When two targets (T1 and T2) are inserted in a stream of distractors presented in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP), identification accuracy is nearly perfect for T1 but is dramatically reduced for T2 (Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992). This phenomenon, called the attentional blink (AB), is most evident when T2 is presented with a temporal lag of 100–500 ms after T1.

The underlying mechanisms of this second-target deficit are currently a matter of debate (for reviews see Dux & Marois, 2009, and Martens & Wyble, 2010). However, there is general agreement that in order to obtain an AB it is necessary that T2 be backward-masked. This is why in the AB literature T2 is typically followed by at least one other item that acts as a backward maskFootnote 1. This practice is largely based on Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's (1998) finding that the AB is not in evidence unless T2 is followed by a trailing item. Here we show that backward masking of T2 is not essential for the AB, and that Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's findings were vitiated by constraints arising from the experimental design.

Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998) studied the role of T2 masking in two main experiments. In experiment 1, observers were required to identify two letter targets inserted in an RSVP stream of digit distractors. A significant AB was obtained when T2 was followed by a digit that functioned as a backward mask, but not when T2 was the last item in the RSVP stream. The authors noted, however, that since performance in the no-mask condition was near perfect, the absence of an AB might have been due to a ceiling constraint imposed by the 100% limit of the response scale.

To rule out this possibility, Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998) implemented a condition in experiment 2 in which accuracy of T2 identification was brought below ceiling by embedding the T2 letter in an aggregate of random dots that lowered the level of performance to approximately 80% correct responses. Yet, no AB was in evidence. On the strength of these findings, Giesbrecht and Di Lollo concluded that backward masking of T2 was essential for the AB, and that the absence of an AB in experiment 1 was not due to ceiling constraints.

However, there is a possibility that the impoverishment of T2 in Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's (1998) experiment 2 might have created a new ceiling arising not from the upper limit of the response scale but from what Norman and Bobrow (1975) referred to as data limitation. Namely, the level of impoverishment might have been such that the T2 letter could not be identified on more than about 80% of the trials even when presented at the longest lag of 700 ms, which is beyond the period of the AB. Were it not for such a data-limitation ceiling, an AB might have been in evidence with no backward masking of T2.

To summarize, it is possible that the absence of an AB in Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's (1998) study might have been due to two different forms of constraint: the upper limit of the response scale in experiment 1, and data limitation in experiment 2. The present work examined both forms of constraint. First, we examined the possibility that in Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's experiment 1, when T2 was neither impoverished nor followed by a mask, performance was constrained by the 100% limit because the task was too easy. The present experiment 1 was designed to lower the identification accuracy for T2 below 100% by using pseudoletters instead of digit distractors. The ensuing increased target-distractor similarity was expected to lead to a corresponding impairment in T2 identification. The intent was to bring T2 performance below the 100% ceiling (Chun & Potter, 1995; Visser, Bischof, & Di Lollo, 2004) and thus allow any latent AB to be revealed.

Next, we examined the possibility that in Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's (1998) study when T2 was impoverished by random-dot noise, the AB was not in evidence because of data limitation. This was done in experiment 2 by using a dynamic threshold-tracking method that is not constrained either by response-scale limits or by data limitations (PEST; Parameter Estimation by Sequential Testing, Taylor & Creelman, 1967). PEST is not constrained by either ceiling or floor considerations because if either constraint is present, PEST cannot converge on the criterial level. To the extent that failure to find an AB was due to ceiling constraints, an AB should be in evidence when those constraints are removed.

Experiment 1

The objective of experiment 1 was to lower T2 performance below 100% by increasing the difficulty of the task relative to that in the study of Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998). As was the case in the latter, no mask was presented after T2. Task difficulty was increased by increasing the similarity of the distractors to the letter target. This was done by replacing the digit distractors with pseudoletters. If an AB were found with pseudoletter distractors, it would mean that the AB can indeed occur with no backward masking of T2. It would also mean that performance in the easier task used by Giesbrecht and Di Lollo was probably constrained by the 100% limit of the response scale.

