Introduction

When people respond to an object or its features, the location occupied by the object can influence the response, which is usually addressed in the Simon task, named for J. R. Simon (1990). In the typical visual version of this task with horizontal stimulus-response (S-R) arrangement, left and right stimulus locations are task-irrelevant and non-spatial attributes (e.g., colors or shapes) convey the task-relevant information, to which left and right keypresses are mapped arbitrarily. Response performance is better when the non-spatial attributes and response positions correspond than when they do not, a finding that is called the spatial or location-based Simon effect (see reviews of Lu & Proctor, 1995; Proctor, Miles, & Baroni, 2011; Simon, 1990; Umiltà & Nicoletti, 1990). The Simon effect also occurs with stimuli whose task-irrelevant spatial information is conveyed not by their physical locations but by the meanings of centrally-presented location words or arrows, resulting in word- and arrow-based Simon effects (Khalid & Ansorge, 2013; Lu & Proctor, 2001; Pellicano, Lugli, Baroni, & Nicoletti, 2009; Proctor, Marble, & Vu, 2000).

The location-based Simon effect has been attributed to responses primed by automatic processing of task-irrelevant stimulus location (direct route), which interferes with responses activated by processing of a task-relevant attribute in accordance with the instructed S-R mapping (indirect route; De Jong, Liang, & Lauber, 1994; Hommel, 1997; Kornblum, Hasbroucq, & Osman, 1990; Ulrich, Schröter, Leuthold, & Birngruber, 2015; Zhang, Zhang, & Kornblum, 1999; Zorzi & Umiltà, 1995), which is named the dual-route model. Similarly, given that the location words and arrow directions can be processed automatically (Hommel, Pratt, Colzato, & Godijn, 2001; Lu & Proctor, 1995), word- and arrow-based Simon effects are ascribed to the automatically processed location word and arrow direction interfering with the incongruent response indicated by their ink colors (Lu & Proctor, 2001; Luo & Proctor, 2016; Proctor et al., 2000; Proctor, Yamaguchi, Zhang, & Vu, 2009).

Response-time distribution and chronometric analyses

Initial studies of the Simon effect and related correspondence effects focused on differences in mean response time (RT) that provide estimates of the central tendency for each condition (see Lu & Proctor, 1995), which are still the focal point of many studies. One problem for analyses limited to mean RT is that it is difficult to specify thoroughly how the experimental manipulation influences RT. On the one hand, the increase of mean RT resulting from non-correspondence may be due to a shift of the RT distribution (see location-based Simon in Fig. 1), an increase in the skew of the RT distribution in one condition compared to the other condition (Tse, Balota, Yap, Duchek, & McCabe, 2010), or both (see word- and arrow-based Simon effects in Fig. 1). On the other hand, RTs may be affected by manipulations that have no obvious effect on mean RT but on the skew of the RT distribution (Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 2000). Therefore, beginning with De Jong et al. (1994), reporting analyses of the Simon effect across the RT distributions (delta plots) has become common (e.g., Davranche & McMorris, 2009; Kubo-Kawai & Kawai, 2010). In studies using a Vincentizing procedure (Ratcliff, 1979), a group RT distribution is obtained by partitioning each participant’s RTs on the corresponding and noncorresponding trials into percentile bins (e.g., 10), ranging from shortest to longest, and the Simon effect (the difference) is calculated for each bin. Most findings are that the location-based Simon effect is largest for fast responses but decreases, and even may reverse, as responses slow when the imperative stimulus is displayed in a left or right location (Ansorge & Wühr, 2004; Ellinghaus, Karlbauer, Bausenhart, & Ulrich, 2017; Pratte, Rouder, Morey, & Feng, 2010; Servant, Montagnini, & Burle, 2014; Ulrich et al., 2015). In contrast, the word- and arrow-based Simon effects increase as RTs increase (Miles & Proctor, 2012; Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean response times and standard errors for each of the bins for the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, the word-based Simon effect in Experiment 2, and the arrow-based Simon effect in Experiment 3

Likewise, in a range of other tasks and conditions, there is growing interest in fitting a mathematical function to empirical RT distributions to quantify their characteristics in order to allow measurement of more than central tendency (Heathcote, Popiel, & Mewhort, 1991; Hockley, 1984; Ratcliff, 1978, 1979; Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 1996; Spieler et al., 2000; White, Risko, & Besner, 2016; Yap, Balota, Tse, & Besner, 2008). The convolution of a Gaussian and an exponential distribution, called the ex-Gaussian distribution, has a good fit to empirical RT distributions in a range of tasks (Heathcote et al., 1991; Hockley, 1984; Hohle, 1965; Spieler et al., 1996), and the stability of the parameter estimates has been most fully demonstrated in Ratcliff s (1978, 1979) pioneering work in testing an explicit model of memory retrieval, and in other studies with the Stroop task paradigm (Heathcote et al., 1991; Spieler et al., 1996; Spieler et al., 2000; Steinhauser & Hübner, 2009).

