Emotion regulation refers to the processes engaged when individuals try to influence the type or amount of emotion that they experience and how emotions are expressed (Gross, 1998b). Emotion regulatory behaviors and strategies are normally used to increase or decrease emotional experiences, and successful emotion regulation is related to beneficial psychological, social, and physical health outcomes (Gross, 2002).

One of the most flexible and efficacious emotion regulation strategies is reappraisal, which involves reinterpreting the meaning of emotionally evocative stimuli (Gross, 1998b; Gross & Levenson, 1993; Richards & Gross, 2000). By changing a stimulus’s affective value, reappraisal can effectively up- or down-regulate subjective reports of emotion, facial expression, and physiological measures of arousal (Dillon & Labar, 2005; Eippert et al., 2007; Gross, 1998a; Gross & Levenson, 1997; Hajcak, Moser, & Simons, 2006; Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006; Jackson, Malmstadt, Larson, & Davidson, 2000). A working model of the cognitive control of emotion (Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002) posits that emotion generation and regulation involve the interaction of appraisal systems, such as the amygdala (e.g., encoding the affective properties of the stimuli in a bottom-up fashion) with control systems implemented in the prefrontal cortex (PFC, which supports top-down cognitive control; Miller, 2000; Ochsner et al., 2002; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of reappraisal have provided evidence for this model. These studies implicate increases in ventromedial and lateral PFC activity (Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin, & Davidson, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2002; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004; Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist, & Ochsner, 2008) and decreases in amygdala activity (Ochsner & Gross, 2007; Phillips, Henry, Hosie, & Milne, 2008) when negative emotion is down-regulated via reappraisal. Specifically, left lateral PFC has been shown to be differentially activated during the cognitive reappraisal of unpleasant stimuli (Davidson, 2004; Goldin, McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2008; however, see Ochsner & Gross, 2007; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004; Wager et al., 2008).

In addition to fMRI, event-related potentials (ERPs) have also been used to provide more temporally fine-grained indices of the effects of reappraisal. Specifically, the late positive potential (LPP), a widely distributed ERP component that is larger throughout the presentation of emotional as compared to neutral pictures and words (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000; Dillon, Cooper, Grent-’t-Jong, Woldorff, & LaBar, 2006; Foti & Hajcak, 2008; Hajcak, Dunning, & Foti, 2007; Hajcak & Olvet, 2008; Pastor et al., 2008; Schupp et al., 2000) has been shown to be sensitive to reappraisal instructions (Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006; Krompinger, Moser, & Simons, 2008; Moser, Krompinger, Dietz, & Simons, 2009). For example, Hajcak and Nieuwenhuis found that when participants were asked to reappraise unpleasant pictures, the LPP was reduced relative to the control condition. Similar modulations of the LPP following more open-ended emotion regulation instructions have also been reported (Krompinger et al., 2008; Moser, Hajcak, Bukay, & Simons, 2006). This LPP modulation is thought to share similar functional sensitivity with amygdala reactivity in response to emotion regulation manipulations (Hajcak, MacNamara, & Olvet, 2010).

Although ERPs have been used to track the effects of cognitive reappraisal on the processing of emotional stimuli (i.e., as a dependent variable), electrocortical activity has not been used to index the PFC-mediated regulatory mechanisms responsible for these effects. In the present study, we examined the novel possibility that event-related EEG oscillations may be quantified to explore cortical activation that is enhanced during cognitive reappraisal. Specifically, frontal alpha (i.e., 8–13 Hz) appears to reflect the inhibition and disengagement of task-relevant cortical regions (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Rohm, Pollhuber, & Stadler, 2000; Klimesch, Sauseng, & Hanslmayr, 2007; Knyazev, 2007). For example, task-related decreases in alpha power have been reported in response to increased attention (Babiloni, Miniussi, et al., 2004; Dockree et al., 2004; Pfurtscheller & Lopes da Silva, 1999), memory retrieval (Babiloni, Babiloni, et al., 2004; Hwang et al., 2005; Klimesch, 1999; Krause, Astrom, Karrasch, Laine, & Sillanmaki, 1999), and other cognitive processes related to frontal regulatory mechanisms. Thus, reduced power in the alpha band appears to index increased frontal activation.

