Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 4/2008

01-05-2008

Maternal Modeling and the Acquisition of Fear and Avoidance in Toddlers: Influence of Stimulus Preparedness and Child Temperament

Auteurs: Kathrin Dubi, Ronald M. Rapee, Jane L. Emerton, Carolyn A. Schniering

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 4/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of maternal modeling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance towards fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant, novel stimuli in a sample of 71 toddlers. Children were shown a rubber snake or spider (fear-relevant objects) and a rubber mushroom or flower (fear-irrelevant objects), which were alternately paired with either negative or positive expression by their mothers. Both stimuli were presented again after a 1- and a 10-min delay, while mothers maintained a neutral expression. The toddlers showed increased fear and avoidance of the objects following negative reaction from their mothers than following positive maternal expression. This was similarly true for both fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli. In addition, no association was found between child temperament and behavioral responses and a weak association emerged between child temperament and emotional responses. The results demonstrate that young children can rapidly form conditioned emotional and behavioral responses via maternal reactions regardless of stimulus preparedness or child temperament. It is suggested that early maternal modeling may be relevant to a broad spectrum of fearful reactions.
Voetnoten
1
At this stage, the mothers were only instructed and given written suggestions on how to display appropriate emotions but these instructions and suggestions were not paired with any of the stimuli. Moreover, the names of the stimuli were not mentioned at any time, the mothers and the experimenter did not face the toddlers, and the stimuli were disguised in the box. Consequently, it is very unlikely that any incidental learning (via hearing or seeing) could have occurred.
 
2
The order and choice of the fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant objects were randomized and counterbalanced between participants. However, the researchers checked for any possible influence of order of modeling condition (positive or negative modeling first) and of type of stimulus (flower, mushroom, snake, or spider). Few interactions were significant and these were difficult to interpret. As there were no significant interactions with the variables of interest, order of modeling and type of stimulus were not considered any further.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Andrews, G., Hall, W., Teesson, M., & Henderson, S. (1999). The mental health of Australians. Canberra, Australia: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care. Andrews, G., Hall, W., Teesson, M., & Henderson, S. (1999). The mental health of Australians. Canberra, Australia: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care.
go back to reference Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10, 176–181.CrossRef Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10, 176–181.CrossRef
go back to reference Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2007). Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2616–2627.PubMedCrossRef Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2007). Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2616–2627.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (1994). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In C. Lewis, & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind (pp. 133–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (1994). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In C. Lewis, & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind (pp. 133–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. (1969). Relative efficacy of desensitization and modeling approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and attitudinal changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 173–199.PubMedCrossRef Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. (1969). Relative efficacy of desensitization and modeling approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and attitudinal changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 173–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. (1967). Vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 16–23.PubMedCrossRef Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. (1967). Vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 16–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1966). Vicarious classical conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 54–62.PubMedCrossRef Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1966). Vicarious classical conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 54–62.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Belsky, J., & Park, S. Y. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and child effects in the case of child inhibition in US and Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 338–347.CrossRef Belsky, J., & Park, S. Y. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and child effects in the case of child inhibition in US and Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 338–347.CrossRef
go back to reference Bernstein, G. A., Borchardt, C. M., & Perwien, A. R. (1996). Symptoms of anxiety disorders in normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 759–765. Bernstein, G. A., Borchardt, C. M., & Perwien, A. R. (1996). Symptoms of anxiety disorders in normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 759–765.
