Introduction

As we engage in different activities in our day-to-day lives, the passage of time can seem to ebb and flow in different contexts. Indeed, situations that reduce awareness of the passage of time, such as participating in a particularly engrossing activity, can give the impression that time has sped up (e.g., “time flies when you are having fun”), whereas situations that increase awareness of time, such as boredom and anticipation, can give the impression of time slowing (e.g., “a watched pot never boils”). This article considers factors affecting systematic distortions in perceived event duration that have been associated with an event’s predictability.

A number of previous studies have shown that oddball events that deviate from what is expected in an otherwise predictable stream of events tend to be perceived to last longer than expected events with the same objective duration (Pariyadath & Eagleman, 2007; Seifried & Ulrich, 2010; Tse, Intrilligator, Rivest, & Cavanagh, 2004; van Wassenhove, Buonomano, Shimojo, & Shams, 2008). The general paradigm used in these studies presents participants with a deviant oddball stimulus embedded within a sequence of identical, or otherwise predictable, standard stimuli. The task for each sequence is to judge whether the duration of the oddball stimulus is shorter or longer than the duration of the standard. The basic finding is that individuals tend to overestimate the duration of the oddball stimulus.

Tse et al. (2004) conducted one of the first comprehensive investigations of the oddball effect, manipulating perceptual features of oddball stimuli in a sequence of standard stimuli where the standards ranged in duration from 75 to 2,100 ms. Stimuli mostly consisted of simple visual shapes but also included auditory stimulus conditions. Across stimulus conditions, oddballs were substantially overestimated in duration, with the magnitude of this overestimation ranging, on average, from as little as about 25 % for auditory oddballs to as much as nearly 60 % for visual oddballs. The amount of overestimation was found to be influenced by the salience of the oddball stimulus, where salience was operationalized roughly as the amount of perceived change per unit of objective time (e.g., when the oddball was a visually expanding disk, there was more temporal expansion than when the oddball was a stationary disk). The magnitude of the oddball effect also depended on the standard duration, suggesting that some minimal standard duration, estimated to be about 120 ms, was required to obtain the effect.

The theoretical account of the oddball effect proposed by Tse et al. (2004) is that the oddball stimulus receives enhanced attention (similar in form to attentional capture). On this view, enhanced attention to the oddball is hypothesized to increase its perceived duration by increasing the amount of accumulated temporal information over the oddball’s extent. From a mechanistic standpoint, this is realized by a pacemaker–accumulator (internal clock) model of timing, which consists of three stages: clock, memory, and decision (Gibbon, 1977; Gibbon, Church, & Meck, 1984; Treisman, 1963). Of primary interest for the enhanced-attention account is the clock stage, which includes pacemaker, switch, and accumulator components. The pacemaker generates a continuous series of pulses. In response to a to-be-timed stimulus, the switch closes, allowing pulses to flow into the accumulator. The accumulator “collects” the pulses, providing a representation of duration. Attention acts on the internal-clock process by increasing the effective number of pulses that flow into the accumulator (Church, 1984; Lejeune, 1998; Meck, 1983); thus, enhanced attention implies more accumulated pulses and longer perceived duration. A related theoretical account proposed by Ulrich, Nitschke, and Rammsayer (2006) is that the oddball effect is not due to enhanced attention per se but, rather, is associated with increased arousal, which is assumed to increase the rate of the pacemaker (Penton-Voak, Edwards, Percival, & Wearden, 1996; Treisman, Faulkner, Naish, & Brogan, 1990), also resulting in more accumulated pulses and longer perceived duration.

A different theoretical account is that the oddball effect is an indirect consequence of reduced neural activity in response to the repeated standard stimulus (Pariyadath & Eagleman, 2007, 2012; Schindel, Rowlands, & Arnold, 2011). On this repetition suppression view, the temporal expansion of the oddball event is a by-product of reduced neural activity to the repeated or more generally predictable stimulus, rather than the result of enhanced attention to the oddball stimulus. This view assumes that duration is represented by the magnitude of neural response. Thus, because exposure to the repeated standard stimulus produces reduced neural activity, the duration representation for the standard is temporally contracted (i.e., shortened), and an equivalent-duration oddball is perceived as longer in comparison. Some support for a repetition suppression account of the oddball effect comes from evidence of a “debut” effect (Pariyadath & Eagleman, 2007); namely, when participants are presented with a sequence of identical stimuli, the duration of the first stimulus tends to be perceived to be longer than the durations of successive repeated stimuli (see also Rose & Summers, 1995, for a similar finding). Also supporting a repetition suppression account is the finding that oddballs that occur in later serial positions tend to be overestimated to a greater degree than oddballs that occur in earlier serial positions (Pariyadath & Eagleman, 2012).

