Skip to main content
Log in

Reliability of composite-task measurements of holistic face processing

  • Published:
Behavior Research Methods Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is growing interest in the study of individual differences in face recognition, including one of its hallmarks, holistic processing, which can be defined as a failure of selective attention to parts. These efforts demand that researchers be aware of, and try to maximize, the reliability of their measurements. Here we report on the reliability of measurements using the composite task (complete design), a measure of holistic processing that has been shown to have relatively good validity. Several studies have used the composite task to investigate individual differences, yet only one study has discussed its reliability. We investigate the reliability of composite-task measurements in eight data sets from five different samples of subjects. In general, we found reliability to be fairly low, but there was substantial variability across experiments. Researchers should keep in mind that reliability is a property of measurements, not of a task, and think about the ways in which measurements in a particular task may be improved before embarking on individual differences research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A version of the composite task featuring half of the trials has been referred to as the “partial design” in the literature (Gauthier & Bukach, 2007); however, the partial design confounds response and congruency (all same trials are incongruent and all different trials are congruent), which has been found to be problematic, because congruency produces a response bias that cannot be separated from sensitivity without the other half of the trials (see Cheung, Richler, Palmeri, & Gauthier, 2008; Richler, Cheung, & Gauthier, 2011a; Richler & Gauthier, in press).

  2. Note that this reliability was computed for 144 trials, and we report reliabilities for 160 trials in the present work.

  3. Note that DeGutis et al.’s (2013) reproduction of this formula included a minor error in which the primary and control conditions were switched.

References

  • Busigny, T., Joubert, S., Felician, O., Ceccaldi, M., & Rossion, B. (2010). Holistic perception of the individual face is specific and necessary: Evidence from an extensive case study of acquired prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4057–4092. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.017

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callender, J. C., & Osburn, H. G. (1979). An empirical comparison of coefficient alpha, Guttman’s Lambda-2, and MSPLIT maximized split-half reliability estimates. Journal of Educational Measurement, 16, 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, O. S., Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2008). Revisiting the role of spatial frequencies in the holistic processing of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1327–1336. doi:10.1037/a0011752

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiou, J., & Spreng, R. A. (1996). The reliability of difference scores: A re-examination. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior, 9, 158–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chua, K.-W., Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (2014). Becoming a Lunari or Taiyo expert: Learner attention to parts drives holistic processing of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1174–1182. doi:10.1037/a0035895

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGutis, J., Wilmer, J., Mercado, R. J., & Cohan, S. (2013). Using regression to measure holistic face processing reveals a strong link with face recognition ability. Cognition, 126, 87–100. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2006). The Cambridge Face Memory Test: Results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic faces. Neuropsychologia, 44, 576–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eide, P., Kemp, A., Silberstein, R. B., Nathan, P. J., & Stough, C. (2002). Test–retest reliability of the emotional Stroop task: Examining the paradox of measurement change. Journal of Psychology, 136, 514–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, I., & Bukach, C. (2007). Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? Cognition, 103, 322–330. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, I., Klaiman, C., & Schultz, R. T. (2009). Face composite effects reveal abnormal face processing in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 470–478.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J., & Tanaka, J. (1998). Training “Greeble” experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research, 38, 2401–2428. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00442-2

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goffaux, V., & Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are “spatial”—Holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1023–1039. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.1023

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konar, Y., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (2010). Holistic processing is not correlated with face-identification accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 38–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residulized change as measures of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28, 701–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malgady, R. G., & Colon-Malgady, G. (1991). Comparing the reliability of difference scores and residuals in analysis of covariance. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 803–807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGugin, R. W., Richler, J. J., Herzmann, G., Speegle, M., & Gauthier, I. (2012). The Vanderbilt Expertise Test reveals domain-general and domain-specific sex effects in object recognition. Vision Research, 69, 10–22. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2012.07.014

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B. C. (2007). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 8–15. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw–Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palermo, R., Willis, M. L., Rivolta, D., McKone, E., Wilson, E. C., & Calder, A. J. (2011). Impaired holistic coding of facial expression and facial identity in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1226–1235.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peter, J., Gilbert, A., Churchill, J., & Brown, T. J. (1993). Caution in the use of difference scores in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 662–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Bukach, C. M., & Gauthier, I. (2009). Context influences holistic processing of nonface objects in the composite task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 530–540. doi:10.3758/APP.71.3.530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Cheung, O. S., & Gauthier, I. (2011a). Beliefs alter holistic face processing . . . if response bias is not taken into account. Journal of Vision, 11(13), 17:1–13. doi:10.1167/11.13.17

  • Richler, J. J., Cheung, O. S., & Gauthier, I. (2011b). Holistic processing predicts face recognition. Psychological Science, 22, 464–471. doi:10.1177/0956797611401753

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (in press). A meta-analysis and review of holistic processing. Psychological Bulletin.

  • Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2012). Meanings, mechanisms, and measures of holistic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(553), 1–6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00553

    Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Tanaka, J. W., Brown, D. D., & Gauthier, I. (2008). Why does selective attention to parts fail in face processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1356–1368.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Wong, Y. K., & Gauthier, I. (2011d). Perceptual expertise as a shift from strategic interference to automatic holistic processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 129–134. doi:10.1177/0963721411402472

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D., Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement of change. Quantitative Methods in Psychology, 92, 726–748.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossion, B. (2013). The composite face illusion: A whole window into our understanding of holistic face perception. Visual Cognition, 21, 139–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1996). Measurement error in psychological research: Lessons from 26 research scenarios. Psychological Methods, 1, 199–223. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, G. P., Allen, D. N., Jorgensen, M. L., & Cramer, S. L. (2005). Test–retest reliability of standard and emotional Stroop tasks an investigation of color–word and picture–word versions. Assessment, 12, 330–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, B. (1994). Guidelines for authors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54, 837–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troje, N. F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (1996). Face recognition under varying poses: The role of texture and shape. Vision Research, 36, 1761–1771. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(95)00230-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A. C.-N., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2009). Conditions for facelike expertise with objects: Becoming a Ziggerin expert—but which type? Psychological Science, 20, 1108–1117. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02430.x

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16, 747–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D. W., & Williams, R. H. (1998). Reliability scores under realistic assumptions about properties of pre-test and post-test scores. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 51, 343–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author note

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (Grant No. SBE-0542013), Vanderbilt Vision Research Center (Grant No. P30-EY008126), and National Eye Institute (Grant No. R01 EY013441-06A2). We thank Riaun Floyd and Magen Speegle for their help with data collection and stimulus construction, and Jeremy Wilmer and Joseph DeGutis for helpful discussions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. Ross.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ross, D.A., Richler, J.J. & Gauthier, I. Reliability of composite-task measurements of holistic face processing. Behav Res 47, 736–743 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0497-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0497-4

Keywords

Navigation