Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2012

01-03-2012 | Review

Mind and movement

Auteurs: Herbert Heuer, Sandra Sülzenbrück

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Voluntary movements embrace both intentional, conscious and post-intentional, largely automatic processes. Here, we examine these types of processes and the relations between them during preparation and execution of voluntary movements. First, a general overview is given about how intentional and post-intentional components are interleaved to enable successful control of purposeful movements. Second, we briefly describe some post-intentional processes that are triggered by preceding intentions. Third, we discuss findings according to which such post-intentional processes or their results can become accessible to conscious awareness. Under such conditions, automatic and conscious processes can co-occur. We show that intentional interventions into post-intentional processes can be overridden by automatic processes, can interfere with automatic processes and can be independent so that their outcomes add to those of automatic processes.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F. X., Goodale, M. A., et al. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685.PubMedCrossRef Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F. X., Goodale, M. A., et al. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Arend, I., Johnston, S., Shapiro, K., et al. (2006). Task-irrelevant motion and flicker attenuate the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 600–607.PubMedCrossRef Arend, I., Johnston, S., Shapiro, K., et al. (2006). Task-irrelevant motion and flicker attenuate the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 600–607.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Azanon, E., Longo, M. R., Soto-Faraco, S., Haggard, P., et al. (2010). The posterior parietal cortex remaps touch into external space. Current Biology, 20, 1304–1309.PubMedCrossRef Azanon, E., Longo, M. R., Soto-Faraco, S., Haggard, P., et al. (2010). The posterior parietal cortex remaps touch into external space. Current Biology, 20, 1304–1309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.PubMedCrossRef Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.CrossRef Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.CrossRef
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., Starkes, J. L., et al. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMedCrossRef Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., Starkes, J. L., et al. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bizzi, E., Accornero, N., Chapple, W., Hogan, N., et al. (1984). Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2738–2744.PubMed Bizzi, E., Accornero, N., Chapple, W., Hogan, N., et al. (1984). Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2738–2744.PubMed
go back to reference Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., Sperling, A., et al. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 29, 336–342.CrossRef Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., Sperling, A., et al. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 29, 336–342.CrossRef
go back to reference Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., Nagle, M., et al. (1979). Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 692–700.PubMedCrossRef Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., Nagle, M., et al. (1979). Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 692–700.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bridgeman, B., Peery, S., Anand, S., et al. (1997). Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 456–469.PubMedCrossRef Bridgeman, B., Peery, S., Anand, S., et al. (1997). Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 456–469.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Castiello, U., Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., et al. (1991). Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal subjects. Brain, 114, 2639–2655.PubMedCrossRef Castiello, U., Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., et al. (1991). Temporal dissociation of motor responses and subjective awareness. A study in normal subjects. Brain, 114, 2639–2655.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chase, R. A., Harvey, S., Standfast, S., Rapin, I., Sutton, S., et al. (1961). Studies on sensory feedback I: Effect of delayed auditory feedback on speech and key tapping. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 141–152.CrossRef Chase, R. A., Harvey, S., Standfast, S., Rapin, I., Sutton, S., et al. (1961). Studies on sensory feedback I: Effect of delayed auditory feedback on speech and key tapping. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 141–152.CrossRef
go back to reference Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2001). Defining the cortical visual systems: “What”, “Where”, and “How”. Acta Psychologica, 107, 43–68.PubMedCrossRef Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2001). Defining the cortical visual systems: “What”, “Where”, and “How”. Acta Psychologica, 107, 43–68.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davidson, P. R., Jones, R. D., Sirisena, H. R., Andreae, J. H., et al. (2000). Detection of adaptive inverse models in the human motor system. Human Movement Science, 19, 761–795.CrossRef Davidson, P. R., Jones, R. D., Sirisena, H. R., Andreae, J. H., et al. (2000). Detection of adaptive inverse models in the human motor system. Human Movement Science, 19, 761–795.CrossRef
go back to reference Day, B. I., & Lyon, I. N. (2000). Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target. Experimental Brain Research, 130, 159–168.CrossRef Day, B. I., & Lyon, I. N. (2000). Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target. Experimental Brain Research, 130, 159–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Fiehler, K., Bannert, M. M., Bischoff, M., Blecker, C., Stark, R., Vaitl, D., et al. (2011). Working memory maintenance of grasp–target information in the human posterior pariety cortex. Neuroimage, 54, 2401–2411.PubMedCrossRef Fiehler, K., Bannert, M. M., Bischoff, M., Blecker, C., Stark, R., Vaitl, D., et al. (2011). Working memory maintenance of grasp–target information in the human posterior pariety cortex. Neuroimage, 54, 2401–2411.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiehler, K., Burke, M., Bien, S., Röder, B., Rösler, F., et al. (2009). The human dorsal action control system develops in the absence of vision. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1–12.PubMedCrossRef Fiehler, K., Burke, M., Bien, S., Röder, B., Rösler, F., et al. (2009). The human dorsal action control system develops in the absence of vision. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1–12.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiehler, K., Burke, M., Engel, A., Bien, S., Rösler, F., et al. (2008). Kinesthetic working memory and action control within the dorsal stream. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 243–253.PubMedCrossRef Fiehler, K., Burke, M., Engel, A., Bien, S., Rösler, F., et al. (2008). Kinesthetic working memory and action control within the dorsal stream. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 243–253.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–1140.PubMedCrossRef Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–1140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Dalery, J., d’Amato, T., et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one’s own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454–459.PubMedCrossRef Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Dalery, J., d’Amato, T., et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one’s own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Franz, V. H. (2001). Acrion does not resist visual illusions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 457–459.PubMedCrossRef Franz, V. H. (2001). Acrion does not resist visual illusions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 457–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: consistent data and no dissociation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 920–950.CrossRef Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: consistent data and no dissociation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 920–950.CrossRef
