Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2015

01-03-2015 | Original Paper

Mental representations derived from spatial descriptions: the influence of orientation specificity and visuospatial abilities

Auteurs: Chiara Meneghetti, Francesca Pazzaglia, Rossana De Beni

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study aimed to investigate the orientation dependence effect and the role of visuospatial abilities in mental representations derived from spatial descriptions. The analysis focused on how the orientation effect and the involvement of visuospatial abilities change when survey and route descriptions are used, and the initial and main orientation of an imaginary tour. In Experiment 1, 48 participants listened to survey or route descriptions in which information was mainly north-oriented (matching the initial heading and main direction of travel expressed in the description). In Experiment 2, 40 participants listened to route descriptions in which the initial orientation (north-oriented) was mismatched with the main direction of travel (east-oriented). Participants performed pointing task while facing north vs south (Exp. 1 and 2), and while facing east vs west (Exp. 2), as well as a map drawing task and several visuospatial measures. In both experiments, the results showed that pointing was easier while facing north than while facing south, and map drawings were arranged with a north-up orientation (with no difference between survey and route descriptions). In Experiment 2, pointing while facing east was easier than in the other pointing conditions. The results obtained with the visuospatial tasks showed that perspective-taking (PT) skill was the main predictor of the ability to imagine positions misaligned with the direction expressed in the descriptions (i.e. pointing while facing south in Experiment 1; pointing while facing north, south or west in Experiment 2). Overall, these findings indicate that mental representations derived from spatial descriptions are specifically oriented and their orientation is influenced by the main direction of travel and by the initial orientation. These mental representations, and the adoption of counter-aligned imaginary orientations, demand visuospatial skills and PT ability in particular.
Voetnoten
1
Males are more accurate in pointing tasks than females (as shown by previous studies; Fields & Shelton, 2006; Pazzaglia & De Beni, 2006), so gender was considered as a factor in the analysis of variance and regression models.
 
2
In a preliminary analysis for Experiment 1, the Leg factor (Leg 1 vs. 2 vs. 3) was included in the ANOVA to check for any differences in pointing performance as a function of the leg involved; no significant effects were found, so this factor was not included in the final analyses.
 
3
Experiment 1. Two separate stepwise regression models were run on pointing while facing north and south for all visuospatial measures; both models were significant [F (2, 45) = 12.04 p ≤ .001 and F (3, 47) = 21.09 p ≤ .001, respectively], explaining 35 % and 59 % of the variance, respectively. In the first stepwise regression (pointing while facing north), the variables entered were MRT (β = −.40; t = −3.28 p ≤ .01) and sense of direction (β = −.36, t = −2.95, p ≤ .01); in the second stepwise regression (pointing while facing south), the variables entered were OPT (β = .43; t = 3.65, p ≤ .001), sense of direction (β = −.28, t = −2.68, p ≤ .01) and MRT (β = −.29, t = −2.63, p ≤ .01).
 
4
Experiment 2. The final 4 × 2 analysis of variance was based on the degrees of error without the segment going from the entrance to the ticket booth in Leg 4. When the same analysis was run including both the segments of Leg 4, the results showed the main effect of orientation F (3, 114) = 10.63, p ≤ .01 η p 2  = .28; post hoc comparisons confirmed that pointing while facing north (M = 32.63, SD 28.34) was easier than pointing while facing south (M = 41.68, SD 34.11) (as reported in the manuscript); on the other hand, pointing while facing east (M = 52.34, SD 17.45) coincided with a worse performance than pointing while facing west (M = 27.15, SD 23.20, p < .01) or north (p = .01) due to the greater error for the segment going from the entrance to the ticket booth.
 