The overarching goal of the present study was to provide new empirical constraints for theory. The experiments reported below are, in part, motivated by the fact that in the general oddball paradigm, the deviant oddball stimulus is, by design, a physically different stimulus (i.e., the oddball is defined by what the stimulus is). The oddball, however, is also necessarily an unlikely stimulus in an otherwise identical series of stimuli. To separate the potential contributions of both types of “oddness” to the oddball effect, we used an auditory oddball paradigm in which one of two types of oddball could occur on each trial. The two types of oddball varied in their physical pitch difference from the standard tone and their likelihood of occurrence.

Participants heard nine-tone sequences consisting of identical (standard) tones and one embedded oddball tone, identified by a different pitch, which occurred in the fifth to the eighth serial position. Oddball tones were either near or far in pitch from the standard, and each oddball pitch was presented on either 75 % or 25 % of the trials (see Fig. 1). To determine whether any effects of oddball pitch distance were absolute or relative effects, we also compared conditions where the near- and far-pitch oddballs were both either higher (see Fig. 1a) or lower (see Fig. 1b) in pitch than the standard. If any pitch effect reflects the relative pitch distance of the oddball from the standard, rather than the absolute pitch height of the oddball, then near- and far-pitch oddballs that are both higher versus both lower in pitch than the standard should produce the same pattern of results.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram illustrating a single trial in the auditory oddball paradigm. Participants heard a sequence of nine tones that included an embedded oddball tone identified by a different pitch that occurred in the fifth, sixth, seventh, or eighth position. Oddball tones were presented either higher in pitch (a) or lower in pitch (b) than the standard tones

In Experiment 1, participants judged whether a variable-duration oddball was shorter or longer than the duration of the standard, while in Experiment 2, participants were asked simply to detect the oddball and respond as quickly as possible for conditions that matched Experiment 1. The motivation for the second experiment was to consider the possibility that the oddball effect might be due, at least in part, to how quickly individuals are able to initiate timing the oddball in response to its onset. Some suggestion that this may be true comes from the study by Pariyadath and Eagleman (2012), which showed that oddballs in later serial positions tend to be overestimated to a greater degree than oddballs in earlier serial positions. Critically, in this and almost all other oddball studies, the oddball position varied from trial to trial; thus, the later position oddballs afforded greater temporal preparation (i.e., they could be better anticipated). If the speed of detecting the oddball contributed to the degree of lengthening, more generally, then conditions yielding longer perceived oddball durations in Experiment 1 should yield faster oddball detection times in Experiment 2.

Experiment 1

Method

Participants

Seventy-two undergraduate students (50 female; 18–24 years, M = 19.1, SD = 1.3) from Michigan State University, with self-reported normal hearing, participated in the experiment in return for partial course credit.Footnote 1 Participants varied in number of years of formal music training (M = 3.9, SD = 3.8). An additional 14 individuals completed the experiment but were not included in the final sample, due to failure to attend to the task/follow instructions or exceptionally poor performance.Footnote 2

Design

The design of the experiment was a 2 (Pitch Direction: higher, lower) × 2 (Pitch Distance: far, near) × 2 (Pitch–Likelihood Pairing: far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %) × 4 (Oddball Serial Position: 5th, 6th, 7th, or 8th) × 9 (Oddball Duration: −20 %, −15 %, −10 %, −5 %, 0 %, +5 %, +10 %, +15 %, +20 %) mixed factorial. Oddball pitch distance, position, and duration were within-subjects variables; pitch direction and pitch−likelihood pairing were between-subjects variables. Separate groups of participants completed the oddball–higher (n = 39) and oddball–lower (n = 33) pitch direction conditions. Within each pitch direction condition, participants were randomly assigned either to the far–75 %/near–25 % pitch–likelihood pairing condition where the far-pitch oddball occurred on 75 % of trials and the near-pitch oddball occurred on 25 % of trials (oddball-higher, n = 20; oddball-lower, n = 18) or to the far–25 %/near–75 % pitch–likelihood pairing condition where the far-pitch oddball occurred on 25 % of the trials and the near-pitch oddball occurred on 75 % of the trials (oddball–higher, n = 19; oddball–lower, n = 15).