go back to reference Franz, V. H., Hesse, C., Kollath, S., et al. (2009). Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: No shift from dorsal to ventral control. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1518–1531.PubMedCrossRef Franz, V. H., Hesse, C., Kollath, S., et al. (2009). Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: No shift from dorsal to ventral control. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1518–1531.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., et al. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, 355, 1771–1788.CrossRef Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., et al. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, 355, 1771–1788.CrossRef
go back to reference Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1991). WIlled action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 244, 241–246.PubMedCrossRef Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1991). WIlled action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 244, 241–246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallagher, A. G., McClure, N., McGuigan, J., Ritchie, K., Sheehy, N. P., et al. (1998). An ergonomic analysis of the fulcrum effect in the acquisition of endoscopic skills. Endoscopy, 30, 617–620.PubMedCrossRef Gallagher, A. G., McClure, N., McGuigan, J., Ritchie, K., Sheehy, N. P., et al. (1998). An ergonomic analysis of the fulcrum effect in the acquisition of endoscopic skills. Endoscopy, 30, 617–620.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghilardi, M., Ghez, C., Dhawan, V., Moeller, J., Mentis, M., Nakamura, T., et al. (2000). Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Research, 871, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef Ghilardi, M., Ghez, C., Dhawan, V., Moeller, J., Mentis, M., Nakamura, T., et al. (2000). Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Research, 871, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., Pélisson, D., Prablanc, C., et al. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748–750.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., Pélisson, D., Prablanc, C., et al. (1986). Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2004). An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 203–211.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2004). An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 203–211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodwin, G. M., McCloskey, D. I., Matthews, P. B. C., et al. (1972). The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. Brain, 95, 705–748.PubMedCrossRef Goodwin, G. M., McCloskey, D. I., Matthews, P. B. C., et al. (1972). The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. Brain, 95, 705–748.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gosselin-Kessiby, N., Kalaska, J. F., Messier, J., et al. (2009). Evidence for a proprioception-based rapid on-line error correction mechanism for hand orientation during reaching movements in blind subjects. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3485–3496.PubMedCrossRef Gosselin-Kessiby, N., Kalaska, J. F., Messier, J., et al. (2009). Evidence for a proprioception-based rapid on-line error correction mechanism for hand orientation during reaching movements in blind subjects. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3485–3496.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.PubMedCrossRef Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gucciardi, D. F., & Dimmock, J. A. (2008). Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 45–59.CrossRef Gucciardi, D. F., & Dimmock, J. A. (2008). Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 45–59.CrossRef
go back to reference Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. A. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122–136.PubMedCrossRef Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. A. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hegele, M., & Heuer, H. (2010a). The impact of augmented information on visuo-motor adaptation in younger and older adults. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12071.PubMedCrossRef Hegele, M., & Heuer, H. (2010a). The impact of augmented information on visuo-motor adaptation in younger and older adults. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12071.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hegele, M., & Heuer, H. (2010b). Adaptation to a direction-dependent visuomotor gain in the young and elderly. Psychological Research, 74, 21–34.PubMedCrossRef Hegele, M., & Heuer, H. (2010b). Adaptation to a direction-dependent visuomotor gain in the young and elderly. Psychological Research, 74, 21–34.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S.W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2: Motor skills (pp. 121–180). London: Academic Press. Heuer, H. (1996). Coordination. In H. Heuer & S.W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2Motor skills (pp. 121–180). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008). Adaptation to visuo-motor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 190–202.PubMedCrossRef Heuer, H., & Hegele, M. (2008). Adaptation to visuo-motor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 190–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H., & Klein, W. (2006). The modulation of intermanual interactions during the specification of the directions of bimanual movements. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 162–181.CrossRef Heuer, H., & Klein, W. (2006). The modulation of intermanual interactions during the specification of the directions of bimanual movements. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 162–181.CrossRef
go back to reference Inoue, K., Kawashima, R., Satoh, K., Kinomura, S., Sugiura, M., Goto, R., et al. (2000). A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage, 11, 505–516.PubMedCrossRef Inoue, K., Kawashima, R., Satoh, K., Kinomura, S., Sugiura, M., Goto, R., et al. (2000). A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage, 11, 505–516.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Motor awareness without perceptual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 43, 227–237.PubMedCrossRef Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Motor awareness without perceptual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 43, 227–237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jordan, M. I. (1996). Computational aspects of motor control and motor learning. In H. Heuer, S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2: Motor skills (pp. 71–120). London: Academic Press. Jordan, M. I. (1996). Computational aspects of motor control and motor learning. In H. Heuer, S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2: Motor skills (pp. 71–120). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Kalveram, K.-Th. (1998). Wie das Individuum mit seiner Umwelt interagiert. Lengerich: Pabst. Kalveram, K.-Th. (1998). Wie das Individuum mit seiner Umwelt interagiert. Lengerich: Pabst.
