Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies 6/2021

27-04-2021 | Original Paper

Listening In: How Parent-Child Communication Relates to Social and Physical Aggression

Auteurs: Diana J. Meter, Samuel E. Ehrenreich, Kurt Beron, Marion K. Underwood

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies | Uitgave 6/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In this study, we investigated the relations between features of parent-child conversations (neutral talk, positive and negative in-dyad and out-dyad talk) and children’s social and physical aggression from ages 9–18. Participants were 297 youth (52% girls) of about 9 years old at Time 1 and their parent. Fifty-two percent of this United States sample identified as White, 20% Black, 20% Hispanic, 8% other races/ethnicities. One hundred eighty-seven parents participated in the parent-child observation task. Ninety four percent of parent participants were mothers. Parent-child conversations were observed in the laboratory during preadolescence, and teachers reported child’s aggression. Using multinomial logit analyses, we found that coded observations of communication features predicted membership in linear trajectories of social and physical aggression across nine years of adolescence; trajectories were derived via mixture modeling. Parent and child communication characteristics were related to trajectories of aggression that spanned preadolescence and adolescence; however, not all predicted associations were significant. Children’s talk about neutral topics predicted being on a lower social aggression trajectory. Positive out-dyad talk from children was related to being on a lower physical aggression trajectory, as was parent in-dyad positive talk. After controlling for other factors, neither parent nor child in- or out-dyad negative talk was associated with social or physical aggression. These findings highlight the importance of positive communication by youth and toward youth in association with long-term social adjustment.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Aspland, H., & Gardner, F. (2003). Observational measures of parent‐child interaction: An introductory review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(3), 136–143.CrossRefPubMed Aspland, H., & Gardner, F. (2003). Observational measures of parent‐child interaction: An introductory review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(3), 136–143.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. General Learning Press. Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. General Learning Press.
go back to reference Björkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles, 30(3-4), 177–188.CrossRef Björkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles, 30(3-4), 177–188.CrossRef
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 719–788). John Wiley & Sons. Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 719–788). John Wiley & Sons.
go back to reference Ehrenreich, S. E., Beron, K. J., & Underwood, M. K. (2016). Social and physical aggression trajectories from middle childhood through late adolescence as predictors of maladjustment at age 18: Rule-breaking behaviors, internalizing symptoms, and borderline and narcissistic features. Developmental Psychology, 52(3), 457–462. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000094.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ehrenreich, S. E., Beron, K. J., & Underwood, M. K. (2016). Social and physical aggression trajectories from middle childhood through late adolescence as predictors of maladjustment at age 18: Rule-breaking behaviors, internalizing symptoms, and borderline and narcissistic features. Developmental Psychology, 52(3), 457–462. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​dev0000094.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Fanti, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. (2010). Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: Findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1159–1175. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020659.CrossRefPubMed Fanti, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. (2010). Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: Findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1159–1175. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0020659.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kahlbaugh, P. E., & Haviland, J. M. (1994). Nonverbal communication between parents and adolescents: A study of approach and avoidance behaviors. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18(1), 91–113.CrossRef Kahlbaugh, P. E., & Haviland, J. M. (1994). Nonverbal communication between parents and adolescents: A study of approach and avoidance behaviors. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18(1), 91–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Karriker‐Jaffe, K. J., Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T. & & Suchindran, C. (2008). The development of aggression during adolescence: Sex differences in trajectories of physical and social aggression among youth in rural areas. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1227–1236.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Karriker‐Jaffe, K. J., Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T. & & Suchindran, C. (2008). The development of aggression during adolescence: Sex differences in trajectories of physical and social aggression among youth in rural areas. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1227–1236.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Ladd, G. W. (2005). Children’s peer relations and social competence: A century of progress. Yale University Press. Ladd, G. W. (2005). Children’s peer relations and social competence: A century of progress. Yale University Press.
go back to reference Ladd, G. W., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Parenting and the development of children’s peer relationships. In: M. H. Bornstein, (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (Vol. 5, pp. 269–309). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ladd, G. W., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Parenting and the development of children’s peer relationships. In: M. H. Bornstein, (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (Vol. 5, pp. 269–309). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Lambert, S. F., & Cashwell, C. S. (2004). Preteens talking to parents: Perceived communication and school-based aggression. The Family Journal, 12(2), 122–128.CrossRef Lambert, S. F., & Cashwell, C. S. (2004). Preteens talking to parents: Perceived communication and school-based aggression. The Family Journal, 12(2), 122–128.CrossRef
