Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

25-09-2020 | ORIGINAL PAPER

Meditators Exercise Better Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Awareness

Auteurs: Narayanan Srinivasan, Shradhanjali Tripathi, Ishan Singhal

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 12/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Objectives

Meditation practice has shown improvements in perception, attention, and cognitive control. Here, we study the extent of endogenous and exogenous control meditators have over their visual awareness, when they view a bi-stable image.

Methods

In experiment one, we investigate the differences between meditators and controls in terms of endogenous perceptual control, i.e., when they are instructed to speed up or slow down perceptual switches when viewing a Necker cube. In experiment two, we investigate how stimulus context and flankers (exogenous perceptual control) affect dwell times of visual percepts for meditators and non-meditators.

Results

Our results show that meditators (in comparison with controls) have longer dwell times in the neutral baseline condition. Meditators are also able to speed up and slow down the dwell times of their percepts more (relative to their baseline) than non-meditators. The dwell time distributions of meditators (mean, variance, and skewness) are also immune to flanker effects while viewing bi-stable images.

Conclusions

The results show that meditators have better endogenous (instruction driven) and exogenous (stimulus driven) control compared with non-meditators over the contents and properties of their perceptual experience. The control exhibited by meditators is critical for not only understanding the effects of meditation but points to the need for models of visual perception and cognitive control to incorporate those effects.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 1226–1231.CrossRef Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 1226–1231.CrossRef
go back to reference Atmanspacher, H., Bach, M., Filk, T., & Kornmeier, J. (2008). Cognitive time scales in a Necker-Zeno model for bistable perception. The Open Cybernetics & Systemics Journal, 2(1), 234-251.CrossRef Atmanspacher, H., Bach, M., Filk, T., & Kornmeier, J. (2008). Cognitive time scales in a Necker-Zeno model for bistable perception. The Open Cybernetics & Systemics Journal, 2(1), 234-251.CrossRef
go back to reference Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 47–52.CrossRef Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 47–52.CrossRef
go back to reference Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Types of attention matter for awareness: A study with color afterimages. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 1039-1048.CrossRef Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Types of attention matter for awareness: A study with color afterimages. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 1039-1048.CrossRef
go back to reference Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). Theta activity and meditative states: spectral changes during concentrative meditation. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 31–38.CrossRef Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). Theta activity and meditative states: spectral changes during concentrative meditation. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 31–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21.CrossRef Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21.CrossRef
go back to reference Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 272–272.CrossRef Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 272–272.CrossRef
go back to reference Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature neuroscience, 7(3), 308–313.CrossRef Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature neuroscience, 7(3), 308–313.CrossRef
go back to reference Carter, O. L., Presti, D. E., Callistemon, C., Ungerer, Y., Liu, G. B., & Pettigrew, J. D. (2005). Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Tibetan Buddhist monks. Current Biology, 15(11), R412–R413.CrossRef Carter, O. L., Presti, D. E., Callistemon, C., Ungerer, Y., Liu, G. B., & Pettigrew, J. D. (2005). Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Tibetan Buddhist monks. Current Biology, 15(11), R412–R413.CrossRef
go back to reference Dienes, Z. (2008). Understanding psychology as a science: An introduction to scientific and statistical inference. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Dienes, Z. (2008). Understanding psychology as a science: An introduction to scientific and statistical inference. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
go back to reference Gigante, G., Mattia, M., Braun, J., & Del Giudice, P. (2009). Bistable perception modeled as competing stochastic integrations at two levels. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(7), e1000430.CrossRef Gigante, G., Mattia, M., Braun, J., & Del Giudice, P. (2009). Bistable perception modeled as competing stochastic integrations at two levels. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(7), e1000430.CrossRef
go back to reference Hodgins, H. S., & Adair, K. C. (2010). Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 872–878.CrossRef Hodgins, H. S., & Adair, K. C. (2010). Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 872–878.CrossRef
go back to reference Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.CrossRef Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Kawabata, N. (1986). Attention and depth perception. Perception, 15(5), 563–572.CrossRef Kawabata, N. (1986). Attention and depth perception. Perception, 15(5), 563–572.CrossRef
go back to reference Kornmeier, J., Friedel, E., Wittmann, M., & Atmanspacher, H. (2017). EEG correlates of cognitive time scales in the Necker-Zeno model for bistable perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 136–150.CrossRef Kornmeier, J., Friedel, E., Wittmann, M., & Atmanspacher, H. (2017). EEG correlates of cognitive time scales in the Necker-Zeno model for bistable perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 136–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Liebert, R. M., & Burk, B. (1985). Voluntary control of reversible figures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61(3), 1307–1310.CrossRef Liebert, R. M., & Burk, B. (1985). Voluntary control of reversible figures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61(3), 1307–1310.CrossRef