In line with previous work, we predicted that the parietal LPP would be larger for unpleasant than for neutral pictures and that the LPP would be reduced following instructions to reappraise unpleasant pictures. On the basis of fMRI research implicating increased activity in left frontal regions during reappraisal, we examined whether cognitive reappraisal would be accompanied by reduced alpha spectral power over left frontal regions and whether greater decreases in frontal alpha (i.e., increased frontal activation) would predict a reduced LPP. Additionally, although the EEG/ERP evidence regarding gender differences in emotion processing and regulation is not very clear (Bradley, Codispoti, Sabatinelli, & Lang, 2001; Meyers & Smith, 1987; Rozenkrants & Polich, 2008; Weinberg & Hajcak, 2010), we explored gender differences in the proposed LPP and alpha modulation during cognitive reappraisal of emotional stimuli.

Method

Participants

A group of 49 undergraduate students (23 male, 26 female) participated in the study. Five of the participants were excluded due to poor quality EEG recordings; therefore, 44 participants (23 male, 21 female) were included in the final EEG analyses. The study was approved by the Stony Brook University Institutional Review Board (IRB), and all participants gave informed consent and received course credit.

Stimulus materials

A set of 50 unpleasant pictures (e.g., car crashes or angry dogs) and 50 neutral pictures (e.g., neutral landscapes or household objects) were selected from the International Affective Picture SystemFootnote 1 (IAPS; Lang, Bradley & Cuthbert, 2005). Normative ratings indicated that the unpleasant pictures were less pleasant (valence: M = 2.51, SD = 0.78) and more emotionally arousing (M = 5.78, SD = 0.68) than the neutral pictures (M = 5.02, SD = 0.44, and M = 3.44, SD = 0.41, respectively; higher numbers indicate more pleasant and higher arousal ratings). Each picture was displayed in color and filled the computer screen (which measured 48.26 cm diagonally). Participants were seated approximately 60 cm from the screen, and the images occupied about 40 ° of visual angle horizontally and vertically. Partway through picture presentation, the participants heard the word “normal” or “reduce” played through earphones, which served as the emotion regulation instructions.

Procedure

The participants were told that they would be viewing unpleasant and neutral pictures and that during picture presentation they would hear one of two words. If they heard the word “normal,” participants were told that they should continue viewing the picture as they normally would. If they heard the word “reduce,” they were told that they should reduce their emotional response to the picture by making the picture seem less emotional. The participants were told that they should do this by changing either the meaning of the picture or their perspective on the depicted characters and events. For example, they could tell themselves that the people depicted in a house fire would survive, or that the photo of a gruesome war scene was taken from a movie. The experimenter provided examples by presenting IAPS pictures and reinterpreting the picture meanings for the participant. Special attention was paid to the neutral pictures; specifically, the experimenter explained that it might be difficult for participants to reduce their response to a picture that was already relatively neutral, but that they should try to do so nonetheless. Again, the experimenter gave examples using neutral IAPS pictures (e.g., a building in a picture was boring and nothing interesting happened there).

Next, participants performed six practice trials (on IAPS pictures that were not used in the actual task) to familiarize themselves with the paradigm. After each of the “reduce” trials, the experimenter asked participants to indicate how they had reduced their emotional response to the picture, thus providing an opportunity for the experimenter to determine that participants had understood the directions and were completing the task as instructed (i.e., by using cognitive reappraisal).

Similarly to other emotion regulation paradigms (e.g., Moser et al., 2006), and to minimize the task difficulty, trials were blocked by picture type into four blocks—two neutral and two unpleasant. The block order was determined randomly for each participant. Prior to each picture-type block, participants were informed about the types of pictures that they would be seeing in the upcoming block (e.g., the screen read, “In the next block, you will only see unpleasant pictures.”).