Methods

Observers

Twenty-seven undergraduate students participated for course credit. All reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive to the purpose of the experiment.

Apparatus and stimuli

The experiment was run in a dimly lit room. Observers sat at a distance of approximately 60 cm from a NEC AccuSync 120 computer monitor refreshed at a rate of 140 Hz. They viewed an RSVP stream that included black pseudoletter distractors and two target letters (all 48-point Geneva font, height 1.0º) in the centre of the screen against a white background. Each item remained on the screen for 50 ms, and was followed by a 50-ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI) during which the screen remained blank, yielding a presentation rate of ten items/s. The RSVP stream contained a variable number of pseudoletter distractors and two letter targets (T1 and T2) selected randomly from the English alphabet, except I, O, Q, and Z. The number of distractors preceding T1 was determined randomly on each trial and varied between five and ten, inclusive. On any given trial, the distractors were drawn from the set of pseudoletters illustrated in Fig. 1, with the constraint that the selected pseudoletter was not one of the two preceding items.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pseudoletters used in experiments 1 and 2

T2 was presented at one of three lags after T1: 100, 300, or 900 ms, i.e., lags 1, 3, and 9, respectively. The lag was selected randomly on each trial, with the constraint that there was an equal number of trials per lag. Pseudoletter distractors continued to be presented during the inter-target lag. The RSVP stream ended with a 7-ms presentation of T2. The sequence of events on any given trial is illustrated in the left-hand panel of Fig. 2. In the present context, the AB was defined as the difference between lags 3 and 9. Lag 1 was included for consistency with previous AB studies.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Left-hand panel: display sequence in experiment 1. Illustrated is an inter-target lag of 300 ms (lag 3). Right-hand panel: Mean percentage of correct T2 responses at each lag given that T1 had been reported correctly (T2|T1). T1  =  first target; T2  =  second target

Design and procedure

At the beginning of each trial, a small fixation cross was presented in the center of the screen. Observers initiated each trial by pressing the space bar. They were instructed to ignore the distractors and to report the identity of T1 and T2, one at a time at their leisure, by pressing the corresponding keys on the keyboard in response to prompts on the screen that were displayed approximately 10° above the RSVP stream. This substantial spatial separation obviated any backward masking of T2. Observers were also instructed that they could report the targets in either order, and that they could guess if unsure. The experiment began with 10 practice trials, which were followed by 300 trials, 100 for each of the three lags.

Results and discussion

In this and the next experiment, only those trials in which T1 was identified correctly were included for analysis. This procedure is commonly adopted in AB experiments on the grounds that, on trials in which T1 is identified incorrectly, the source of the error is unknown, and thus its effect on T2 processing cannot be estimated. Collapsed across lags, the percentage of correct T1 responses was 80.1. The T2 results, illustrated in Fig. 2, were analyzed in a one-way within-subject analysis of variance (ANOVA) with lag as the factor at three levels (lag 1, lag 3, and lag 9). The analysis revealed a marginally significant effect of Lag, F(2, 52)  =  2.99, p  =  0.059, msE  =  25.26. The quadratic component was significant, F(1, 26)  =  5.23, p  =  0.031, msE  =  23.81. A planned paired t test between lags 3 and 9 revealed a significant AB, t(26)  =  2.75, p  =  0.011.

As illustrated in Fig. 2, the overall level of T2 performance was close to ceiling. However, increasing the target-distractor similarity by using pseudoletters was successful in revealing a significant AB, even when T2 was not followed by a mask. This result demonstrates that the absence of a significant AB in the corresponding experiment by Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998) might have been due to a ceiling constraint imposed by the response scale. The essential point is that an AB can be obtained without backward masking of T2 and without a task switch between the two targets.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 was designed to examine the possibility, outlined in the introduction, that the absence of an AB in Giesbrecht and Di Lollo's (1998) experiment 2 in which T2 was degraded with random-dot noise might have stemmed from a data-limitation ceiling. To avoid such a ceiling, we used a method which, as noted above, is free from ceiling constraints. Instead of using accuracy as a response measure, we determined the number of noise dots that were required to identify T2 on about 80% of the trials. We reasoned that, to the extent that the processing of T2 is impaired during the period of the AB, a lower level of degradation (fewer noise dots) would be needed at lag 3 than at lag 9 to maintain the criterial level of performance.