Stimuli and apparatus

Standard stimuli were 350-ms sine tones. In the oddball–higher condition, the standard was 400 Hz, and the near- and far-pitch oddballs were 550 Hz (standard + 150 Hz) and 700 Hz (standard + 300 Hz), respectively. In the oddball–lower condition, the standard was 700 Hz, and the near- and far-pitch oddballs were 550 Hz (standard − 150 Hz) and 400 Hz (standard − 300 Hz), respectively. The interonset interval between successive stimuli was fixed at 700 ms. Stimuli were generated using MATLAB software (The Mathworks, Inc.) and were presented at a comfortable listening level over Sennheiser HD-280 Pro headphones (Old Lyme, CT). Tone presentation and response collection were controlled by E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) running on a Dell Optiplex 760 series computer with Creative Sound Blaster Audigy soundcard installed. Participants made responses using a serial button response box.

Procedure

On each trial, participants heard an isochronous sequence of nine tones that consisted of eight 350-ms standard tones and one embedded oddball tone that varied in duration from trial to trial (Fig. 1). Participants judged whether the oddball duration was shorter or longer than the standard duration by pressing one of two labeled buttons on a response box. The variable-duration oddball was presented at either the near pitch or the far pitch in the fifth, sixth, seventh, or eighth sequence position. The duration of the oddball took on one of nine values that varied relative to the standard duration, T = 350 ms; these were −20 %, −15 %, −10 %, −5 %, 0 %, +5 %, +10 %, +15 %, and +20 %, corresponding to durations of 280, 297.5, 315, 332.5, 350, 367.5, 385, 402.5, and 420 ms, respectively. Far-pitch and near-pitch oddballs occurred in different proportions, depending on the assigned pitch–likelihood pairing condition: far–75 %/near–25 % versus far–25 %/near–75 %.

Experimental blocks were preceded by a 12-trial practice block consisting of 3 trials presenting the 25 % likely oddball (randomly selected from the possible durations) and 9 trials presenting the 75 % likely oddball (once at each oddball duration). Each experimental block consisted of 144 trials, with 36 presentations of the 25 % likely oddball (4 times per duration) randomly intermixed with 108 presentations of the 75 % likely oddball (12 times per duration). Oddball position varied randomly across trials. Participants in both the oddball–higher and oddball–lower pitch direction conditions completed three experimental blocks, for a total of 432 trials (108 trials with 25 % likely oddball and 324 trials with 75 % likely oddball). Participants were required to take a 1-min break every 36 trials and a 2-min break between each block. At the end of the experimental portion, a questionnaire about participant background demographics and any strategies used during the experiment was administered. The entire experiment took approximately 90 min.

Data analysis

Proportions of longer responses were determined for each of the nine oddball durations in each condition. Response proportions were then used to generate psychometric curves for each participant in each condition. Resulting curves were used to derive estimates of the point of subjective equality (PSE) and the just noticeable difference (JND) using the z-transform method with response proportions of 0 and 1 replaced by 1/(2n) and 1 − (1/2n), respectively, where n is the number of observations (Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). The PSE corresponds to the duration for which the participant responds longer 50 % of the time. The JND is half the stimulus-duration distance between the 25th and 75th percentiles of the psychometric curve and represents a discrimination threshold estimate. For the JND estimates, we randomly sampled observations in the 75 % likelihood condition so that the number of sampled observations matched the number of observations in the 25 % likelihood condition to eliminate known estimation biases associated with differences in the number of observations across conditions (e.g., Hautus, 1995). JNDs are reported herein as a percentage of standard duration, T = 350 ms. For the oddball–higher pitch direction condition, R 2 measures of goodness of fit for the estimated psychometric curves ranged between .83 and .93 across conditions. For the oddball–lower pitch direction condition, R 2 measures of goodness of fit for the estimated psychometric curves ranged between .79 and .93 across conditions. PSEs and JNDs were subjected to mixed-measures ANOVAs with pitch direction and pitch–likelihood pairing as between-subjects variables and pitch distance and position as within-subjects variables.