Each trial began with a white fixation cross that was presented in the center of a black background for 1,000 ms. Following this, participants viewed an unpleasant or neutral picture, depending on the block; 1,000 ms after picture onset, they received the auditory instruction informing them to continue viewing the picture as they normally would (“normal”) or to reduce their emotional response to the picture using cognitive reappraisal (“reduce”). The picture remained onscreen for 6,000 ms beyond the instruction; thus, the total picture presentation time was 7,000 ms.Footnote 2 Participants were asked to reduce their emotional response to exactly half of the unpleasant and half of the neutral pictures in the experiment. Across the entire experiment, there were 100 trials: 25 unpleasant normal, 25 unpleasant reduce, 25 neutral normal, and 25 neutral reduce. Each participant viewed all pictures, and picture assignments to the “reduce” or “normal” conditions were determined randomly within the constraints noted above. The order of the “reduce” and “normal” trials was determined randomly for each participant; the intertrial interval was 1,000 ms, during which time participants viewed a white fixation cross centered on a black background. The participants received a break after each block.

EEG recording and data reduction

Continuous EEG was recorded using an elastic cap and the ActiveTwo BioSemi system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). Sixty-four electrode sites were used, based on the 10–20 system, as well as one electrode on each of the left and right mastoids. Four facial electrodes recorded the electrooculogram (EOG) generated from eyeblinks and eye movements: Vertical eye movements and blinks were measured with two electrodes placed ~1 cm above and below the right eye; horizontal eye movements were measured with two electrodes placed ~1 cm beyond the outer edge of each eye. The EEG signal was preamplified at the electrode to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. The data were digitized at 24-bit resolution with a least significant bit (LSB) value of 31.25 nV and a sampling rate of 512 Hz, using a low-pass fifth-order sync filter with a –3-dB cutoff point at 104 Hz. Each active electrode was measured online with respect to a common mode sense active electrode producing a monopolar (nondifferential) channel.

Offline preprocessing was performed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and custom MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA) scripts. Data were filtered with low and high cutoffs of 0.01 and 30 Hz, respectively, and were then re-referenced to the averaged electrical activity from all 64 scalp sites. Due to the onsets of two events in each trial (i.e., the picture followed by the auditory instruction), two separate analyses were conducted. First, ERPs were analyzed in response to picture onset; to this end, the EEG was segmented for unpleasant and neutral pictures beginning 200 ms prior to the picture onset and continuing for 1,000 ms (i.e., until the onset of the reappraisal instruction). Second, for the analysis of the electrophysiological response to reappraisal, the EEG was segmented for each condition beginning 200 ms prior to the instruction onset and continuing for 6,000 ms (i.e., until the end of the picture presentation). For each trial, the baseline was defined as the 200 ms prior to the event (picture or instruction onset), which was subtracted from the poststimulus onset data for baseline correction.

Eye-blink and ocular corrections were performed using the partial signal space projection (pSSP) method proposed by Nolte and Hämäläinen (2001), such that the contribution to the estimated spatial structure of eye-blink artifact was removed only from the artifact-ridden epochs, leaving as much information as possible in the data (Nolte & Hämäläinen, 2001). This artifact rejection procedure identified a voltage step of more than 75 μV between sample points and a peak-to-peak voltage difference of 150 μV within an epoch. Additional artifacts were identified through visual inspection, and the contaminated epochs were subsequently rejected.

Robust averaging was used to create artifact-free ERPs (Wager, Keller, Lacey, & Jonides, 2005); spectral power over the alpha (8–13 Hz) frequency range was obtained by applying a Morlet wavelet transform from 150 ms before the onset of the auditory instruction to 5,500 ms after the auditory instruction. This “windowing” of the original epoch was performed to avoid edge effects—distortions in the frequency transformation at the edges of an epoch (Harris, 1978). To compute induced (non-phase-locked) spectral power, the average waveform was subtracted from each individual trial before applying the time-frequency transform to the single-trial data, which were then averaged to yield induced oscillatory power. The resulting spectral power over time was expressed as percentage amplitude changes relative to a prestimulus baseline period, to standardize power levels across the frequency band. Spectral power was than computed at each electrode as the average alpha activity over the poststimulus epoch length.