The number of random dots presented simultaneously with T2 was varied dynamically by PEST at each lag, separately for each observer. PEST was set to converge to a level of 80% correct T2 identifications, given that T1 had been identified correctly (T2|T1). On the hypothesis that backward masking is necessary for the AB, the number of noise dots required to reach the 80% criterion (DOTC) should be invariant with lag when T2 is not followed by a trailing mask. In contrast, a smaller DOTC at lag 3 than at lag 9 would indicate that T2 identification at lag 3 is relatively impaired, consistent with the conclusion that an AB can occur with no backward masking of T2.

Methods

Observers

Eighteen undergraduate students participated for course credit. They were drawn from the same population as experiment 1. None had participated in the previous experiment.

Apparatus and stimuli

Apparatus and stimuli were the same as in experiment 1 except for the following. An aggregate of noise dots was overlaid on T2. Each dot consisted of a small black patch (2 × 2 pixels, 0.1° side). The location of each dot was chosen randomly within an imaginary square (height 0.9°) containing the T2 letter. The number of dots was varied by PEST, as described below.

Design and procedure

Design and procedures were the same as in experiment 1, with the following exceptions. The exposure duration of T2 was 50 ms. The dependent measure was the value of DOTC. To arrive at DOTC, PEST increased the number of noise dots when the task was too easy (T2|T1 over the criterial level), and reduced it when the task was too hard. A Wald (1947) sequential likelihood-ratio test determined whether the immediately preceding run of responses was above or below criterial level. The Wald routine was called only on trials in which T1 had been identified correctly. The assumption was made that the criterial level of performance had been approximated after four reversals in the direction of adjustment of the number of noise dots. The final score (DOTC) was the mean number of dots over the 16 trials following the fourth reversal. Thus, DOTC represented the level of T2 degradation required to achieve the criterial level. The observers were instructed to report the two targets in any order, guessing if unsure. For each observer, three concurrent and randomly intermixed PEST sequences were run, one for each lag.

Results and discussion

Collapsed across lags, the percentage of correct T1 responses was 90.3. The percentages of correct T2|T1 responses were 78.9, 78.5, and 81.3 for lags 1, 3, and 9, respectively. The values of DOTC, illustrated in Fig. 3, were analyzed in a one-way within-subject ANOVA with lag as the factor at three levels (lags 1, 3, and 9). The analysis revealed a marginally significant effect of lag, F(2, 34)  =  3.22, p  =  0.052, msE  =  26.77. The quadratic component was significant, F(1, 17)  =  5.88, p  =  .027, msE  =  26.60. A planned paired t test between lags 3 and 9 revealed a significant AB, t(17)  =  2.48, p  =  0.024.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Left-hand panel: display sequence in experiment 2. Illustrated is an inter-target lag of 300 ms (lag 3). The number of noise dots overlaid on T2 was varied dynamically to converge on 80% correct T2 identifications (DOTC). Right-hand panel: Mean DOTC scores averaged across observers at each lag. T1  =  first target; T2  =  second target

The results of experiment 2 are unambiguous: fewer noise dots could be tolerated at lag 3 than at lag 9 to achieve the criterial level of about 80% correct T2|T1 identifications. Clearly, an AB can be obtained in the absence of any backward masking of T2. Furthermore, the present results indicate that a data-limitation ceiling was probably responsible for the absence of an AB in the study of Giesbrecht & Di Lollo, (1998, experiment 2), in which T2 was degraded by a fixed number of noise dots.

General discussion

It is generally agreed that, unless there is a task switch between the two targets, T2 must be backward-masked in order to obtain an AB (Giesbrecht & Di Lollo, 1998; Kawahara, Di Lollo, & Enns, 2001; Kawahara, Zuvic, Enns, & Di Lollo, 2003; Vogel & Luck, 2002). Here we show that an AB can occur without backward masking of T2 even in the absence of task switching.

