = .356; CFI = .998; TLI = .998; RMSEA = .014). The significant parameter estimates are graphically represented in Fig. 3, with the (co)variance and correlation estimates being displayed in Table 2. A negative covariance was observed between the intercepts of mindfulness and negative affect (\( {\Psi}_{\alpha_{\mathrm{MFN}},{\alpha}_{\mathrm{NA}}}=-.012 \), SE = .004, p = .009), which indicates that higher general levels of mindfulness were associated with lower general levels of negative affect. The final model did not contain a slope for negative affect, which implies that there was no increase or decrease of negative affect over the course of the MBCT. A negative covariance was observed between the intercept for negative affect and the slope for mindfulness (\( {\Psi}_{\beta_{\mathrm{MFN}},{\alpha}_{\mathrm{NA}}}=-.001 \), SE = .001, p = .036), which suggests that patients with higher general levels of negative affect showed smaller increases in mindfulness over the MBCT course. Week-to-week effects showed substantial AR effects for mindfulness (ρAR(1) = .303, SE = .049, p < .001) and negative affect (ρAR(1) = .218, SE = .037, p < .001) which were stable over time. No CL effects were found from mindfulness towards negative affect (bMFN(t − 1),NA(t) = −.010, SE = .010, p = .291) or vice versa (bNA(t − 1),MFN(t) = −.137, SE = .102, p = .181). As such, the level of mindfulness at a certain session did not predict the level of negative affect at the next session or vice versa.

Discussion

The current study was based on data from patients with recurrent depression in (partial) remission receiving an 8-week MBCT as part of two multicenter RCTs. The overall trajectory and week-to-week associations of mindfulness and positive/negative affect were investigated across the intervention period by using the ALT modeling technique. When looking at overall trajectories, mindfulness and positive affect significantly increased, while the overall decrease of negative affect was non-significant. Patients with larger increases in mindfulness showed significantly larger increases in positive affect. In addition, patients with higher general levels of negative affect showed significantly smaller increases in mindfulness. Week-to-week effects showed no significant reciprocal CL effects between mindfulness and positive affect or negative affect, except for positive affect at session 2 which was positively associated with mindfulness at session 3.

Mindfulness and Positive Affect

As expected, patients with larger increases in mindfulness showed significantly larger increases in positive affect over the MBCT course. This is in accordance with existing theories (e.g., Garland et al. 2009, 2015) suggesting positive (reciprocal) associations between mindfulness and positive affect. In addition, it is in line with previous research showing effects of MBIs on increasing positive affect (e.g., Geschwind et al. 2011; Schroevers and Brandsma 2010). However, the current study did not find significant effects between mindfulness and subsequent positive affect on a weekly basis, while only positive affect at session 2 positively significantly predicted mindfulness at session 3. The latter might be explained by the given home practice during session 2 for the coming week, namely registering one pleasant event on a daily basis with accompanied thoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations. However, this is speculative. Another study investigating mindfulness and positive affect in a community sample also did not find changes in positive affect on a certain day to predict changes in mindfulness the following day, although reverse associations were found (Snippe et al. 2015). In contrast, positive moment-to-moment relations between mindfulness and subsequent positive affect, and vice versa, were found in patients with recurrent MDD during a mindful walking intervention using experience sampling methodology (Gotink et al. 2016). Perhaps, the time span used in the current study was not fine-grained enough to detect these CL effects. More importantly, however, the differences in outcomes could be explained by the different analytic techniques adopted. When not taking overall changes during the intervention period and inter-individual differences therein into account (i.e., fitting intercept-only models while inter-individual differences in slopes exist), unmodeled overall changes could “show up” as spurious CL (and AR) effects, rendering the substantive interpretation of these effects impossible. Simulation studies show that ignoring or misspecifying (i.e., assuming linearity when true changes are non-linear) the overall change over time results in incorrect estimates for CL and AR effects (Voelkle 2008). Indeed, when a bivariate intercept-only ALT model was fitted for mindfulness and positive affect in the current study, instead of a bivariate intercept and slope model, CL effects from mindfulness to positive affect became significant, as half of the CL effects from positive affect to mindfulness. It is therefore very important to carefully build a model that captures the overall trajectories of the variables correctly and that simultaneously accounts for time-specific effects.