Statistical analyses

Quantification of electrocortical signals was performed separately for the pre- and postinstruction durations. In line with prior work in which pictures have been presented for durations greater than 1,000 ms (Dunning et al., 2011; Foti & Hajcak, 2008; Hajcak et al., 2010; Littel & Franken, 2011), we divided the postinstruction period into several time windows. Note that these windows (1,500–2,000, 2,000–4,500, and 4,500–7,000 ms) were labeled relative to the picture onset (“early,” “mid,” and “late”). Since the preinstruction period only lasted 1,000 ms, we did not divide this period into more than one time window, which is also consistent with our prior research (MacNamara, Foti, & Hajcak, 2009; MacNamara, Ochsner, & Hajcak, 2011). Importantly, the LPP was scored in an identical time window in both the pre- and early postinstruction periods (i.e., from 500 to 1,000 ms following both picture and instruction onsets). The LPP was scored where it was maximal—at a pooling consisting of electrodes CPz, Pz, and POz (see Fig. 1A). Given our hypothesis regarding frontal alpha, we quantified alpha-band power separately for each hemisphere (left, at FC5, F7, F3; right, at FC4, F8, and F4).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Differential scalp topographies of late positive potential (LPP) amplitudes (post reappraisal instruction onset) in response to a normally viewed unpleasant pictures minus normally viewed neutral pictures; b normally viewed unpleasant pictures minus those that were reappraised; and c normally viewed neutral pictures minus those that were reappraised, during the early (left), middle (center), and late (right) LPP windows

The preinstruction LPP was analyzed with a 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (gender: male or female) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA). The postinstruction LPP was analyzed with a 3 (LPP window: early, middle, or late) × 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (regulation: normal or reduced) × 2 (gender: male or female) mixed-model ANOVA. Alpha power was analyzed using a 2 (laterality: left or right) × 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (regulation: normal or reduced) × 2 (gender: male or female) mixed-model ANOVA. In all analyses, the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied for cases in which Mauchly’s test showed that the assumption of sphericity was not met. Significant interactions were followed up using post-hoc tests. If significant, the LPP and alpha effects were intercorrelated separately for each task condition and also as differential scores (normal minus reduced) for each picture type in order to explore the underlying associations between these electrocortical measures.

Results

Figure 2 shows the grand-averaged waveforms from 0 to 7,000 ms following picture onset for three representative midline electrodes. The LPP amplitudes and alpha power for each task condition are presented in Table 1.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Grand-averaged ERP waveforms from 0 to 7,000 ms following picture onset at representative midline electrode sites, presented separately for unpleasant and neutral pictures in each of the “normal” and “reduced” conditions

Table 1 ERP amplitudes and EEG spectral power (reported as means ± SEMs) for each task condition following instruction onset

Since none of the analyses showed significant gender differences and/or gender-related interactions (ps > .1), the data were reanalyzed without modeling Gender as a factor. Thus, the preinstruction LPP was reanalyzed using paired t tests (unpleasant vs. neutral); postinstruction LPP, using a 3 (LPP window: early, middle, or late) × 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (regulation: normal or reduced) ANOVA; and postinstruction alpha, using a 2 (laterality: left or right) × 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (regulation: normal or reduced) ANOVA. The results of these analyses are reported below.

Pre-instruction ERPs

As expected, there was a significantly higher LPP amplitude in response to unpleasant as compared to neutral pictures [t(43) = 5.06, p < .001; unpleasant > neutral].