go back to reference Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2529310 http://www.jstor.org/stable/2529310CrossRefPubMed Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​2529310 http://www.jstor.org/stable/2529310CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lytton, H. (1973). Three approaches to the study of parent‐child interaction: ethological, interview and experimental. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(1), 1–17.CrossRefPubMed Lytton, H. (1973). Three approaches to the study of parent‐child interaction: ethological, interview and experimental. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(1), 1–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McKechnie, L. E. F. (2012). Reactivity. In L. M. Given (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of qualitative research methods, (p. 730). SAGE Publications. McKechnie, L. E. F. (2012). Reactivity. In L. M. Given (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of qualitative research methods, (p. 730). SAGE Publications.
go back to reference Muthén, B. O., Muthén, L. K., & Asparouhov, T. (2016). Regression and mediation analysis using Mplus. Muthén & Muthén. Muthén, B. O., Muthén, L. K., & Asparouhov, T. (2016). Regression and mediation analysis using Mplus. Muthén & Muthén.
go back to reference Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Muthén & Muthén. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
go back to reference Nagin, D. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Harvard University Press. Nagin, D. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Gender differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer victimization: Social and physical aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 242–266. Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Gender differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer victimization: Social and physical aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 242–266.
go back to reference Sillars, A. L. (1991). Behavioral observation. In B. M. Montgomery & S. Duck, (Eds). Studying interpersonal interaction, (pp. 197-218). Guilford Press. Sillars, A. L. (1991). Behavioral observation. In B. M. Montgomery & S. Duck, (Eds). Studying interpersonal interaction, (pp. 197-218). Guilford Press.
go back to reference Snyder, J. J. (2016). Coercive family processes and the development of child social behavior and self-regulation. In Dishion, T. J. & Snyder, J. J. (Eds). The oxford handbook of coercive relationship dynamics, (pp. 101-113). Oxford University Press. Snyder, J. J. (2016). Coercive family processes and the development of child social behavior and self-regulation. In Dishion, T. J. & Snyder, J. J. (Eds). The oxford handbook of coercive relationship dynamics, (pp. 101-113). Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. Guilford Press. Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. Guilford Press.
go back to reference Underwood, M. K., Beron, K. J., Gentsch, J. K., Galperin, M. B., & Risser, S. D. (2008). Family correlates of children’s social and physical aggression with peers: Negative interparental conflict strategies and parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(6), 549–562. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408097134.CrossRef Underwood, M. K., Beron, K. J., Gentsch, J. K., Galperin, M. B., & Risser, S. D. (2008). Family correlates of children’s social and physical aggression with peers: Negative interparental conflict strategies and parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(6), 549–562. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​0165025408097134​.CrossRef
go back to reference Vaillancourt, T. (2005). Indirect aggression among humans: Social construct of evolutionary adaptation? In R. E. Tremblay, W.W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 158-177). Guilford Press. Vaillancourt, T. (2005). Indirect aggression among humans: Social construct of evolutionary adaptation? In R. E. Tremblay, W.W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 158-177). Guilford Press.
go back to reference Vaillancourt, T., Miller, J. L., Fagbemi, J., Coté, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Trajectories and predictors of indirect aggression: Results from a nationally representative longitudinal study of Canadian children aged 2–10. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 314–326. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20202.CrossRefPubMed Vaillancourt, T., Miller, J. L., Fagbemi, J., Coté, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Trajectories and predictors of indirect aggression: Results from a nationally representative longitudinal study of Canadian children aged 2–10. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 314–326. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​ab.​20202.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vollet, J. W., George, M. J., Kurup, A. R., & Underwood, M. K. (2019). Leading by example: Parental promotion of prosocial behavior predicts decreases in social aggression. Poster session presented at the Meetings of the Society for Research on Child Development (SRCD), Baltimore, MD. Vollet, J. W., George, M. J., Kurup, A. R., & Underwood, M. K. (2019). Leading by example: Parental promotion of prosocial behavior predicts decreases in social aggression. Poster session presented at the Meetings of the Society for Research on Child Development (SRCD), Baltimore, MD.
go back to reference Wickrama, K.A.S, Lee, T.K., O’Neal, C.W., & Lorenz, F.O. (2016). Higher-order growth curves and mixture modeling. Routledge. Wickrama, K.A.S, Lee, T.K., O’Neal, C.W., & Lorenz, F.O. (2016). Higher-order growth curves and mixture modeling. Routledge.
Metagegevens
Titel
Listening In: How Parent-Child Communication Relates to Social and Physical Aggression
Auteurs
Diana J. Meter
Samuel E. Ehrenreich
Kurt Beron
Marion K. Underwood
Publicatiedatum
27-04-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Child and Family Studies / Uitgave 6/2021
Print ISSN: 1062-1024
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2843
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-01959-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2021

Journal of Child and Family Studies 6/2021 Naar de uitgave