Fitting the ex-Gaussian distribution to an empirical RT distribution results in estimates of three parameters (μ, σ, and τ). The parameter Μ reflects the mean of the Gaussian component of the distribution. A change in μ reflects a shift of the RT distribution between different conditions, and it indicates that a variable has an early acting influence on the majority of RTs. σ reflects the standard deviation associated with the Gaussian component, and a change in σ reflects the differences of standard deviation of the RT distributions between different conditions. τ reflects the mean and standard deviation of the exponential component. A change in τ reflects the change in the tail of the RT distribution between different conditions, and it indicates that a variable has a late-acting influence on some trials. Importantly, the mean of the ex-Gaussian distribution equals μ + τ, and its variance equals μ2 + τ2.

The ex-Gaussian analysis offers another way of characterizing the influence of factors on RT distributions, and these ex-Gaussian parameters provide more information about changes in the RT distribution than are reflected in analyses of mean RTs and delta plot functions (Andrews & Heathcote, 2001; Balota & Spieler, 1999; Heathcote, Popiel, & Mewhort, 1991; Spieler et al., 1996; White et al., 2016). For example, ex-Gaussian distribution analysis can decompose effects of response- and task-related conflicts in an across-task associative priming paradigm that are not distinguishable in analyses of mean RTs (Moutsopoulou & Waszak, 2012). This decomposition is possible because conflict related to stimulus–response associations retrieved after a switch of tasks is reflected in the Gaussian component (μ and σ), whereas conflict related to the retrieval of stimulus–task associations is reflected in the exponential component (τ). Moreover, ex-Gaussian distribution analysis allows one to see whether some experimental effect on mean RT arises from an early shift of the RT distribution (a change in μ), an increase of late responses in the tail of the RT distribution (a change in τ), or both (Tse et al., 2010). For example, an ex-Gaussian analysis revealed that a semantically-based Stroop effect (effect of incongruent color-associated word on responding to color) was restricted to the arithmetic mean and μ but not τ (White et al., 2016), suggesting that the semantically-based Stroop effect on mean RT arises from the early faster responses. By contrast, the typical color-word Stroop effect is obtained on all three ex-Gaussian parameters (Spieler et al., 1996, 2000), providing evidence that the effect occurs early and lasts for a long time. Moreover, we can obtain σ for each condition and, by calculating their differences, can determine whether the delta plot is positive or negative (Zhang & Kornblum, 1997).

Mode-specific or shared mechanism

There is agreement that spatial locations activate visual-spatial codes, and location words are symbolic and activate semantic-spatial codes (i.e., codes of spatial meaning; Hommel et al., 2001; Notebaert, De Moor, Gevers, & Hartsuiker, 2007). Arrows are usually regarded as symbolic (e.g., Freitas, Bahar, Yang, & Banai, 2007; Tipples, 2002), although they also have visual-spatial properties (e.g., for a left-pointing arrow, the arrowhead is located to the left of the shaft). Therefore, arrows may activate visual- and semantic-spatial codes. Although the spatial codes activated by various spatial modes are different, they may indicate the same concepts (e.g., left or right) and have shared representations, as evidenced in some studies (e.g., De Houwer, Beckers, Vandorpe, & Custers, 2005; Notebaert et al., 2007; Proctor et al., 2009). Both location words and arrows yield S-R mapping effects when the location information conveyed by them is relevant (Wang & Proctor, 1996) and Simon effects when it is irrelevant (Proctor & Vu, 2002). Moreover, the location-based Simon effect is eliminated when location-based Simon task trials are intermixed with ones in which participants respond to the words left and right with incompatibly mapped keypresses (Notebaert et al., 2007; Proctor et al., 2000; Vu, Ngo, Minakata, & Proctor, 2010).