go back to reference Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition. Child Development, 74, 1899–1917.PubMedCrossRef Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition. Child Development, 74, 1899–1917.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blackford, J. U., & Walden, T. A. (1998). Individual differences in social referencing. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 89–102.CrossRef Blackford, J. U., & Walden, T. A. (1998). Individual differences in social referencing. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 89–102.CrossRef
go back to reference Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2 (Cognition, perception, and language. (5th ed.) pp. 309–370. New York: Wiley. Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2 (Cognition, perception, and language. (5th ed.) pp. 309–370. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 79–89.PubMedCrossRef Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 79–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Capps, L., Sigman, M., Sena, R., Henker, B., & Whalen, C. (1996). Fear, anxiety and perceived control in children of agoraphobic parents. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 445–452.CrossRef Capps, L., Sigman, M., Sena, R., Henker, B., & Whalen, C. (1996). Fear, anxiety and perceived control in children of agoraphobic parents. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 445–452.CrossRef
go back to reference Cicchetti, D. V., & Sparrow, S. S. (1981). Developing criteria for establishing the interrater reliability of specific items in a given inventory. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86, 127–137.PubMed Cicchetti, D. V., & Sparrow, S. S. (1981). Developing criteria for establishing the interrater reliability of specific items in a given inventory. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86, 127–137.PubMed
go back to reference Cook, E. W., Hodes, R. L., & Lang, P. J. (1986). Preparedness and phobia: Effect of stimulus content on human visceral conditioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 195–207.PubMedCrossRef Cook, E. W., Hodes, R. L., & Lang, P. J. (1986). Preparedness and phobia: Effect of stimulus content on human visceral conditioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 195–207.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448–459.PubMedCrossRef Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 372–389.CrossRef Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 372–389.CrossRef
go back to reference De Jong, P. J., Andrea, H., & Muris, P. (1997). Spider phobia in children: Disgust and fear before and after treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 559–562.PubMedCrossRef De Jong, P. J., Andrea, H., & Muris, P. (1997). Spider phobia in children: Disgust and fear before and after treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 559–562.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175.PubMedCrossRef De Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dimberg, U. (1986). Facial reactions to fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli. Biological Psychology, 23, 153–161.PubMedCrossRef Dimberg, U. (1986). Facial reactions to fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli. Biological Psychology, 23, 153–161.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Egliston, K.-A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1871–1882.PubMedCrossRef Egliston, K.-A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1871–1882.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A. (1993). Somatic symptoms and panic attacks: A retrospective study of learning experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 269–278.PubMedCrossRef Ehlers, A. (1993). Somatic symptoms and panic attacks: A retrospective study of learning experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 269–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (Serial No. 242). Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (Serial No. 242).
go back to reference Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. (1993). Prevalence and comorbidity of DSM-III diagnoses in a birth cohort of 15 year olds. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1127–1134.PubMedCrossRef Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. (1993). Prevalence and comorbidity of DSM-III diagnoses in a birth cohort of 15 year olds. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1127–1134.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Field, A. P. (2006). The behavioral inhibition system and the verbal information pathway to children’s fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 35, 337–345. Field, A. P. (2006). The behavioral inhibition system and the verbal information pathway to children’s fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 35, 337–345.
go back to reference Field, A. P., Argyris, N. G., & Knowles, K. A. (2001). Who’s afraid of the big bad wolf: A prospective paradigm to test Rachman’s indirect pathways in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1259–1276.PubMedCrossRef Field, A. P., Argyris, N. G., & Knowles, K. A. (2001). Who’s afraid of the big bad wolf: A prospective paradigm to test Rachman’s indirect pathways in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1259–1276.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Field, A. P., Hamilton, S. J., Knowles, K. A., & Plews, E. L. (2003). Fear information and social phobic beliefs in children: A prospective paradigm and preliminary results. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 113–123.PubMedCrossRef Field, A. P., Hamilton, S. J., Knowles, K. A., & Plews, E. L. (2003). Fear information and social phobic beliefs in children: A prospective paradigm and preliminary results. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 113–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Field, A. P., & Lawson, J. (2003). Fear information and the development of fears during childhood: Effects on implicit fear responses and behavioural avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1277–1293.PubMedCrossRef Field, A. P., & Lawson, J. (2003). Fear information and the development of fears during childhood: Effects on implicit fear responses and behavioural avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1277–1293.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Fullard, W. B., McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1984). Assessing temperament in one- to three-year old children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9, 205–216.PubMedCrossRef Fullard, W. B., McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1984). Assessing temperament in one- to three-year old children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9, 205–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019.CrossRef Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modeling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behavior in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279–287.PubMedCrossRef Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modeling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behavior in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279–287.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1984). The effects of positive maternal affect on infant responses to pleasant, ambiguous, and fear-provoking toys. Child Development, 55, 1231–1236.CrossRef Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1984). The effects of positive maternal affect on infant responses to pleasant, ambiguous, and fear-provoking toys. Child Development, 55, 1231–1236.CrossRef
go back to reference Hirshberg, L. M., & Svejda, M. (1990). When infants look to their parents: I. Infants’ social referencing of mothers compared to fathers. Child Development, 61, 1175–1186.PubMedCrossRef Hirshberg, L. M., & Svejda, M. (1990). When infants look to their parents: I. Infants’ social referencing of mothers compared to fathers. Child Development, 61, 1175–1186.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hornik, R., & Gunnar, M. R. (1988). A descriptive analysis of infant social referencing. Child Development, 59, 626–634.PubMedCrossRef Hornik, R., & Gunnar, M. R. (1988). A descriptive analysis of infant social referencing. Child Development, 59, 626–634.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. R. (1987). The effect of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.CrossRef Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. R. (1987). The effect of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.CrossRef