Results

Figure 2 shows proportions of longer responses as a function of oddball pitch distance and likelihood for the oddball–higher pitch direction condition (Panel a) and the oddball–lower pitch direction condition (Panel b). Corresponding estimates of PSE are shown in Fig. 3a and b, respectively. The 2 (Pitch Direction: higher, lower) × 2 (Pitch Distance: near, far) × 2 (Pitch–Likelihood Pairing: far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %) mixed-measures ANOVA on mean PSEs showed a significant main effect of oddball pitch distance, F(1, 68) = 26.7, p < .001, η p 2 = .28, and main effect of pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 68) = 7.7, p = .007, η p 2 = .10, but no main effect of pitch direction, F(1, 68) = 0.1, p = .8, η p 2 = .001, and no reliable two-way or three-way interactions (all ps > .2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean proportions of longer responses, with standard error bars, as a function of oddball duration for far-pitch oddballs (filled triangles) and near-pitch oddballs (open circles) in 25 % likelihood (dotted lines) and 75 % likelihood (solid lines) conditions for oddballs higher in pitch than the standard (a) and oddballs lower in pitch than the standard (b)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean estimates of point-of-subjective-equality for far-pitch oddballs and near-pitch oddballs in 25 % (shaded bars) and 75 % (solid bars) likelihood conditions for oddballs higher in pitch than the standard (a) and oddballs lower in pitch than the standard (b)

In general, psychometric curves for far-pitch oddballs were shifted to the left relative to near-pitch oddballs, and estimates of PSEs were shorter (corresponding to longer perceived durations) for far-pitch oddballs (M = 345.9 ms, SD = 22.5 ms) than for near-pitch oddballs (M = 370.8 ms, SD = 38.1 ms), t(71) = 5.18, p < .001, d = 0.61. Oddball likelihood also affected PSEs, but the effect depended on oddball pitch. Far-pitch oddballs that occurred 25 % of the time were perceived to be somewhat longer (M = 340.8 ms, SD = 20.6 ms) than far-pitch oddballs that occurred 75 % of the time (M = 350.5, SD = 23.3 ms), t(70) = 1.87, p = .06, d = 0.44. In contrast, near-pitch oddballs that occurred 25 % of the time were perceived to be shorter (M = 380.3 ms, SD = 48.6) than near-pitch oddballs that occurred 75 % of the time (M = 360.2, SD = 16.1), t(70) = 2.30, p = .024, d = 0.54.

Table 1 shows mean relative JNDs as a function of oddball pitch distance and likelihood for oddball–higher (left columns) and oddball–lower (right columns) pitch direction conditions. The 2 (Pitch Direction: higher, lower) × 2 (Pitch Distance: near, far) × 2 (Pitch–Likelihood Pairing: far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %) mixed-measures ANOVA on JNDs showed a significant main effect of oddball pitch distance, F(1, 68) = 4.98, p = .03, η p 2 = .07, but no main effect of pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 68) = 1.8, p = .18, η p 2 = .03, no main effect of pitch direction, F(1, 68) = 0.1, p = .8, η p 2 = .001, and no reliable two-way or three-way interactions (all ps > .2). Far-pitch oddballs yielded lower discrimination thresholds (M = 9.6 %, SD = 3.6 %) than did near-pitch oddballs (M = 10.7 %, SD = 5.1 %), t(71) = 2.23, p = .029, d = 0.26.