The different spatial codes, generated by various location modes sharing spatial representations, triggered an issue of whether the spatial information conveyed by these various stimulus modes influences responses in a similar way or with identical processes. Some findings suggest that different location modes affect responses with mode-specific mechanisms, as the Simon effects based on them seem to have different temporal dynamics. For example, the location-based Simon effect decreases as RTs increase across the RT distribution for a left- or right-displayed imperative stimulus, whereas the word- and arrow-based Simon effects increase as RTs increase (Miles & Proctor, 2012; Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009). Wascher and colleagues (Wascher, Schatz, Kuder, & Verleger, 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005, 2007) suggested that increasing and decreasing delta plots of Simon effects obtained in various tasks are the result of two distinct mechanisms. One is responsible for a “visuomotor Simon effect” underlying the location-based Simon task, and the other for a “cognitive Simon effect” occurring in other forms of the Simon task, which is the product of high-level cognitive interference. The visuomotor Simon effect arises from the stimulus location automatically activating the corresponding response via the visuomotor pathway (Wascher et al., 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005, 2007), and the activation quickly fades out over time, resulting in a decreasing delta plot. The cognitive Simon effect increases over time as the task-irrelevant activation is slower and does not decay, leading to a positive delta plot.

In contrast to the explanation of two different mechanisms, some studies have proposed that even though the slopes of the delta plots are different in variants of the task, their underlying mechanism is the same. For example, Töbel, Hübner, and Stürmer (2014) found that the correspondence of the previous trial had a similar effect on the delta plots of horizontal and vertical visual Simon tasks. In both tasks, the slopes of the slowest quantiles of the delta plot decreased more following non-corresponding trials and the accuracy in the fastest quantiles was higher after incompatible trials than it was after compatible trials.

Studies of cognitive computational models also suggest that the location-based Simon effect (with a decreasing delta plot) and flanker effect, and Stroop effect for which the delta plot is increasing) can be fit by the same models (Servant, Montagnini, & Burle, 2014; Ulrich et al., 2015). According to the diffusion model for conflict tasks (Ulrich et al., 2015), the shape of delta functions largely varies with the relative speeds of the automatic activation and the controlled processes. Automatic activation of stimulus location developing quickly makes the negative slope of the delta functions of location-based Simon effect, whereas color word and arrow activation developing slowly make the positive slope of the delta functions of the flanker and color-word Stroop effects. These studies argue for a single underlying mechanism as the most parsimonious explanation to the time course of the Simon effect.

Present study

In the current study, we examined whether these various Simon effects occur on each of the ex-Gaussian parameters, and whether there are differences between them. To our knowledge, no studies have used the ex-Gaussian analysis to explore the Simon effect, particularly for the arrow- and word-based Simon effects. Given that ex-Gaussian distribution analysis can isolate the differential effects of task and response conflict in priming paradigms (Moutsopoulou & Waszak, 2012) and of color-word and semantic Stroop effects (Spieler et al., 1996, 2000; White et al., 2016), we expected that it could distinguish different Simon effects.

Specifically, in Experiment 1, the location-based Simon task with horizontal S-R arrangement was used. Given that the location-based Simon effect is largest for fast responses but decreases for slower responses across the RT distribution (Ansorge & Wühr, 2004; Pratte et al., 2010), the Simon effect on mean RT may mainly arise from a shift of the RT distribution (μ in the corresponding condition is smaller than that in the noncorresponding condition) and little from the tail of the RT distribution. Moreover, previous studies have demonstrated that effects of response conflict were mainly observed on μ and σ but not τ (Spieler et al., 2000; Steinhauser & Hübner, 2009; White et al., 2016). Thus, the location-based Simon effect would be expected to occur on μ but not τ, given that the effect is mainly associated with response selection (Lu & Proctor, 1995). These expected results would help to test the diffusion model for conflict tasks (Ulrich et al., 2015), which assumes that automatic activation of stimulus location develops quickly, producing a location-based Simon effect in fast responses but not slow responses.

In Experiments 2 and 3 we examined separately the word- and arrow-based Simon effects on ex-Gaussian parameters. Given that the word- and arrow-based Simon effects increase as RTs increase (Miles & Proctor, 2012; Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009), they may arise from a shift of the RT distribution and from the tail of the RT distribution in the incompatible condition, compared to that in the compatible condition. Thus, we predicted that these Simon effects would occur on μ and τ. These expected results would also help support the diffusion model for conflict tasks (Ulrich et al., 2015), which assumes that automatic activation of words and arrows develops across time, resulting in their associated Simon effects occurring in both the fast and slow responses.