Mindfulness and Negative Affect

Against expectations, negative affect did not significantly decrease over the MBCT course, which is contrary to previous studies (van der Velden et al. 2015). However, it seems in line with the absence of a clear main effect of MBCT on depression severity found in the clinical trials from which the current data were derived. Another explanation could be that participants were more able to accept negative affect rather than engaging in elaboration or rumination (Garland et al. 2015), therefore having more room for positive affect. Across the intervention period, higher general levels of negative affect were associated with significantly smaller increases in mindfulness, which suggests that negative affect might act as a barrier to develop mindfulness during MBCT. This is in line with the mindfulness-to-meaning theory (Garland et al. 2015), which asserts that negative emotions narrow the scope of awareness. Week-to-week CL associations between mindfulness and subsequent negative affect were non-significant. This is inconsistent with previous research by Gotink et al. (2016), which found moment-to-moment negative associations between mindfulness and subsequent negative affect during a mindful walking intervention in mainly recurrently depressed patients. However, in an ESM study in a general sample, increases in mindfulness on a certain day preceded decreases in negative affect the next day, but not the other way around (Snippe et al. 2015). One explanation could be that the current study included participants who were (partly) in remission which may have resulted in a restriction of a range of negative affect. More pronounced effects of mindfulness on negative effect or vice versa might be found when MBCT targets acutely depressed individuals. In addition, the relatively low average level of teacher competence compared to previous studies (e.g., Kuyken et al. 2015) may have attenuated the effect of MBCT on negative affect in general. Finally, the same methodological shortcomings apply to these findings as mentioned above (i.e., time span used and differences in analytical techniques adopted).

Strengths

The current study is the first to investigate the mutual associations of mindfulness and positive affect and negative affect during MBCT for recurrent depression. It included weekly assessments during the intervention in 235 patients and used an advanced analysis technique which enables to make a distinction between the overall trajectories across the entire MBCT course and week-to-week effects of mindfulness, positive affect, and negative affect. This made it possible to study processes of mindfulness and affect in much more detail compared with pre-post studies and increased the statistical power to find potential associations between them. Finally, data from the present study were based on two highly ecologically valid RCTs which were embedded in a standard clinical care setting. This real-life setting increases generalization of our findings to patients with remitted recurrent depression in both secondary care and tertiary care.

Limitations and Future Research

There were also some limitations. First, no weekly assessments took place in the control group, which makes it unclear whether similar effects would be observed with the passing of time or in any other group activity. In subsequent research, MBCT should be compared to both passive and active control groups in order to determine whether changes in mindfulness and affect are specific for MBCT. Second, results of the current study are limited to patients with remitted depression and cannot be generalized to patients with acute depression. For future research, it is recommended to investigate changes in mindfulness and affect across individuals with different mental health problems with different levels of severity receiving MBCT (e.g., anxiety disorder) in order to determine the specificity of changes in mindfulness and affect. Third, another possible methodological problem concerns the questionnaires that were used in the current study. Mindfulness was assessed with the MAAS (Brown and Ryan 2003), which has been questioned in terms of its validity and ability to measure mindfulness (Van Dam et al. 2010). The MAAS taps into one aspect of mindfulness: (lack of) “attention towards the present moment”. The “purposeful orientation to one’s experience with a non-judgmental attitude” is not captured by the MAAS. In addition, the I-PANAS-SF (Thompson 2007) might have limitations in terms of representing only highly aroused forms of positive and negative feelings (Diener et al. 2009). Moreover, the negative affect scale of the I-PANAS-SF includes a wide range of different emotions (e.g., anger, fear), which each might follow a different course during MBCT for recurrent depression.
In future research, it would be recommended to include regular measurements administering different facets of mindfulness such as the facets from the Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire short form (FFMQ-15; Baer et al. 2008). With regard to momentary affect measures, it seems important to include more subtle emotions too (e.g., calm, relaxed), considering that, particularly, these emotions are assumed to increase during meditation (Jones et al. 2018). Furthermore, it would be valuable to repeatedly assess depressive symptoms to further determine changes in mindfulness and affect as the possible mechanisms of change in MBCT for depression. Moreover, it is encouraged to use ESMs, in order to capture dynamics on a smaller time scale. Finally, it is very important to carefully build a model that captures the overall trajectories of the variables correctly and that simultaneously accounts for time-specific effects. Authors should aim to provide ample insight into the analysis techniques and methods that were used, in order to increase the chance to replicate findings and properly compare results.
In summary, the current study supports a positive association in strength of increase between mindfulness and positive affect during MBCT for recurrent depression. Negative affect might run a more irregular course and might be associated with a smaller increase in mindfulness during MBCT for recurrent depression. Reciprocal CL associations between mindfulness and positive/negative affect that were established in previous research were generally not confirmed, possibly due to the more rigorous methodology of the current study and the time frame used. For future research, it is encouraged to use ESM in order to capture dynamics on a smaller time scale. In addition, the use of the ALT modeling technique is highly recommended to enable a better interpretation of the processes of stability and change during MBCT for recurrent depression.