Post-instruction ERPs

We found significant effects of LPP window [F(2, 86) = 16.37, p < .001; early > middle > late] and picture type [F(1, 43) = 6.48, p < .05; unpleasant > neutral], as well as an LPP window by regulation interaction [F(2, 86) = 3.72, p < .05]. The three-way interaction between LPP window, picture type, and regulation did not reach significance [F(2, 86) = 2.57, p < .1]. Post-hoc tests revealed that, unlike in the early and late (ps > .1) LPP windows, reappraisal reduced the LPP in the middle LPP window [t(43) = 2.20, p < .05] across participants. This reappraisal effect was primarily driven by the unpleasant pictures [t(43) = 2.70, p < .01] rather than the neutral pictures (p > .1), in line with the trend toward a three-way interaction noted above. Other main effects and interactions did not reach significance (ps > .1). The scalp distributions of the LPP modulation in response to unpleasant and neutral pictures are shown in Fig. 1B and C, respectively.

Post-instruction alpha

We found a significant effect of picture type [F(1, 43) = 4.79, p < .05; unpleasant > neutral] and a three-way laterality by picture type by regulation interaction [F(1, 43) = 5.25, p < .05]. The main effect of regulation condition approached but did not reach significance [F(1, 43) = 3.56, p = .066; normal > reduced]; all other main effects and interactions also failed to reach significance (ps > .1).

Post-hoc analyses were performed separately for each hemisphere using a 2 (picture type: unpleasant or neutral) × 2 (regulation: normal or reduced) repeated measures ANOVA. The results indicated a significant effect of picture type in both hemispheres [left, F(1, 43) = 3.97, p < .05; right, F(1, 43) = 4.96, p < .05], such that alpha power was significantly higher in response to unpleasant as compared to neutral pictures. However, the regulation effect was only significant in the left hemisphere [left, F(1, 43) = 5.05, p < .05, normal > reduced; right, p > .1]. The picture type by regulation interaction did not reach significance in either hemisphere (ps > .1). Thus, across all study participants, we found a bilateral increase in frontal alpha power in response to unpleasant as compared to neutral pictures; moreover, alpha power was decreased following reappraisal as compared to control instructions in the left hemisphere (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Induced frontal alpha power for all task conditions (unpleasant normal, unpleasant reduced, neutral normal, and neutral reduced) following the instruction onset, shown separately for each hemisphere

In contrast to what we expected, there were no significant correlations between LPP amplitude and frontal alpha power (all ps > .1). Moreover, preinstruction emotional reactivity (LPP amplitude in response to unpleasant pictures minus LPP in response to neutral pictures) did not predict emotion regulation effects (postinstruction LPP amplitude and alpha power in response to pictures that were viewed normally minus those that were reappraised; all ps > .1).

Discussion

In the present study, we used electrophysiological data to investigate the cortical activation related to cognitive reappraisal. In line with previous work, unpleasant pictures elicited a more positive LPP than did neutral pictures (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Foti, Hajcak, & Dien, 2009; Hajcak et al., 2007; Pastor et al., 2008), and the LPP in response to unpleasant pictures was reduced following instructions to reappraise (Foti & Hajcak, 2008; Hajcak et al., 2006; Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006). In the present study, we also showed that frontal alpha increased bilaterally in response to unpleasant versus neutral pictures. Moreover, cognitive reappraisal was found to reduce left frontal alpha band power, suggesting increased cortical activation in this condition (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 2007) and that the left PFC may be differentially activated during cognitive reappraisal (Davidson, 2004; Goldin et al., 2008; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004). Finally, the present results did not reveal gender differences in LPP amplitude and frontal alpha power in response to cognitive reappraisal.

The LPP effects (i.e., reduced LPP amplitude in response to reappraised as compared to normally viewed pictures) highlight the role of top-down influences on the evaluation of affective stimuli (Hajcak et al., 2010). In the present study, the effect of reappraisal was observed in the middle LPP window, where reappraisal-related LPP modulations have previously been shown to be maximal (MacNamara et al., 2009). The lack of reappraisal effects in the early window might be attributed to the fact that unlike previous emotion regulation studies (Foti & Hajcak, 2008; Hajcak et al., 2006; Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006), reappraisal instructions were presented after picture onset in the present study, potentially delaying top-down attentional effects (see also Hajcak, Dunning, & Foti, 2009). During the late LPP window, the effect of reappraisal may not have reached significance because of increased variability in participants’ success at sustaining cognitive reappraisal, as is suggested by the high standard errors of the means in this window (Table 1).