Experiment 1: Location-based Simon effect

Method

Participants

Twenty right-handed participants (eight males), aged 18–24 years, took part in Experiment 1. All participants in this experiment and the others were native speakers of Chinese (Putonghua), had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and were naïve as to the purpose of the experiment.

Apparatus, stimuli, procedure, design

Stimuli were presented on a super VGA high-resolution color monitor with grey background. A personal computer, running E-Prime 1.1 software, controlled the presentation of stimuli, timing operations, and data collection. Participants placed their heads on a chinrest and viewed the monitor from a distance of 58 cm in a dimly lit room. The stimuli were red and a green squares (0.85° × 0.85°), whose centers were 4.0 ° to the left or right of the center of screen.

Each participant performed eight practice trials followed by 128 test trials (64 compatible and 64 incompatible, randomly intermixed). Each trial began with onset of a white central fixation cross (0.4° × 0.4°). After 1 s, a square in red or green appeared for 150 ms. After that, the gray screen became blank, during which the trial terminated if the participant responded or after 1.5 s if no response had been made. After the response, a 1-s inter-trial interval occurred, during which the screen remained blank. Responses were made by pressing a left key (V) for the red ink color or right key (M) for the green ink color on the computer keyboard with the left or right index finger. The mapping of colors to left or right responses was counterbalanced across participants. The response keys and computer screen were aligned such that the fixation point and the midway point between the two response keys were on the participant’s sagittal midline. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation and to respond to the targets as quickly and accurately as possible.

Data analysis

The practice trials were excluded from the RT and percent error (PE) analyses in Experiments 13. In Experiment 1, the following test trials were excluded from the RT (including Vincentile analysis and ex-Gaussian) analyses: those with (a) incorrect response to the target (3.8 %), (b) RTs longer than 1,500 ms or shorter than 150 ms (0.0 %), and (c) outside of three standard deviations of the mean for each condition per participant (1.3 %). Mean correct RTs and PEs in Experiments 13 are then presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Mean percentage of error (PE), mean response time (RT, in ms), μ, σ, τ, and standard deviation (in parentheses) for the compatible and incompatible conditions in the location-based Simon (Experiment 1), word-based Simon (Experiment 2), and arrow-based Simon (Experiment 3) tasks

Secondly, using the Vincentizing procedure (Ratcliff, 1979), RTs were rank ordered from shortest to longest in each condition for each participant in Experiments 13, divided into five equally sized bins and averaged in each bin. The Simon effects for each spatial mode were then calculated for each bin in Experiments 13, as shown in Fig. 2. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on RT, with bin and location-based Simon (compatible, incompatible) as within-subject variables. Considering the way in which the RT data were grouped, the bin main effect was significant in all analyses, so it was not reported and discussed here or in the following experiments.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The probability density functions separately for compatible and incompatible conditions in the location-based Simon task in Experiment 1 (top panel) and in the word-based Simon task in Experiment 2 (middle panel), and in the arrow-based Simon task in Experiment 3 (bottom panel)

Lastly, the same correct RT data were fit to the ex-Gaussian function for each participant in each condition by using the timefit function in the retimes package (Massidda, 2013). The timefit function uses maximum likelihood estimation to generate the three parameters (μ, σ, τ). Separate repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed on these parameters, with word-based Simon and location-based Simon as within-subject variables.

Results

PE.

There was a Simon effect of 1.3 %, with fewer response errors in the compatible (3.1 %) than in the incompatible condition (4.4 %), but the difference was not statistically significant, t(19) = 1.07, p = .297.

RT.

Vincentile analysis. The main effect of location-based Simon was significant, F(1, 19) = 17.43, p = .001, MSE = 1,218, η p 2 = .479, reflecting a Simon effect of 21 ms. The interaction between bin and location-based Simon was significant, F(4, 76) = 6.00, p < .001, MSE = 223, η p 2 = .240. Further analysis showed that the Simon effect was not significant for bin 5, p = .672, but was for the other bins, ps < .017. Trend analysis showed that the Simon effect decreased linearly from bin 1 to bin 5 (see Fig. 2), F(1, 19) = 7.59, p = .013, MSE = 1,389, η p 2 = .286.

Ex-Gaussian analysis. The location-based Simon effect was significant on μ, t(19) = 4.53, p < .001, with a 23 ms Simon effect, and σ, t(19) = -2.41, p = .026, with a -12 ms Simon effect, but not on τ, t(19) = 1.04, p = .311.