go back to reference Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012) OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012) OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324.
go back to reference MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., & Wallace, B. A. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21(6), 829–839.CrossRef MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., & Wallace, B. A. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21(6), 829–839.CrossRef
go back to reference Meng, M., & Tong, F. (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision, 4(7), 539-551. Meng, M., & Tong, F. (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision, 4(7), 539-551.
go back to reference Mishra, M. V., & Srinivasan, N. (2017). Exogenous attention intensifies perceived emotion expressions. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), nix022 Mishra, M. V., & Srinivasan, N. (2017). Exogenous attention intensifies perceived emotion expressions. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), nix022
go back to reference Moreno-Bote, R., Rinzel, J., & Rubin, N. (2007). Noise-induced alternations in an attractor network model of perceptual bistability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98(3), 1125–1139.CrossRef Moreno-Bote, R., Rinzel, J., & Rubin, N. (2007). Noise-induced alternations in an attractor network model of perceptual bistability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98(3), 1125–1139.CrossRef
go back to reference Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (1999). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness: the role of attention. Perception, 28(5), 551–574.CrossRef Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (1999). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness: the role of attention. Perception, 28(5), 551–574.CrossRef
go back to reference Pöppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(2), 56-61.CrossRef Pöppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(2), 56-61.CrossRef
go back to reference Poppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiologia Experimentalis, 64(3), 295–302. Poppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiologia Experimentalis, 64(3), 295–302.
go back to reference Sauer, S., Lemke, J., Wittmann, M., Kohls, N., Mochty, U., & Walach, H. (2012). How long is now for mindfulness meditators? Personality and Individual Differences, 52(6), 750–754.CrossRef Sauer, S., Lemke, J., Wittmann, M., Kohls, N., Mochty, U., & Walach, H. (2012). How long is now for mindfulness meditators? Personality and Individual Differences, 52(6), 750–754.CrossRef
go back to reference Singh, A., Pammi, V. C., Guleria, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2019). Concentrative (Sahaj Samadhi) meditation training and visual awareness: an fMRI study on color afterimages. Progress in Brain Research, 244, 185–206.CrossRef Singh, A., Pammi, V. C., Guleria, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2019). Concentrative (Sahaj Samadhi) meditation training and visual awareness: an fMRI study on color afterimages. Progress in Brain Research, 244, 185–206.CrossRef
go back to reference Srinivasan, N., & Baijal, S. (2007). Concentrative meditation enhances preattentive processing: a mismatch negativity study. Neuroreport, 18(16), 1709–1712.CrossRef Srinivasan, N., & Baijal, S. (2007). Concentrative meditation enhances preattentive processing: a mismatch negativity study. Neuroreport, 18(16), 1709–1712.CrossRef
go back to reference Srinivasan, N., & Singh, A. (2017). Concentrative meditation influences visual awareness: a study with color afterimages. Mindfulness, 8(1), 17–26.CrossRef Srinivasan, N., & Singh, A. (2017). Concentrative meditation influences visual awareness: a study with color afterimages. Mindfulness, 8(1), 17–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Sundareswara, R., & Schrater, P. R. (2008). Perceptual multistability predicted by search model for Bayesian decisions. Journal of Vision, 8(5), 12.1–19. Sundareswara, R., & Schrater, P. R. (2008). Perceptual multistability predicted by search model for Bayesian decisions. Journal of Vision, 8(5), 12.1–19.
go back to reference Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., & Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(7), 450–461.CrossRef Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., & Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(7), 450–461.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Ee, R., Noest, A. J., Brascamp, J. W., & van den Berg, A. (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts of ambiguous stimuli revealed by a two-parameter analysis: means do not make the difference. Vision Research, 46(19), 3129–3141.CrossRef Van Ee, R., Noest, A. J., Brascamp, J. W., & van den Berg, A. (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts of ambiguous stimuli revealed by a two-parameter analysis: means do not make the difference. Vision Research, 46(19), 3129–3141.CrossRef
go back to reference van Leeuwen, C., & Smit, D. J. A. (2012). Restless minds, wandering brains. In S. Edelman, T. Fekete, & N. Zach (Eds.), Being in time: Dynamical models of phenomenal experience (pp. 121–148). John Benjamins. van Leeuwen, C., & Smit, D. J. A. (2012). Restless minds, wandering brains. In S. Edelman, T. Fekete, & N. Zach (Eds.),  Being in time: Dynamical models of phenomenal experience (pp. 121–148). John Benjamins.
Metagegevens
Titel
Meditators Exercise Better Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Awareness
Auteurs
Narayanan Srinivasan
Shradhanjali Tripathi
Ishan Singhal
Publicatiedatum
25-09-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 12/2020
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01496-2