The novelty of the present results lies in the finding of reduced left frontal alpha power as a function of cognitive reappraisal. Frontal alpha-band oscillation appears to index cortical inhibition (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 2000; Klimesch et al., 2007; Knyazev, 2007), and its reduction has been linked to increased PFC activity during working memory and attentional processes (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 2007; Park et al., 2006). In the context of reappraisal, regulatory control and goal-directed monitoring appear to rely on the lateral PFC (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Miller & Cohen, 2001; Ochsner & Gross, 2007; Wager et al., 2008). Therefore, reduced frontal alpha power might provide a useful electrocortical index of PFC activation during cognitive reappraisal. Importantly, reappraisal-related reductions in frontal alpha power were observed at left but not at right frontal electrode sites, which is of interest because functional neuroimaging studies have also demonstrated greater activation in left PFC during cognitive reappraisal (Johnstone et al., 2007; Ochsner et al., 2002).

One possible reason for the left-sided bias in alpha modulation could be that reappraisal seems to be a verbally mediated process (Ochsner & Gross, 2004), and left prefrontal regions have been implicated in interference tasks involving verbal stimuli (Bunge, Ochsner, Desmond, Glover, & Gabrieli, 2001; D’Esposito, Postle, Jonides, & Smith, 1999). Specifically, the left inferior frontal gyrus (Broca’s area) is implicated in inhibitory control processes (Swick, Ashley, & Turken, 2008), especially those related to semantic selection (Thompson-Schill et al., 1998) and interference resolution in working memory (Thompson-Schill et al., 2002), and its activity has been shown to be negatively correlated with alpha rhythm (Goldman, Stern, Engel, & Cohen, 2002). Therefore, future studies might evaluate the possibility of examining frontal alpha in emotion regulation tasks that do not require verbally mediated changes in stimulus meaning (e.g., attentional deployment; Dunning & Hajcak, 2009; Hajcak et al., 2007; Hajcak, Macnamara, Foti, Ferri, & Keil, 2011).

Interestingly, our results showed increased alpha in response to unpleasant as compared to neutral pictures across both hemispheres. Similar findings of increased alpha spectral power in response to emotionally salient stimuli have been reported previously (e.g., Aftanas, Varlamov, Pavlov, Makhnev, & Reva, 2001). Given that conscious down-regulation of emotion may rely on prefrontally mediated inhibition of the amygdala (Beauregard, Lévesque, & Bourgouin, 2001; Lévesque et al., 2003; Lévesque et al., 2004; Ochsner et al., 2002; Ochsner, Knierim, et al., 2004; Phan et al., 2005; Urry et al., 2006), and given previous reports of a reciprocal relationship between the amygdala and prefrontal brain regions (Amaral & Price, 1984; Ghashghaei & Barbas, 2002; Ghashghaei, Hilgetag, & Barbas, 2007), it is plausible that increased emotional arousal may be associated with reduced frontal activity, manifested in increased frontal alpha for unpleasant relative to neutral pictures. It is important to note, however, that in the present study, LPP amplitude and alpha power were uncorrelated.

The present results showing higher alpha power in response to unpleasant as compared to neutral are also consistent with results from recent neuroimaging studies that have investigated the differences between the effects of spontaneous and effortful emotion regulation. For example, Viviani et al. (2010) showed decreased activity in frontal executive attentional areas (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) during spontaneous avoidance of unpleasant relative to neutral stimuli, whereas during intentional emotional regulation, these authors reported increased activity in the dorsolateral PFC. In light of these results, these authors argued that spontaneous emotion regulation should not be attributed to the intense recruitment of effortful control processes (Viviani et al., 2010). Similarly, the present results suggest decreased frontal cortical activation (increased frontal alpha) in response to unpleasant relative to neutral pictures during normal viewing (i.e., spontaneous emotion regulation), as well as increased frontal activation (decreased frontal alpha) during reappraisal (i.e., instructed emotion regulation) as compared to normal-viewing trials.