Discussion

The location-based Simon effect was obtained on mean RT, and the effect decreased as RT increased in the Vincentile analyses, replicating findings obtained in previous studies (Ansorge & Wühr, 2004; Pratte et al., 2010; Proctor et al., 2009; Ellinghaus et al., 2017). The location-based Simon effect of 21 ms was similar to Ellinghaus et al.’s Experiment 1 (22 ms) and Experiment 2 (24 ms), in which the location-based Simon effect on mean RT and delta functions were invariant against changes of stimulus duration (150 ms or until response emitted). Likewise, as predicted, we obtained the location-based Simon effects on μ and σ, but not on τ, consistent with the findings that response conflict is mainly observed on μ and σ but not on τ (Spieler et al., 1996; Steinhauser & Hübner, 2009; White et al., 2016).

The location-based Simon effect occurred on μ but not on τ, which showed that the effect on mean RT is because the RT distribution in the incompatible condition, compared to that in the compatible condition, shifts from left to right, as shown in Fig. 1. These results suggest that the location-based Simon effect mainly arises from the faster responses and not from the slower responses. Likewise, the effect on σ was negative, showing that variance was larger in the compatible condition than in the incompatible condition, as shown in Fig. 1. The larger variance in the compatible condition results in a decreasing delta plot function, as shown in Fig. 2, compatible with the mathematical proof in Zhang and Kornblum (1997), which showed that the reduction in location-based Simon effect with an increase of RT occurs when the variance of the RT distribution for compatible trials is larger than that for incompatible trials.

Experiment 2: Word-based Simon effect

Method

Twenty new right-handed participants (eight males), aged 19–26 years, took part in Experiment 2. The apparatus, stimuli, procedure, design and data analysis were identical to Experiment 1, except that the red and green squares were replaced by Chinese single-character words 左 (left) and 右 (right) (0.85° × 0.85°) of high frequency (2,404 and 1,856 in Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary, 1985). They were written in red or green and presented on the center of the screen, and responses were made to their colors. Experiment 2 had a variable, word-based Simon (compatible, incompatible), with 64 observations per experimental condition.

Results

As in Experiment 1, test trials wherein participants responded incorrectly to the target (2.1 %) and RTs longer than 1,500 ms or less than 150 ms (0.4 %) were excluded from the RT analyses, and test trial RTs that were not within three standard deviations of the mean (2.2 %) for each condition per participant were excluded from the RT analyses.

PE.

The word-based Simon effect was significant, t(19) = 3.12, p = .006, with fewer errors in the compatible condition (1.5 %) than in the incompatible condition (3.5 %).

RT.

Vincentile analysis. The word-based Simon effect was significant, F(1, 19)= 9.78, p = .006, MSE = 2,025, η p 2 = .340, which reflects a Simon effect of 24 ms. Moreover, the interaction between bin and word-based Simon was significant, F(4, 76) = 5.80, p < .001, MSE = 488, η p 2 = .234. Further analysis showed that the location-based Simon effect was not significant for bin 1, t(19) = .64, p = .529, approached significance for bin 2, t(19) = 2.02, p = .057, and was significant for bins 3 to 5, ps < .011. Trend analysis revealed that the word-based Simon effect increased linearly from bin 1 to bin 5, F(1, 19) = 6. 36, p = .021, MSE = 3,368, η p 2 = .251 (see Fig. 2). In addition, a comparative analysis showed that the word-based Simon effect on mean RT was not different from the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, t(38) = .06, p = .952.

Ex-Gaussian analysis

The word-based Simon effect was significant on μ, t(19) = 2.26, p = .036, on σ, t(19) = 2.24, p = .037, and on τ, t(19) = 2.13, p = .047, indicating 12 ms, 11 ms and 13 ms Simon effects on these parameters, separately. An analysis comparing Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the word-based Simon effect was smaller than the location-based Simon effect on μ, t(38) = 2.10, p = .042, but larger than it on σ, t(38) = 3.24, p = .002, and on τ, t(38) = 2.35, p = .024. In addition, the word-based Simon effect on σ was not different from the absolute value of the location-based Simon effect, t(38) = .14, p = .892.