go back to reference Knoblich, G., & Kircher, T. T. J. (2004). Deceiving oneself about being in control: Conscious detection of changes in visuo-motor coupling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 657–666.PubMedCrossRef Knoblich, G., & Kircher, T. T. J. (2004). Deceiving oneself about being in control: Conscious detection of changes in visuo-motor coupling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 657–666.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Komilis, E., Pelisson, D., Prablanc, C., et al. (1993). Error processing in pointing at randomly feedback-induced double-step stimuli. Journal of Motor Behavior, 25, 299–308.PubMedCrossRef Komilis, E., Pelisson, D., Prablanc, C., et al. (1993). Error processing in pointing at randomly feedback-induced double-step stimuli. Journal of Motor Behavior, 25, 299–308.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F., Mentis, M., Barnes, A., Veytsman, M., Eidelberg, D., et al. (2004). Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 924–933.PubMedCrossRef Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F., Mentis, M., Barnes, A., Veytsman, M., Eidelberg, D., et al. (2004). Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 924–933.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leube, D., Knoblich, G., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Bartels, M., Kircher, T. T. J., et al. (2003). The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements. Neuroimage, 20, 2084–2090.PubMedCrossRef Leube, D., Knoblich, G., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Bartels, M., Kircher, T. T. J., et al. (2003). The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements. Neuroimage, 20, 2084–2090.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mazzoni, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2006). An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3642–3645.PubMedCrossRef Mazzoni, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2006). An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3642–3645.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mechsner, F. (2004). A psychological approach to human voluntary movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 355–370.PubMedCrossRef Mechsner, F. (2004). A psychological approach to human voluntary movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 355–370.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Metzger, W. (1965). Über die Notwendigkeit kybernetischer Vorstellungen in der Theorie des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 171, 336–342.PubMed Metzger, W. (1965). Über die Notwendigkeit kybernetischer Vorstellungen in der Theorie des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 171, 336–342.PubMed
go back to reference Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Sutter, C. (2009). Perceiving one’s own movements when using a tool. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 359–365.PubMedCrossRef Müsseler, J., & Sutter, C. (2009). Perceiving one’s own movements when using a tool. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 359–365.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In: W. Prinz, & A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 255–293). Berlin: Springer. Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In: W. Prinz, & A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 255–293). Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Newell, K. M., & van Emmerik, R. E. A. (1989). The acquisition of coordination: preliminary analysis of learning to write. Human Movement Science, 8, 17–32.CrossRef Newell, K. M., & van Emmerik, R. E. A. (1989). The acquisition of coordination: preliminary analysis of learning to write. Human Movement Science, 8, 17–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Pélisson, D., Prablanc, C., Goodale, M. A., Jeannerod, M., et al. (1986). Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Experimental Brain Research, 62, 303–311.CrossRef Pélisson, D., Prablanc, C., Goodale, M. A., Jeannerod, M., et al. (1986). Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Experimental Brain Research, 62, 303–311.CrossRef
go back to reference Pisella, L., Grea, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., Desmurget, M., Rode, G., et al. (2000). An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 729–736.PubMedCrossRef Pisella, L., Grea, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., Desmurget, M., Rode, G., et al. (2000). An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 729–736.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pisella, L., Sergio, L., Blangero, A., Torchin, H., Vighetto, A., Rossetti, Y., et al. (2009). Optic ataxia and the function of the dorsal stream: contributions to perception and action. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3033–3044.PubMedCrossRef Pisella, L., Sergio, L., Blangero, A., Torchin, H., Vighetto, A., Rossetti, Y., et al. (2009). Optic ataxia and the function of the dorsal stream: contributions to perception and action. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3033–3044.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prablanc, C., & Martin, O. (1992). Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 67, 455–469.PubMed Prablanc, C., & Martin, O. (1992). Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 67, 455–469.PubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1992). Why don’t we perceive our brain states? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1–20.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1992). Why don’t we perceive our brain states? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1993). Adaptive coordination and alignment of eye and hand. Journal of Motor Behavior, 25, 75–88.PubMedCrossRef Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1993). Adaptive coordination and alignment of eye and hand. Journal of Motor Behavior, 25, 75–88.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roll, J. P., & Vedel, J. P. (1982). Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography. Experimental Brain Research, 47, 177–190.CrossRef Roll, J. P., & Vedel, J. P. (1982). Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography. Experimental Brain Research, 47, 177–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L., Pelisson, D., et al. (2000). Eye blindness and hand sight: temporal aspects of visuo-motor processing. Visual Cognition, 7, 785–809.CrossRef Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L., Pelisson, D., et al. (2000). Eye blindness and hand sight: temporal aspects of visuo-motor processing. Visual Cognition, 7, 785–809.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.CrossRef Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A., McGown, C., Quinn, J. T., Hawkins, B., et al. (1986). Unexpected inertial loading in rapid reversal movements: violations of equifinality. Human Movement Science, 5, 263–273.CrossRef Schmidt, R. A., McGown, C., Quinn, J. T., Hawkins, B., et al. (1986). Unexpected inertial loading in rapid reversal movements: violations of equifinality. Human Movement Science, 5, 263–273.CrossRef
go back to reference Slachevsky, A., Pillon, B., Fourneret, P., Pradat-Diehl, P., Jeannerod, M., Dubois, B., et al. (2001). Preserved adjustment but impaired awareness in a sensory-motor conflict following prefrontal lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 332–340.PubMedCrossRef Slachevsky, A., Pillon, B., Fourneret, P., Pradat-Diehl, P., Jeannerod, M., Dubois, B., et al. (2001). Preserved adjustment but impaired awareness in a sensory-motor conflict following prefrontal lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 332–340.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (2006). 10 years of illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1501–1504.PubMedCrossRef Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (2006). 10 years of illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1501–1504.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Striemer, C. L., Yukovsky, J., Goodale, M. A., et al. (2010). Can intention override the “automatic pilot”. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 623–632.CrossRef Striemer, C. L., Yukovsky, J., Goodale, M. A., et al. (2010). Can intention override the “automatic pilot”. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 623–632.CrossRef
go back to reference Sülzenbrück, S., & Heuer, H. (2009a). Learning the visuomotor transformation of virtual and real sliding levers: Simple approximations of complex transformations. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 153–165.CrossRef Sülzenbrück, S., & Heuer, H. (2009a). Learning the visuomotor transformation of virtual and real sliding levers: Simple approximations of complex transformations. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 153–165.CrossRef
go back to reference Sülzenbrück, S., & Heuer, H. (2009b). Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 145–159.PubMedCrossRef Sülzenbrück, S., & Heuer, H. (2009b). Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 145–159.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Trevarthen, C. B. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung, 31, 299–337.PubMedCrossRef Trevarthen, C. B. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung, 31, 299–337.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge: MIT Press. Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Verwey, W. B., & Heuer, H. (2007). Nonlinear visuomotor transformations: locus and modularity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1629–1659.CrossRef Verwey, W. B., & Heuer, H. (2007). Nonlinear visuomotor transformations: locus and modularity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1629–1659.CrossRef
go back to reference Voigt, E. (1933). Über den Aufbau von Bewegungsgestalten. Neue Psychologische Studien, 9, 1–32. Voigt, E. (1933). Über den Aufbau von Bewegungsgestalten. Neue Psychologische Studien, 9, 1–32.
go back to reference West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292.PubMedCrossRef West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.PubMedCrossRef Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11, 1317–1329.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Woollacott, M. H., & Jensen, J. L. (1996). Posture and locomation. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2: Motor skills (pp. 333–403). London: Academic Press. Woollacott, M. H., & Jensen, J. L. (1996). Posture and locomation. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 2: Motor skills (pp. 333–403). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., et al. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8, 547–558.PubMedCrossRef Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., et al. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8, 547–558.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zattara, M., & Bouisset, S. (1986). Chronometric analysis of the posturo-kinetic programming of voluntary movement. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 215–223.PubMed Zattara, M., & Bouisset, S. (1986). Chronometric analysis of the posturo-kinetic programming of voluntary movement. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 215–223.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Mind and movement
Auteurs
Herbert Heuer
Sandra Sülzenbrück
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0332-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2012

Psychological Research 2/2012 Naar de uitgave