go back to reference Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2004). From anxious temperament to disorder: An etiological model of generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 51–74). New York: Guilford. Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2004). From anxious temperament to disorder: An etiological model of generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 51–74). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225.CrossRef Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225.CrossRef
go back to reference Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473.PubMedCrossRef Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.PubMed Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.PubMed
go back to reference King, N. J., Gullone, E., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Etiology of childhood phobias: Current status of Rachman’s three pathways theory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 297–309.PubMedCrossRef King, N. J., Gullone, E., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Etiology of childhood phobias: Current status of Rachman’s three pathways theory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 297–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 447–465.CrossRef Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 447–465.CrossRef
go back to reference Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.CrossRef Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.CrossRef
go back to reference Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137–159.PubMedCrossRef Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137–159.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995a). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation. Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995a). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation.
go back to reference Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995b). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–342.PubMedCrossRef Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995b). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marks, I. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders. New York: Oxford University Press. Marks, I. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference McNally, R., & Reiss, G. (1982). The preparedness theory of phobias and human safety-signal conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20, 153–159.PubMedCrossRef McNally, R., & Reiss, G. (1982). The preparedness theory of phobias and human safety-signal conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20, 153–159.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of childhood water phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 499–501.PubMedCrossRef Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of childhood water phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 499–501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). The etiology of phobias: A nonassociative account. Clinical Psychological Review, 15, 23–48.CrossRef Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). The etiology of phobias: A nonassociative account. Clinical Psychological Review, 15, 23–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Merckelbach, H., De Jong, P. J., Muris, P., & Van den Hout, M. A. (1996). The etiology of specific phobias: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 337–361.CrossRef Merckelbach, H., De Jong, P. J., Muris, P., & Van den Hout, M. A. (1996). The etiology of specific phobias: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 337–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., & Schouten, E. (1996). Pathways to fear in spider phobic children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 935–938.PubMedCrossRef Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., & Schouten, E. (1996). Pathways to fear in spider phobic children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 935–938.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mineka, S. (1985). Animal models of anxiety-based disorders: Their usefulness and limitations. In A. Tuma, & J. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 199–244). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mineka, S. (1985). Animal models of anxiety-based disorders: Their usefulness and limitations. In A. Tuma, & J. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 199–244). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122, 23–38.PubMed Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122, 23–38.PubMed
go back to reference Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372.PubMedCrossRef Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 927–937.PubMedCrossRef Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 927–937.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (1996). Conditioning and ethological models of anxiety disorders: Stress-in-dynamic-context anxiety models. In D. Hope (Ed.), Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fear (43rd annual Nebraska symposium on motivation). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (1996). Conditioning and ethological models of anxiety disorders: Stress-in-dynamic-context anxiety models. In D. Hope (Ed.), Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fear (43rd annual Nebraska symposium on motivation). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
go back to reference Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718–735.PubMedCrossRef Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718–735.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infant’s responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237.PubMedCrossRef Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infant’s responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Muris, P., Bodden, D., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. (2003). Fear of the beast: A prospective study on the effects of negative information on childhood fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 195–208.PubMed Muris, P., Bodden, D., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. (2003). Fear of the beast: A prospective study on the effects of negative information on childhood fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 195–208.PubMed
go back to reference Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (2001). The etiology of childhood specific phobia: A multifactorial model. In M. W. Vasey, & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 355–385). New York: Oxford University Press. Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (2001). The etiology of childhood specific phobia: A multifactorial model. In M. W. Vasey, & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 355–385). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Merckelbach, H., & Meesters, C. (1996). The role of parental fearfulness and modeling in children’s fears. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 265–268.PubMedCrossRef Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Merckelbach, H., & Meesters, C. (1996). The role of parental fearfulness and modeling in children’s fears. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 265–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Murray, L., Cooper, P. J., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., & Sack, C. (in press). Intergenerational transmission of maternal social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes. Child Development. Murray, L., Cooper, P. J., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., & Sack, C. (in press). Intergenerational transmission of maternal social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes. Child Development.