Discussion

The word-based Simon effect was obtained on mean RT, and the effect became larger as RT increased in the Vincentile analyses. This pattern replaces findings obtained for keypress responses in previous studies (Experiment 2 in Khalid & Ansorge, 2013; Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009, 2011), although left-right eye-movement responses have been shown to yield a decreasing delta plot (Khalid & Ansorge, 2013).

As predicted, we obtained the word-based Simon effects on μ, σ, and τ. The word-based Simon effect on RT may arise from a shift of the RT distribution and from the tail of the RT distribution in the incompatible condition, compared to that in the compatible condition, suggesting that the word-based Simon effect occurred in both fast and slow responses. Compared to the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, the word-based Simon effect was not different from it on mean RT, but was smaller than it on μ and larger than it on τ, suggesting that the word-based Simon effect has a greater contribution from the slow responses. Moreover, the word-based Simon effect on σ was positive, whereas the location-based Simon effect was negative, but the absolute value was not different: The variances across the RT distributions were not different for the two tasks, but the direction of the effect was opposite.

Experiment 3: Left- or right-pointing arrows

Method

Twenty new right-handed subjects (nine males), aged 18–24 years, took part in Experiment 3. Apparatus, stimuli, procedure, design were identical to Experiment 2, except that the location words were replaced with green or red left- or right-pointing arrows (➞ and ➞, 0.85° × 0.85°, with 0.51° × 0.85° for arrow head and 0.34° × 0.15° for arrow bar), and responses were made to their colors.

Results

Test trials with incorrect response to the target (3.2 %) and RTs longer than 1,500 ms or less than 150 ms (1.6 %), and test trials RTs were not within three standard deviation of mean (2.1 %) for each condition per participant were excluded from the RT analyses.

PE.

The main effect of arrow-based Simon was significant, t(19) = 5.25, p < .001, indicating a Simon effect of 5.2 %, with fewer response errors in the compatible (2.2 %) than in the incompatible condition (7.4 %).

RT.

Vincentile analysis. The arrow-based Simon effect was significant, F(1, 19)= 23.40, p < .001, MSE = 6,699, η p 2 = .552, reflecting a Simon effect of 56 ms. Moreover, the interaction between bin and arrow-based Simon was significant, F(4, 76) = 11.91, p < .001, MSE = 760, η p 2 = .385. The Simon effect was significant for each bin, ps < .001. Trend analysis showed that the arrow-based Simon effect increased linearly from bin 1 to bin 5, F(1, 19) = 13.27, p = .002, MSE = 5,049, η p 2 = .419 (see Fig. 2). In addition, a comparison analysis showed that the arrow-based Simon effect on mean RT was larger than both the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, t(38) = 2.85, p = .007, and the word-based Simon effect in Experiment 2, t(38) = 2.77, p = .009.

Ex-Gaussian analysis. The arrow-based Simon effect was significant on μ, t(19) = 4.30, p < .001, on σ, t(19) = 3.40, p = .003, and on τ, t(19) = 2.84, p = .010, indicating 37 ms, 20 ms, and 18 ms Simon effects on these parameters, separately. Compared to the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, the arrow-based Simon effect was larger on σ, t(38) = 4.16, p < .001, and on τ, t(38) = 3.00, p = .005, but not on μ, t(38) = 1.34, p = .188. In addition, there also was no difference in absolute value of σ, t(38) = 1.21, p = .234, whereas the direction of the effect is reverse. Moreover, the arrow-based Simon effect was larger than the word-based Simon effect in Experiment 2 on μ, t(38) = 2.88, p = .007, but not on τ, t(38) = .75, p = .460, or σ, t(38) = 1.32, p < .195.

Discussion

The arrow-based Simon effect was obtained on mean RT, and the effect became larger at longer RTs in the Vincentile analyses, replicating findings obtained in previous studies (Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2011). As predicted, we obtained the word-based Simon effects on μ and σ, and on τ. The arrow-based Simon effect on RT may arise from a shift of the RT distribution and from the tail of the RT distribution in the incompatible condition, compared to that in the compatible condition, suggesting that the arrow-based Simon effect occurred in the fast and slow responses.