Table 1 Mean just-noticeable-difference as a function of oddball pitch distance and likelihood for Experiment 1 (with standard deviations in parentheses)

Finally, we considered an effect of oddball position on PSEs and JNDs. Table 2 shows mean PSEs and JNDs as a function of position for oddball–higher (left columns) and oddball–lower (right columns) pitch direction conditions. For this analysis, it was necessary to collapse across near- and far-pitch oddballs to obtain enough observations per sequence position to reliably estimate PSEs and JNDs for each subject. A 2 (Pitch Direction: higher, lower) × 4 (Position: 5th, 6th, 7th, 8th) × 2 (Pitch–Likelihood Pairing: far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %) mixed-measures ANOVA on PSEs revealed a significant main effect of position, F(3, 204) = 24.03, p < .001, η p 2 = .26, but no main effect of pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 68) = 0.1, p = .75, η p 2 = .002, no main effect of pitch direction, F(1, 68) = 1.3, p = .26, η p 2 = .02, and no reliable two-way or three-way interactions (all ps > .6). For the ANOVA on JNDs, there was a main effect of position, F(3, 204) = 5.33, p = .003, η p 2 = .07, but no main effect of pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 68) = 0.97, p = .33, η p 2 = .01, no main effect of pitch direction, F(1, 68) = 0.35, p = .56, η p 2 < .01, and no reliable two-way or three-way interactions (all ps > .4). Oddballs that occurred in later sequence positions were perceived to be longer (i.e., they had shorter PSEs) than oddballs that occurred in earlier sequence positions. In addition, oddballs that occurred in later positions resulted in better discrimination (smaller JNDs) than did oddballs that occurred in earlier positions.

Table 2 Mean point-of-subjective-equality (PSE) and just-noticeable-difference (JND) as a function of sequence position for Experiment 1 for oddball–higher (left columns) and oddball–lower (right columns) pitch directions (with standard deviations in parentheses)

Discussion

Results from Experiment 1 showed that far-pitch oddballs were perceived to be longer than near-pitch oddballs, with a larger difference in low-likelihood conditions. Far-pitch oddballs that occurred on 25 % of trials were perceived to be longer than far-pitch oddballs that occurred on 75 % of trials, while near-pitch oddballs that occurred on 25 % of trials were perceived to be shorter than near-pitch oddballs that occurred on 75 % of trials. The same general pattern was observed for oddballs that were higher and lower in pitch than the standard. There was an additional effect of position on PSEs and JNDs; PSEs tended to shorten (perceived durations lengthened) and JNDs tended to be reduced for oddballs in later positions.

It is unlikely that the effect of oddball pitch we observed is an absolute pitch height effect, because we observed the same pattern of results for oddballs that were higher and lower in pitch than the standard. Thus, the results from Experiment 1 suggest that the far versus near difference we observed is an effect of being relatively near or far in pitch from the standard tone, regardless of whether the oddball is higher or lower than the standard. Moreover, it is unlikely that the pitch effect we observed could be due to a difference in the perceived loudness of the oddball, associated with its change in frequency. Note here that the oddball–higher and oddball–lower pitch direction conditions swapped the pitches of the standard tone and the far oddball tone, so if the pitch effect was due to a difference in loudness, we should have not observed the same pattern in the higher and lower pitch direction conditions.

The observed effects of oddball pitch distance (near vs. far) potentially can be explained in terms of either a repetition suppression account or an attention-based account, since both can be interpreted to predict that far-pitch oddballs should produce longer perceived durations than near-pitch oddballs. The observed interaction between pitch and likelihood, however, is harder to interpret from either perspective, since both repetition suppression and attention-based accounts arguably would predict a priori that less likely oddballs would be perceived to be longer than more likely oddballs regardless of pitch, which is not what we observed. Rather, low likelihood enhanced the oddball effect for the far-pitch oddball and reduced the oddball effect for near-pitch oddballs. Moreover, we observed a pronounced shortening of perceived oddball duration in some conditions, rather than the lengthening predicted by repetition suppression or enhanced-attention accounts.

The strongest support for the repetition suppression account is the observed effect of position on PSEs—namely, that PSEs shorten (perceived durations lengthen) for oddballs in later positions. From a repetition suppression perspective, increasing the number of standard stimuli prior to an oddball stimulus should produce greater repetition suppression, thereby increasingly shortening the represented duration of the standard and, consequently, resulting in shorter PSEs for judged oddball durations. One alternative possible explanation of the position effect is that oddballs in later positions are simply more temporally predictable, thus enabling faster initiation of a timing process, akin to a variable foreperiod effect on reaction times (RTs; Grondin & Rammsayer, 2003; Mo & George, 1977; Niemi & Näätänen, 1981).