5
Experiment 2. Two separate stepwise regression models were run on pointing while facing north and south for all visuospatial measures, and both models were significant [F (2, 39) = 12.53, p ≤ .001 and F (2, 39) = 14.11, p ≤ .001, respectively], explaining 40 and 43 % of the variance, respectively. In both models, the variables entered were the OPT [pointing while facing north: (β = .40), t = 2.89, p ≤ .01; pointing while facing south: (β = .42), t = 3.16, p ≤ .01], and the SIT [pointing while facing north: (β = −.37), t = −2.66, p = .01; pointing while facing south: (β = −.37), t = −2.72, p = .01]. The two stepwise regression models run on pointing while facing east and west generated significant results only for pointing while facing west, F (1, 39) = 13.05 p ≤ .01, explaining 27 % of the variance; the only variable entered in the model was the OPT [(β = .51), t = 3.61, p ≤ .001].
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bosco, A., Filomena, S., Sardone, L., Scalisi, T. G., & Longoni, A. M. (1996). Spatial models derived from verbal descriptions of fictitious environments: the influence of study time and the individual differences in visuo-spatial ability. Psychologische Beiträge, 38, 451–464. Bosco, A., Filomena, S., Sardone, L., Scalisi, T. G., & Longoni, A. M. (1996). Spatial models derived from verbal descriptions of fictitious environments: the influence of study time and the individual differences in visuo-spatial ability. Psychologische Beiträge, 38, 451–464.
go back to reference Cornoldi, C., Rizzo, A., & Pra Baldi, A. (1991). Prove avanzate MT di comprensione della lettura [Advanced MT reading comprehension task]. Florence: Organizzazioni Speciali. Cornoldi, C., Rizzo, A., & Pra Baldi, A. (1991). Prove avanzate MT di comprensione della lettura [Advanced MT reading comprehension task]. Florence: Organizzazioni Speciali.
go back to reference Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [Doctoral dissertation]. Montreal: McGill University. Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [Doctoral dissertation]. Montreal: McGill University.
go back to reference Gagnon, S. A., Brunyé, T. T., Gardony, A., Noordzij, M. L., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2013). Stepping into a map: initial heading direction influences spatial memory flexibility. Cognitive Science,. doi:10.1111/cogs.12055.PubMed Gagnon, S. A., Brunyé, T. T., Gardony, A., Noordzij, M. L., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2013). Stepping into a map: initial heading direction influences spatial memory flexibility. Cognitive Science,. doi:10.​1111/​cogs.​12055.PubMed
go back to reference Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., Dubois, V., De Beni, R., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2002). Visuo-spatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 665–685. doi:10.1002/acp.823.CrossRef Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., Dubois, V., De Beni, R., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2002). Visuo-spatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 665–685. doi:10.​1002/​acp.​823.CrossRef
go back to reference Gyselinck, V., & Meneghetti, C. (2011). The role of spatial working memory in understanding verbal descriptions: a window onto the interaction between verbal and spatial processing. In A. Vandienrendonck & A. Szmalec (Eds.), Spatial Working Memory (pp. 159–180). Psychology Press, Portfolio. Gyselinck, V., & Meneghetti, C. (2011). The role of spatial working memory in understanding verbal descriptions: a window onto the interaction between verbal and spatial processing. In A. Vandienrendonck & A. Szmalec (Eds.), Spatial Working Memory (pp. 159–180). Psychology Press, Portfolio.
go back to reference Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial ability at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151–176. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005.CrossRef Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial ability at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151–176. doi:10.​1016/​j.​intell.​2005.​09.​005.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. UK: Lawrence Brlbaum Associates Ltd. Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. UK: Lawrence Brlbaum Associates Ltd.
go back to reference Meneghetti, C., Gyselinck, V., Pazzaglia, F., & Beni, De. (2009). Individual differences in spatial text processing: high spatial ability can compensate for spatial working memory interference. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 577–589. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.07.007.CrossRef Meneghetti, C., Gyselinck, V., Pazzaglia, F., & Beni, De. (2009). Individual differences in spatial text processing: high spatial ability can compensate for spatial working memory interference. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 577–589. doi:10.​1016/​j.​lindif.​2009.​07.​007.CrossRef
go back to reference Meneghetti, C., Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2011b). Spatial mental representations derived from survey and route descriptions: when individuals prefer extrinsic frame of reference. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 150–157. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.003.CrossRef Meneghetti, C., Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2011b). Spatial mental representations derived from survey and route descriptions: when individuals prefer extrinsic frame of reference. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 150–157. doi:10.​1016/​j.​lindif.​2010.​12.​003.CrossRef
go back to reference Meneghetti, C., Ronconi, L., Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2013). Spatial mental representation derived from spatial descriptions: the predicting and mediating role of spatial preferences, abilities and strategies. British Journal of Psychology. doi:10.1111/bjop.12038. Meneghetti, C., Ronconi, L., Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2013). Spatial mental representation derived from spatial descriptions: the predicting and mediating role of spatial preferences, abilities and strategies. British Journal of Psychology. doi:10.​1111/​bjop.​12038.