Compared to the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, the arrow-based Simon effect was larger on τ and mean RT but not on μ, suggesting that the arrow-based Simon effect had a lesser contribution from rapid automatic activation and a larger contribution from relatively slow responses. Compared to the word-based Simon effect in Experiment 2, the arrow-based Simon effect was larger on μ and mean RT but not on τ, suggesting that the arrow-based Simon effect had a larger contribution of rapid automatic activation. Moreover, the arrow-based Simon effect on σ was not different from the word-based Simon effect and negative location-based Simon effect in absolute value, suggesting that the variances across the RT distribution were not different in these tasks. The results taken together imply that the arrow-based Simon effect is a combination of the location-based and word-based effects. There was no difference from the former on μ and no difference from the latter on τ. Thus, the ex-Gaussian analysis provides evidence that the arrow-based Simon effect was largest because it had both spatial and symbolic components.

General discussion

The current study examined whether various Simon effects in different tasks occur on the ex-Gaussian parameters, and whether these effects are different on each parameter. In Experiment 1, we observed a location-based Simon effect on RT, and it decreased from bins 1 to 5. The ex-Gaussian analysis showed that the location-based Simon effect was on μ (with a reverse effect on the variability, σ) but not on τ. In Experiments 2 and 3, the word-based and arrow-based Simon effects were obtained on RT, and they increased from bins 1 to 5. The word-based Simon effect showed approximately similar sized effects on μ and τ, with the former being smaller than that for the location-based Simon effect and the latter being larger. The arrow-based Simon effect did not differ from that of the location-based Simon effect on μ, but it was larger than it on τ. The reverse relation held for the arrow-based Simon effect in comparison to the word-based Simon effect. In other words, the largest Simon effect, that for left- and right-pointing arrows, is composed of two components, a normal distribution effect similar to that of spatial-location stimuli and an exponential tail similar to that of location-word stimuli.

The current findings obtained in the Vincentile analyses are consistent with previous findings that the location-based Simon effect with horizontal S-R arrangement decreases as RTs increase, whereas the word- and arrow-based Simon effects increase (Miles & Proctor, 2012; Pellicano et al., 2009; Proctor et al., 2009). Moreover, the arrow-based Simon effect occurred for each bin, whereas the word-based Simon effect occurred for the late bins, as shown in Fig. 2 which may be because the arrows activated both visual- and semantic-spatial codes. The visual-spatial codes may provide the basis for the arrow-based Simon effect in the early RT bins, as for the location-based Simon effect. Likewise, the semantic-spatial codes may provide the basis for the arrow-based Simon effect in the late RT bins, as for the word-based Simon effect, as shown in Fig. 2. The conclusion that arrows activate visual-spatial codes as well as semantic-spatial codes differs slightly from that reached by Miles and Proctor (2012) on the basis of correlational analyses for the three types of Simon effects. This difference likely is a consequence of the arrows having a distinct left or right arrowhead in the present study but not in theirs.

The present results are compatible with the dual-route model (De Jong et al., 1994; Hommel, 1997; Kornblum et al., 1990; Ulrich et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 1999; Zorzi & Umiltà, 1995), which posits that the speed of task-irrelevant spatial location activated automatically, determines the increasing or decreasing delta plots for the various Simon effects. These results in the Vincentile analyses also were compatible with Wascher’s theory of separate mechanisms (Pratte et al., 2010; Wascher et al., 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2007; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005) that proposes a “visuomotor Simon effect” underlying the location-based Simon task and a “cognitive Simon effect” occurring in other forms of the Simon task.

The reverse location-based Simon effect on σ showed a larger variability for the compatible than for the incompatible RT distribution, whereas the positive word- or arrow-based Simon effect on σ showed a reverse pattern. They are consistent with the simulation in an Appendix of De Jong et al.’s (1994) article and the results of Zhang and Kornblum’s (1997) study. Zhang and Kornblum reported a mathematical proof that showed that the decrease or increase of a compatibility effect with the increase of RT is separately determined by whether the variance of the RT distribution for compatible trials is smaller or larger than that for incompatible trials. Therefore, the increasing or decreasing delta plot seems to be linked to the positive or negative Simon effect on σ, respectively.