More broadly, this raises the question of whether the pitch effect and the interaction with likelihood may also be related to how quickly individuals are able to initiate timing the oddball. To consider this possibility and the potential role of temporal preparation in the position effect, we conducted a second experiment that used the same design and general procedure as Experiment 1, but rather than having participants judge the duration of the oddball relative to the standard, we had participants simply detect the oddball as quickly as possible. On the basis of the hypothesis that more salient and/or temporally predictable oddballs enable individuals to start timing the oddball sooner, we predicted that RT data from Experiment 2 should parallel the PSE data from Experiment 1, such that conditions that yielded shorter PSEs (longer perceived durations) in Experiment 1 should result in shorter detection times in Experiment 2.

Experiment 2

Method

Participants and design

Forty-eight undergraduate students (32 female; 18–25 years, M = 19.4, SD = 1.6) from Michigan State University, with self-reported normal hearing participated in the experiment in return for partial course credit (see note 1). Participants varied in number of years of formal music training (M = 2.7, SD = 4.1). The design parallels Experiment 1; only the task differed. Pitch direction (higher, lower) was crossed with pitch–likelihood pairing (far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %). In the oddball–higher condition, approximately half of the participants were assigned either to the far–75 %/near–25 % condition (n = 12) or to the far–25 %/near–75 % condition (n = 13), where both the near- and far-pitch oddballs were higher in pitch than the standard. In the oddball–lower condition, remaining participants were assigned either to the far–75 %/near–25 % condition (n = 12) or to the far–25 %/near–75 % condition (n = 11) where both the near- and far-pitch oddballs were lower in pitch than the standard. Participants’ task was simply to listen to each sequence and detect the oddball as quickly as possible by pushing a single button on the response box.

Stimuli and apparatus/materials

The stimuli, apparatus, and materials were identical to those in Experiment 1.

Procedure

Participants were instructed to use the index finger of their dominant hand to press the middle button on the response box as soon as they heard the oddball tone amid the ongoing sequence. They received no feedback (i.e., the sequence of tones continued uninterrupted with no beep or other indication of their response being made). At the end of each trial, a fixation cross appeared briefly on the screen, and then the next trial began. All remaining aspects of the procedure were identical to those in Experiment 1. Following the experimental blocks, a questionnaire about participant background demographics and strategies used during the experiment was administered. The entire study took approximately 90 min.

Data analysis

Data were first trimmed to eliminate any responses that occurred prior to the onset of the oddball (assumed to be accidental/false responses) or that were exceedingly long.Footnote 3 These responses accounted for fewer than 2 % (401 out of 20,633) and fewer than 1 % (20 out of 20,633) of total responses, respectively. Similar percentages of responses were trimmed in the 75 % and 25 % likelihood conditions for both near- and far-pitch oddballs in the oddball–higher and oddball–lower pitch direction conditions.

Results

Figure 4 shows mean RTs as a function of oddball pitch and likelihood for the oddball–higher condition (Panel a) and the oddball–lower condition (Panel b). RTs separated by position, pitch distance, and likelihood for the higher and lower conditions are reported in Table 3. The pattern of RTs in Experiment 2 generally paralleled the pattern of PSEs in Experiment 1. The omnibus 2 (Pitch Direction: higher, lower) × 2 (Pitch Distance: near, far) × 4 (Position: 5th, 6th, 7th, 8th) × 2 (Pitch–Likelihood Pairing: far–75 %/near–25 %, far–25 %/near–75 %) mixed-measures ANOVA on mean RTs revealed a main effect of pitch distance, F(1, 44) = 60.92, p < .001, η p 2 = .58, a main effect of position, F(3, 132) = 104.54, p < .001, η p 2 = .70, but no main effect of pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 44) = 2.56, p = 0.117, η p 2 = .06. There was, however, a marginal interaction of pitch distance and pitch–likelihood pairing, F(1, 44) = 3.75, p = .059, η p 2 = .08. There was also a main effect of pitch direction, F(1, 44) = 6.82, p = .012, η p 2 = .13, an interaction between pitch direction and pitch distance, F(1, 44) = 17.05, p < .001, η p 2 = .28, and a marginal interaction of pitch direction and position, F(3, 132) = 2.86, p = .06, η p 2 = .06. There was a four-way interaction of pitch distance, position, pitch direction, and pitch–likelihood pairing, F(3, 132) = 3.41, p = .037, η p 2 = .07. There were no other reliable two-way or three-way interactions (all ps > .15).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean reaction times (RTs; with standard error bars) from Experiment 2 for far-pitch oddballs and near-pitch oddballs in 25 % (shaded bars) and 75 % (solid bars) likelihood conditions for oddballs higher in pitch than the standard (a) and oddballs lower in pitch than the standard (b)