Acknowledgments

The MOMENT study was funded by ZonMW, the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (Grant No. 170992903, awarded to Prof. Dr. A. E. M. Speckens).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Medical Ethics Committee of Arnhem-Nijmegen (CMO; No. 2008/242), and local feasibility was approved by local ethics committees.
All patients were included only after written informed consent had been obtained.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Arbuckle, J. L. (2017). Amos (version 23.0) [computer program]. Chicago: SPSS. Arbuckle, J. L. (2017). Amos (version 23.0) [computer program]. Chicago: SPSS.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. Guilford. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. Guilford.
go back to reference Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.
go back to reference Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., & Oishi, S. (2009). New measures of well-being. In E. Diener (Ed.), The colleted works of Ed Diener. Springer. Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., & Oishi, S. (2009). New measures of well-being. In E. Diener (Ed.), The colleted works of Ed Diener. Springer.
go back to reference First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). User guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders. American Psychiatric Association. First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). User guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders. American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Geschwind, N., Peeters, F., Drukker, M., van Os, J., & Wichers, M. (2011). Mindfulness training increases momentary positive emotions and reward experience in adults vulnerable to depression: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(5), 618–628. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024595.CrossRefPubMed Geschwind, N., Peeters, F., Drukker, M., van Os, J., & Wichers, M. (2011). Mindfulness training increases momentary positive emotions and reward experience in adults vulnerable to depression: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(5), 618–628. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0024595.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huijbers, M. J., Spijker, J., Donders, A. R. T., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Ruhé, H. G., Blom, M. B. J., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., van der Wilt, G. J., Kuyken, W., Spinhoven, P., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2012). Preventing relapse in recurrent depression using mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, antidepressant medication or the combination: trial design and protocol of the MOMENT study. BMC Psychiatry, 12(125). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244x-12-125. Huijbers, M. J., Spijker, J., Donders, A. R. T., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Ruhé, H. G., Blom, M. B. J., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., van der Wilt, G. J., Kuyken, W., Spinhoven, P., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2012). Preventing relapse in recurrent depression using mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, antidepressant medication or the combination: trial design and protocol of the MOMENT study. BMC Psychiatry, 12(125). https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​1471-244x-12-125.
go back to reference Huijbers, M. J., Spinhoven, P., Spijker, J., Ruhe, H. G., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., Kuyken, W., van der Wilt, G. J., Blom, M. B. J., Schene, A. H., Donders, A. R. T., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2015). Adding mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to maintenance antidepressant medication for prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depressive disorder: randomised controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 54–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.023.CrossRefPubMed Huijbers, M. J., Spinhoven, P., Spijker, J., Ruhe, H. G., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., Kuyken, W., van der Wilt, G. J., Blom, M. B. J., Schene, A. H., Donders, A. R. T., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2015). Adding mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to maintenance antidepressant medication for prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depressive disorder: randomised controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 54–61. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jad.​2015.​08.​023.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Huijbers, M. J., Spinhoven, P., Spijker, J., Ruhé, H. G., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., Kuyken, W., van der Wilt, G. J., Blom, M. B. J., Schene, A. H., Rogier, A., Donders, T., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2016). Discontinuation of antidepressant medication after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for recurrent depression: randomised controlled non-inferiority trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(4), 366–373. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.115.168971.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Huijbers, M. J., Spinhoven, P., Spijker, J., Ruhé, H. G., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., Nolen, W. A., Ormel, J., Kuyken, W., van der Wilt, G. J., Blom, M. B. J., Schene, A. H., Rogier, A., Donders, T., & Speckens, A. E. M. (2016). Discontinuation of antidepressant medication after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for recurrent depression: randomised controlled non-inferiority trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(4), 366–373. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1192/​bjp.​bp.​115.​168971.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference IBM Corporation. (2013). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (version 22.0). Armonk: IBM Corporation. IBM Corporation. (2013). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (version 22.0). Armonk: IBM Corporation.