Word- and arrow-based Simon effects on μ, σ, and τ were obtained and they were positive, consistent with the finding in the color-word Stroop task (see Balota et al. 2010; Heathcote et al. 1991; Spieler et al. 1996). As shown in Fig. 2, the word- and arrow-based Simon effects on RT arose from both a shift of the RT distribution (a change in μ) and the tail of the RT distribution (a change in τ) in the incompatible condition, compared to the compatible condition. This pattern suggests that the word- and arrow-based Simon effects occur regardless of whether responding is slow or fast. However, the word-based Simon effect was smaller than the arrow-based Simon effect on μ and mean RT but not on τ and σ, suggesting that the arrow-based Simon effect on mean RT had a relatively greater contribution from fast responses. The disparity on μ and mean RT are not explained by Wascher’s theory of separate mechanisms (Pratte et al., 2010; Wascher et al., 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005, 2007), as both the arrow- and word-based Simon effects are classified as cognitive Simon effects in this theory. However, the disparity on μ and mean RT might be explained by the dual-route model (De Jong et al., 1994; Hommel, 1997; Kornblum et al., 1990; Ulrich et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 1999; Zorzi & Umiltà, 1995), as the relative speeds of the automatic activation produced by the arrows might be faster than that produced by the location words, given that spatial words are more difficult to interpret compared to arrow stimuli, probably due to computational complexity and attentional requirements underlying the comprehension of the stimulus (Gibson & Kingstone, 2006).

The location-based Simon effect occurred on μ, whereas a reverse effect was obtained on σ and no effect was obtained on τ. They showed that the location-based Simon effect on RT mainly arose from a shift of the RT distribution (a change in μ) but not from the tail of the RT distribution (a change in τ) in the incompatible condition, compared to that in the compatible condition, as shown in Fig. 1. They suggest that the location-based Simon effect on RT mainly is evident in faster responses but not slower ones. No location-based Simon effect occurring on τ is in agreement with the view that suggested in prior studies that τ is associated with response conflict or competition (e.g., Spieler et al. 1996; Steinhauser & Hübner, 2009; White et al., 2016).

The location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1 and the word-based Simon effect in Experiment 2 were not different on mean RT or on the absolute value of σ, but the former was larger than the latter on μ and was smaller than the latter on τ, suggesting that the word-based Simon effect arises more from slow responses, whereas the location-based Simon effect arises mainly from fast responses. This disparity may be because the stimulus location activates visual-spatial codes and the location word activates semantic-spatial codes, and the relative speeds of visual-spatial codes may be faster than the semantic-spatial codes, resulting in the different results on μ, σ and τ, which is consistent with the dual-route model (De Jong et al., 1994; Hommel, 1997; Kornblum et al., 1990; Ulrich et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 1999; Zorzi & Umiltà, 1995). Wascher’s theory of separate mechanisms (Pratte et al., 2010; Wascher et al., 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005, 2007) also may explain these different results on μ, σ, and τ, as two different mechanism may be responsible for the location-based and word-based Simon effects, respectively.

Comparing the location-based Simon effect in Experiment 1, the arrow-based Simon effect in Experiment 2 was larger on τ and mean RT but not on μ and on the absolute value of σ, suggesting that the arrow-based Simon effect on mean RT arose from both fast and slow responses, whereas the location-based Simon effect mainly arose from fast responses. This disparity could be explain by the dual-route model (De Jong et al., 1994; Hommel, 1997; Kornblum et al., 1990; Ulrich et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 1999; Zorzi & Umiltà, 1995), as the stimulus location activate the visual-spatial codes, whereas the arrow activates both the visual-spatial and semantic-spatial codes, which make the spatial properties of arrow activated quickly and its semantic properties develop slowly. Wascher’s theory of separate mechanisms (Pratte et al., 2010; Wascher et al., 2001; Wiegand & Wascher, 2005, 2007) may explain different results on σ and τ, while it may not explain indifference on μ for the location- and arrow-based Simon effects.

These different patterns of results of the Simon effects generated by the three spatial modes on ex-Gaussian parameters suggest that spatial information conveyed by stimulus location, location word and arrow direction has different time courses of activation. However, more research is needed to confirm these findings, given that few prior studies have used the ex-Gaussian approach and the current results were from between-subject or between-experiment comparisons. Moreover, we expect this technique to haves a range of applications to various issues in the field of spatial compatibility effects, given that it provides a clear and precise methodology for studying the pattern of response activation across the RT distribution in compatibility tasks.

In conclusion, the location-based Simon effect occurred on mean RT, μ, and σ but not on τ, and the effect was not different from the word-based Simon effect on mean RT but on μ, σ, and τ. Moreover, the location-based Simon effect was not different from the arrow-based Simon effect obtained on μ but on mean RT, σ, and τ. The arrow-based Simon effect was not different from the word-based Simon effect on τ and σ but on μ and mean RT. These different results on mean RT and ex-Gaussian parameters provide evidence that spatial information conveyed by physical location and by location word and arrow direction are different in the time courses of activation.