go back to reference Neal, J. A., Edelmann, R. J., & Glachan, M. (2002). Behavioral inhibition and symptoms of anxiety and depression: Is there a specific relationship with social phobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 361–374.PubMedCrossRef Neal, J. A., Edelmann, R. J., & Glachan, M. (2002). Behavioral inhibition and symptoms of anxiety and depression: Is there a specific relationship with social phobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 361–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A. (1979). Fear relevance, autonomic conditioning, and phobias: A laboratory model. In P.-O. Sjöden, S. Bates, & W. S. Dockens III (Eds.), Trends in behavior therapy (pp. 107–134). New York: Academic Press. Öhman, A. (1979). Fear relevance, autonomic conditioning, and phobias: A laboratory model. In P.-O. Sjöden, S. Bates, & W. S. Dockens III (Eds.), Trends in behavior therapy (pp. 107–134). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Öhman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23, 123–145.PubMedCrossRef Öhman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23, 123–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A. (1993). Stimulus prepotency and fear learning: Data and theory. In N. Birbaumer, & A. Öhman (Eds.), The structure of emotion: Psychophysiological, cognitive, and clinical aspects (pp. 218–239). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Öhman, A. (1993). Stimulus prepotency and fear learning: Data and theory. In N. Birbaumer, & A. Öhman (Eds.), The structure of emotion: Psychophysiological, cognitive, and clinical aspects (pp. 218–239). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
go back to reference Öhman, A., Dimberg, U., & Öst, L.-G. (1985). Biological constraints on the learned fear response. In S. Reiss, & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 123–175). New York: Academic. Öhman, A., Dimberg, U., & Öst, L.-G. (1985). Biological constraints on the learned fear response. In S. Reiss, & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 123–175). New York: Academic.
go back to reference Öhman, A., Eriksson, A., & Olofsson, C. (1975). One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 619–627.PubMedCrossRef Öhman, A., Eriksson, A., & Olofsson, C. (1975). One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 619–627.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hugdahl, K., & Rimmö, P.-A. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 313–337.CrossRef Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hugdahl, K., & Rimmö, P.-A. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 313–337.CrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Hamm, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2000). Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: Orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (pp. 522–575). New York: Cambridge University Press. Öhman, A., Hamm, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2000). Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: Orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (pp. 522–575). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.PubMedCrossRef Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1991). Origins of childhood fears: An evaluation of Rachman’s theory of fear acquisition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 117–123.PubMedCrossRef Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1991). Origins of childhood fears: An evaluation of Rachman’s theory of fear acquisition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 117–123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öst, L. G. (1987). Age of onset in different phobias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 223–229.PubMedCrossRef Öst, L. G. (1987). Age of onset in different phobias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 223–229.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Öst, L. G., & Hugdahl, K. (1981). Acquisition of phobias and anxiety response pattern in clinical patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 439–447.PubMedCrossRef Öst, L. G., & Hugdahl, K. (1981). Acquisition of phobias and anxiety response pattern in clinical patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 439–447.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported temperament from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998–1007.CrossRef Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported temperament from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998–1007.CrossRef
go back to reference Pine, D. S., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S., Moulton, J. L., Lissek, S., Guardino, M., et al. (2005). Face-emotion processing in offspring at risk for panic disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 664–672.PubMedCrossRef Pine, D. S., Klein, R. G., Mannuzza, S., Moulton, J. L., Lissek, S., Guardino, M., et al. (2005). Face-emotion processing in offspring at risk for panic disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 664–672.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prior, M., Sanson, A., Oberklaid, F., & Northam, E. (1987). Measurement of temperament in one to three year old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 121–132. Prior, M., Sanson, A., Oberklaid, F., & Northam, E. (1987). Measurement of temperament in one to three year old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 121–132.
go back to reference Prior, M., Smart, D., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2000). Pathways from infancy to adolescence: Australian Temperament Project 1983–2000. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies. Prior, M., Smart, D., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2000). Pathways from infancy to adolescence: Australian Temperament Project 1983–2000. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies.