go back to reference Jones, D. R., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Smyth, J. M., & Lehman, B. J. (2018). Clarifying the associations between mindfulness meditation and emotion: daily high- and low-arousal emotions and emotional variability. Applied Psychology. Health and Well-Being. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12135. Jones, D. R., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Smyth, J. M., & Lehman, B. J. (2018). Clarifying the associations between mindfulness meditation and emotion: daily high- and low-arousal emotions and emotional variability. Applied Psychology. Health and Well-Being. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​aphw.​12135.
go back to reference Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. Hyperion. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. Hyperion.
go back to reference Kuyken, W., Hayes, R., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Dalgleish, T., Kessler, D., Lewis, G., Watkins, E., Breicha, C., Cardy, J., Causley, A., Cowderoy, S., Evans, A., Gradinger, F., Kaur, S., Lanham, P., Morant, N., Richards, J., Shah, P., et al. (2015). Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy compared with maintenance antidepressant treatment in the prevention of depressive relapse or recurrence (PREVENT): a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 386(9988), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)62222-4.CrossRef Kuyken, W., Hayes, R., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Dalgleish, T., Kessler, D., Lewis, G., Watkins, E., Breicha, C., Cardy, J., Causley, A., Cowderoy, S., Evans, A., Gradinger, F., Kaur, S., Lanham, P., Morant, N., Richards, J., Shah, P., et al. (2015). Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy compared with maintenance antidepressant treatment in the prevention of depressive relapse or recurrence (PREVENT): a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 386(9988), 63–73. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S0140-6736(14)62222-4.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuyken, W., Warren, F. C., Taylor, R. S., Whalley, B., Crane, C., Bondolfi, G., Hayes, R., Huijbers, M. J., Ma, H., Schweizer, S., Segal, Z., Speckens, A. E. M., Teasdale, J. D., van Heeringen, K., Williams, M., Byford, S., Byng, R., & Dalgleish, T. (2016). Efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in prevention of depressive relapse: an individual patient data meta-analysis from randomized trials. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(6), 565–574. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0076.CrossRefPubMed Kuyken, W., Warren, F. C., Taylor, R. S., Whalley, B., Crane, C., Bondolfi, G., Hayes, R., Huijbers, M. J., Ma, H., Schweizer, S., Segal, Z., Speckens, A. E. M., Teasdale, J. D., van Heeringen, K., Williams, M., Byford, S., Byng, R., & Dalgleish, T. (2016). Efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in prevention of depressive relapse: an individual patient data meta-analysis from randomized trials. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(6), 565–574. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​jamapsychiatry.​2016.​0076.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Science, 15, 41–56. Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Science, 15, 41–56.
go back to reference Schroevers, M., Nyklíček, I., & Topman, R. (2008). Validatie van de Nederlandstalige versie van de Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Gedragstherapie, 41, 225–240. Schroevers, M., Nyklíček, I., & Topman, R. (2008). Validatie van de Nederlandstalige versie van de Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Gedragstherapie, 41, 225–240.
go back to reference Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: a new approach to relapse prevention. Guilford. Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: a new approach to relapse prevention. Guilford.
go back to reference Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression (2nd ed.). Guilford. Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression (2nd ed.). Guilford.
go back to reference Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(20), 22–33.PubMed Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(20), 22–33.PubMed
go back to reference Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Rigdeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 615–623.CrossRef Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Rigdeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 615–623.CrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.CrossRef
go back to reference van der Velden, A. M., Kuyken, W., Wattar, U., Crane, C., Pallesen, K. J., Dahlgaard, J., Fjorback, L. O., & Piet, J. (2015). A systematic review of mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in the treatment of recurrent major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 37, 26–39.CrossRef van der Velden, A. M., Kuyken, W., Wattar, U., Crane, C., Pallesen, K. J., Dahlgaard, J., Fjorback, L. O., & Piet, J. (2015). A systematic review of mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in the treatment of recurrent major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 37, 26–39.CrossRef
go back to reference World Health Organization. (2017). Depression and other common mental disorders: global health estimates. World Health Organization. World Health Organization. (2017). Depression and other common mental disorders: global health estimates. World Health Organization.
Metagegevens
Titel
Mindfulness and Affect During Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Recurrent Depression: an Autoregressive Latent Trajectory Analysis
Auteurs
Marleen J. ter Avest
Corina U. Greven
Marloes J. Huijbers
Tom F. Wilderjans
Anne E. M. Speckens
Philip Spinhoven
Publicatiedatum
07-08-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 10/2020
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01453-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 10/2020

Mindfulness 10/2020 Naar de uitgave