It should be noted that frontal alpha effects may also arise due to the choice of referencing scheme used in analyzing EEG data (Hagemann, Naumann, & Thayer, 2001). In keeping with several recently published studies (see, e.g., Accortt, Stewart, Coan, Manber, & Allen, 2011; Babiloni et al., 2012; Del Percio et al., 2010; Schmidt & Hanslmayr, 2009), and given some limitations across all reference schemes (Hagemann et al., 2001), we used average referencing to analyze frontal alpha. One limitation of using average referencing is that it produces frontal alpha effects, which are mere reflections of occipital alpha (Hagemann et al., 2001). Nevertheless, the present results of lateralized modulation of frontal alpha in response to cognitive control would not be expected as an artifact of the average reference scheme.

Although there is no precedence for investigating gender differences on LPP amplitude and event-related alpha during emotion regulation per se, recent fMRI studies have reported inconsistent gender differences in the neural correlates of emotion regulation (Domes et al., 2010; McRae, Oschsner, Mauss, Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008; McRae, Reiman, Fort, Chen, & Lane, 2008). Specifically, McRae and colleagues (McRae, Ochsner, et al., 2008; McRae, Reiman, et al., 2008) reported enhanced activity in the ventral striatum and parts of the PFC (the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and medial PFC), as well as attenuated amygdala reactivity, in women as compared to men during cognitive reappraisal of emotional stimuli. In contrast, Domes et al. (2010) found greater prefrontal activity in males as compared to females during the down-regulation of negative emotion, with no gender differences in amygdala activity or self-reported regulation success. The lack of gender difference in LPP and frontal alpha modulation in response to cognitive reappraisal, even with as large a sample as the present one, perhaps points to similarities across both genders in recruitment of the underlying neural substrates during reappraisal.

The present results extend prior neuroimaging findings to ERPs and suggest that the emotion-modulatory effects of reappraisal may stem from interactions between emotional appraisal processes in emotion-related structures, including the amygdala (Ochsner et al., 2002; Ochsner & Gross, 2004) and in prefrontal regions (D’Esposito, Postle, & Rypma, 2000; Miller & Cohen, 2001). Future studies may investigate within-subjects associations between frontal alpha power and the LPP amplitude by optimizing the study design and data reduction procedures in order to investigate individual variability in emotion regulation. These analyses were not possible in the present study design, due to very low signal-to-noise ratios for each trial. Future studies may also add subjective measures of arousal, ability to regulate emotion, and task difficulty so as to examine emotion regulation as a function of individual differences in the subjective experience of emotional stimuli, and to determine how these measures relate to the frontal and limbic activation measured with fMRI.

Future studies may also investigate neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by deficits in emotion regulation, such as depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and drug addiction. Drug addiction, for example, involves profound impairments in emotion regulation (Fox, Axelrod, Paliwal, Sleeper, & Sinha, 2007); however, when asked to volitionally inhibit cue-induced craving in a laboratory environment, some drug-addicted individuals reported lower craving and showed decreased activity in the brain regions (orbitofrontal cortex) that process the motivational values of reinforcers, including drug-related cues, thereby allowing these individuals to retain some level of control over their drug-related cue reactivity (Volkow et al., 2010). These findings can be further explored using LPP and alpha measures to better understand the extent of emotion regulation impairment in drug addiction and related disorders.

In sum, the present study replicates previous findings of reduced LPP amplitudes in response to cognitive reappraisal and provides the first evidence of induced frontal-alpha spectral power reduction as an electrocortical correlate of cognitive reappraisal. These results suggest that time and frequency domain measures of scalp-recorded EEG activity may each contribute uniquely to the study of the neural substrates and cognitive processes underlying reappraisal. These data are largely consistent with existing functional neuroimaging work, which has highlighted the role of the amygdala–prefrontal circuit in emotion regulation, and they suggest that frontal alpha power may be a useful metric of emotion–cognition interactions involving the PFC.