go back to reference Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acquisition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375–387.PubMedCrossRef Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acquisition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375–387.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rachman, S. (1991). Neoconditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 47–67. Rachman, S. (1991). Neoconditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 47–67.
go back to reference Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized anxiety disorder. In M. W. Vasey, & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 481–503). New York: Oxford University Press. Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized anxiety disorder. In M. W. Vasey, & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 481–503). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Rapee, R. M., & Spence, S. H. (2004). The etiology of social phobia: Empirical evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 737–767.PubMedCrossRef Rapee, R. M., & Spence, S. H. (2004). The etiology of social phobia: Empirical evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 737–767.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infant’s use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person’s emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.PubMedCrossRef Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infant’s use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person’s emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reznick, J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibited and uninhibited children: A follow-up study. Child Development, 57, 660–680.CrossRef Reznick, J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibited and uninhibited children: A follow-up study. Child Development, 57, 660–680.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosen, W. D., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1992). An experimental investigation of infant social referencing: Mothers’ messages and gender differences. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1172–1178.CrossRef Rosen, W. D., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1992). An experimental investigation of infant social referencing: Mothers’ messages and gender differences. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1172–1178.CrossRef
go back to reference Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., Westendorf, D. H., Meunier, S. A., & Tolin, D. F. (2002). Emotional responding to fearful and disgusting stimuli in specific phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1031–1046.PubMedCrossRef Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., Westendorf, D. H., Meunier, S. A., & Tolin, D. F. (2002). Emotional responding to fearful and disgusting stimuli in specific phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1031–1046.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.CrossRef Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.CrossRef
go back to reference Sewell, J., Oberklaid, F., Prior, M., Sanson, A., & Kyrios, M. (1988). Temperament in Australian toddlers. Australian Pediatric Journal, 24, 343–345. Sewell, J., Oberklaid, F., Prior, M., Sanson, A., & Kyrios, M. (1988). Temperament in Australian toddlers. Australian Pediatric Journal, 24, 343–345.
go back to reference Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.CrossRefPubMed Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Siegler, R., DeLoache, J., & Eisenberg, N. (2003). How children develop. New York: Worth. Siegler, R., DeLoache, J., & Eisenberg, N. (2003). How children develop. New York: Worth.
go back to reference Thunberg, M., & Dimberg, U. (2000). Gender differences in facial reactions to fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 45–51.CrossRef Thunberg, M., & Dimberg, U. (2000). Gender differences in facial reactions to fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 45–51.CrossRef
go back to reference Verhulst, F. C., Van der Ende, J., Ferdinand, R. F., & Kasius, M. C. (1997). The prevalence of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a national sample of Dutch adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 329–336.PubMed Verhulst, F. C., Van der Ende, J., Ferdinand, R. F., & Kasius, M. C. (1997). The prevalence of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a national sample of Dutch adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 329–336.PubMed
go back to reference Watt, M. C., & Stewart, S. H. (2000). Anxiety sensitivity mediates the relationships between childhood learning experiences and elevated hypochondriacal concerns in young adulthood. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 49, 107–118.PubMedCrossRef Watt, M. C., & Stewart, S. H. (2000). Anxiety sensitivity mediates the relationships between childhood learning experiences and elevated hypochondriacal concerns in young adulthood. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 49, 107–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watt, M. C., Stewart, S. H., & Cox, B. J. (1998). A retrospective study of the learning history origins of anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 505–525.PubMedCrossRef Watt, M. C., Stewart, S. H., & Cox, B. J. (1998). A retrospective study of the learning history origins of anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 505–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weems, C. F., & Stickle, T. R. (2005). Anxiety disorders in childhood: Casting a nomological net. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 107–134.PubMedCrossRef Weems, C. F., & Stickle, T. R. (2005). Anxiety disorders in childhood: Casting a nomological net. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 107–134.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Maternal Modeling and the Acquisition of Fear and Avoidance in Toddlers: Influence of Stimulus Preparedness and Child Temperament
Auteurs
Kathrin Dubi
Ronald M. Rapee
Jane L. Emerton
Carolyn A. Schniering
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 4/2008
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9195-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2008

Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 4/2008 Naar de uitgave