In experiment 1, we tested the hypothesis that Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998, experiment 1) failed to find an AB when T2 was not backward-masked because performance was constrained by a response-scale ceiling. We used pseudoletter distractors instead of digits to increase the difficulty of the task thus bringing performance below ceiling. We found a small but significant AB even though T2 was not backward-masked.

Experiment 2 examined the possibility that a different type of ceiling, arising from data limitations, might have prevented an AB in the study of Giesbrecht and Di Lollo (1998, experiment 2). Such a ceiling was averted by using a dynamic threshold tracking procedure in which T2 was not backward-masked but was overlaid by a variable number of noise dots. The procedure determined the number of noise dots that could be tolerated to converge on about 80% correct T2 identifications at each lag. The results revealed a significant AB: fewer noise dots were required at lag 3 than at either lags 1 or 9, strongly suggesting that the absence of an AB in Giesbrecht & Di Lollo's study was due to a data-limitation ceiling. The results of both experiments confirmed that an AB can be obtained without backward masking of T2 and without task switching.

T2 deterioration during the AB

It is known that the processing of T2 is delayed during the period of the AB (Jolicœur & Dell'Acqua, 1998; Visser, 2007; Vogel & Luck, 2002). During this delay, the T2 representation is held to be vulnerable to decay and to backward masking, thus giving rise to the AB (Chun & Potter, 1995; Kawahara et al., 2001, 2003). In the present work, backward masking was obviously not a factor, leaving decay as the most likely cause of the AB.

The idea of a decaying T2 representation is encompassed naturally within two-stage (bottleneck) models in which some critical stage of consolidation of the T2 representation is said to be postponed (Chun & Potter, 1995; Jolicœur & Dell'Acqua, 1998). While in a non-consolidated state, the T2 representation decays over time and is vulnerable to masking by trailing items. Thus, a trailing mask is not essential for the AB, although it probably increases its magnitude. In contrast, input-control models ascribe the AB to interference arising not from the requirement to process T1 but to the presence of intervening distractors that disrupt or suppress an input filter (Di Lollo, Kawahara, Ghorashi, & Enns, 2005; Olivers & Meeter, 2008). As presently stated, input-control theories stipulate that masking of T2 is essential for the AB, and do not specify any mechanisms that could account for the presence of an AB when T2 is not masked. Thus, input-control models would need to be modified in order to encompass the present results. A detailed comparison between limited-capacity and input-control theories of the AB is beyond the scope of the present paper. A recent debate on this issue can be found in papers by Dux, Asplund, and Marois (2008), who advocated a resource-depletion account (but see response by Olivers, Spalek, Kawahara, & Di Lollo, 2009), and by Dell'Acqua, Jolicœur, Luria, and Pluchino (2009) who also advocated a capacity-limitation account (but see response by Olivers, Hulleman, Spalek, Kawahara, & Di Lollo, 2010).

It is perhaps worth noting that, in earlier studies, an AB attributable to decay has been found only when the T2 stimulus was severely degraded (Kawahara et al., 2001). This is because, when so degraded, the T2 representation decays to illegibility before the processing delay is over, thus giving rise to an AB. In contrast, when the T2 stimulus is not degraded, it is more likely to remain legible until after the processing delay is over, thus averting an AB.

Decay of the T2 representation during the period of delay is clearly not the only determinant of the AB. Rather, in conventional studies, decay and backward masking are likely to work in conjunction with one another. A direct comparison between the DOTC measure used in experiment 2 and the accuracy measure used in conventional AB studies is obviously not feasible. At a first approximation, the magnitude of the AB can be estimated as 1 minus the ratio of the dependent measures at lags 3 and 9. In Giesbrecht & Di Lollo's study (1998, Fig. 2a), in which the T2 representation was weakened by both masking and decay, that value was .33. The corresponding value in the present experiment 2, in which the T2 representation was weakened only by decay, the value was .18. Regardless of specifics, the main contribution of the present work lies in the decoupling of the decay and the masking components of the AB.