go back to reference Montello, D. R., Waller, D., Hegarty, M., & Richardson, A. E. (2004). Spatial memory of real environments, virtual environments, and maps. In G. Allen (Ed.), Human spatial memory: remembering where (pp. 251–285). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Montello, D. R., Waller, D., Hegarty, M., & Richardson, A. E. (2004). Spatial memory of real environments, virtual environments, and maps. In G. Allen (Ed.), Human spatial memory: remembering where (pp. 251–285). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Morrow, D. G., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Leirer, V. O., Andrassy, J. M., & Kahn, J. (1997). The role of reader age and focus of attention in creating situation models from narratives. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52, 73–80. doi:10.1093/geronb/52B.2.P73.CrossRef Morrow, D. G., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Leirer, V. O., Andrassy, J. M., & Kahn, J. (1997). The role of reader age and focus of attention in creating situation models from narratives. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52, 73–80. doi:10.​1093/​geronb/​52B.​2.​P73.CrossRef
go back to reference Pazzaglia, F. (2008). Text and picture integration in comprehending and memorizing spatial descriptions. In J. F. Rouet & R. K. Lowe (Eds.), Understanding multimedia documents (pp. 43–59). NYC, US: Springer.CrossRef Pazzaglia, F. (2008). Text and picture integration in comprehending and memorizing spatial descriptions. In J. F. Rouet & R. K. Lowe (Eds.), Understanding multimedia documents (pp. 43–59). NYC, US: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2000). Differenze individuali nella rappresentazione dello spazio: presentazione di un questionario autovalutativo [Individual differences in representation of space: presentation of a questionnaire]. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 3, 627–650. doi:10.1421/310. Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2000). Differenze individuali nella rappresentazione dello spazio: presentazione di un questionario autovalutativo [Individual differences in representation of space: presentation of a questionnaire]. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 3, 627–650. doi:10.​1421/​310.
go back to reference Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2001). Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 493–508. doi:10.1080/09541440125778.CrossRef Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2001). Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 493–508. doi:10.​1080/​09541440125778.CrossRef
go back to reference Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2006). Are people with high and low mental rotation abilities differently susceptible to the alignment effect? Perception, 35, 369–383. doi:10.1068/p5465.CrossRefPubMed Pazzaglia, F., & De Beni, R. (2006). Are people with high and low mental rotation abilities differently susceptible to the alignment effect? Perception, 35, 369–383. doi:10.​1068/​p5465.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pazzaglia, F., Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2012). Individual differences in spatial text processing. In V. Gyselinck & F. Pazzaglia (Eds.), From mental imagery to spatial cognition and language: essays in honor of Michel Denis (pp. 127–161). Hove: Psychology Press. Pazzaglia, F., Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2012). Individual differences in spatial text processing. In V. Gyselinck & F. Pazzaglia (Eds.), From mental imagery to spatial cognition and language: essays in honor of Michel Denis (pp. 127–161). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Presson, C. C., DeLange, N., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1987). Orientation specificity in kinesthetic spatial learning: the role of multiple orientations. Memory & Cognition, 15, 225–229. doi:10.3758/BF03197720.CrossRef Presson, C. C., DeLange, N., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1987). Orientation specificity in kinesthetic spatial learning: the role of multiple orientations. Memory & Cognition, 15, 225–229. doi:10.​3758/​BF03197720.CrossRef
go back to reference Presson, C. C., DeLange, N., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1989). Orientation specificity in spatial memory: what makes a path different from a map of the path? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 887–897. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.887.CrossRefPubMed Presson, C. C., DeLange, N., & Hazelrigg, M. D. (1989). Orientation specificity in spatial memory: what makes a path different from a map of the path? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 887–897. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​15.​5.​887.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Richardson, A. E., Montello, D., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments. Memory & Cognition, 27, 741–750. doi:10.3758/BF03211566.CrossRef Richardson, A. E., Montello, D., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments. Memory & Cognition, 27, 741–750. doi:10.​3758/​BF03211566.CrossRef
go back to reference Rinck, M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G. H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The metrics of spatial situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 506–515. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.622. Rinck, M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G. H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The metrics of spatial situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 506–515. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​23.​3.​622.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (rev. ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (rev. ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wilson, P. N., Tlauka, M., & Wildbur, D. (1999). Orientation specificity occurs in both small- and large-scale imagined routes presented as verbal descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 664–679. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.664.CrossRef Wilson, P. N., Tlauka, M., & Wildbur, D. (1999). Orientation specificity occurs in both small- and large-scale imagined routes presented as verbal descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 664–679. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​25.​3.​664.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Mental representations derived from spatial descriptions: the influence of orientation specificity and visuospatial abilities
Auteurs
Chiara Meneghetti
Francesca Pazzaglia
Rossana De Beni
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0560-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2015

Psychological Research 2/2015 Naar de uitgave