Table 3 Mean reaction times as a function of oddball sequence position, oddball pitch distance, and likelihood from Experiment 2 for oddball–higher (left columns) and oddball–lower (right columns) pitch directions (with standard deviations in parentheses)

Paralleling the pattern of results in Experiment 1, RTs were faster for far-pitch oddballs (M = 275.9 ms, SD = 44.1 ms) than for near-pitch oddballs (M = 299.8 ms, SD = 48.1 ms), t(47) = 6.71, p < .001, d = 0.97. Likelihood appeared to enhance the effects of pitch in the same manner as in Experiment 1, but the pattern was less reliable. As was predicted, far-pitch oddballs that were 25 % likely were responded to faster than far-pitch oddballs that were 75 % likely (far–25 %, M = 268.7 ms, SD = 44.2 ms; far–75 %, M = 283.1 ms, SD = 43.7 ms), but not significantly so, t(46) = 1.14, p = .13, d = 0.33, one-tailed. Also as was predicted, near-pitch oddballs that were 25 % likely were responded to slower than near-pitch oddballs that were 75 % likely (near–25 %, M = 312.3 ms, SD = 53.1; near–75 %, M = 287.4 ms, SD = 40.0), t(46) = 1.83, p = .04, d = 0.53, one-tailed. In contrast to the pattern of results in Experiment 1, RTs were faster in the oddball-higher (M = 271.3 ms, SD = 40.6 ms) than in the oddball-lower (M = 303.8 ms, SD = 44.0 ms) pitch direction condition, t(46) = 2.66, p = .011, d = 0.80, two-tailed. Paralleling the position effect for PSEs observed in Experiment 1, oddballs that were presented in later sequence positions were responded to more quickly than oddballs presented in earlier sequence positions.

Finally, we examined the extent to which oddball detection times (RTs) in Experiment 2 predicted perceived duration (PSEs) in Experiment 1 for each of the 32 combinations of oddball pitch direction (higher, lower), pitch distance (near, far), likelihood (25 % and 75 %), and position (5th, 6th, 7th, or 8th). Results, shown in Fig. 5, reveal a significant positive relationship between RTs and PSEs, r(30) = .55, p = .001.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Scatterplot showing the relationship between reaction times (RTs) in Experiment 2 and mean estimates of point-of-subjective-equality in Experiment 1 for the 32 combinations of oddball position (5th, 6th, 7th, 8th), pitch distance (near, far), likelihood (25 %, 75 %), and pitch direction (higher, lower)

Discussion

Response times in the oddball detection task varied as a function of oddball pitch distance, likelihood, and position in a manner that generally paralleled the pattern of PSEs in Experiment 1. Consistent with the possibility that the effect of position on PSEs observed in Experiment 1 reflects a form of temporal preparation akin to a variable foreperiod effect, oddballs that occurred in later serial positions were responded to more quickly than oddballs that occurred in earlier serial positions. Moreover, far-pitch oddballs were detected faster than near-pitch oddballs, with a greater effect of pitch when the oddballs were rare, but less reliably so than in Experiment 1. One reason for the somewhat weaker effects for oddball detection times in Experiment 2 may be due to insufficient power. Experiment 2 also differed from Experiment 1 in that, in Experiment 2, we observed an effect of pitch direction on RTs, which was not observed for PSEs in Experiment 1. The reason for this difference is, at present, not clear. Nonetheless, the results of the two experiments taken together provide initial support for the view that systematic distortions in the perceived duration of auditory oddballs may be at least partly determined by differences in how quickly individuals are able to initiate timing the oddball following its onset.

General discussion

Five main findings emerged from the present investigation. First, in Experiment 1, far-pitch oddballs were perceived to be longer than near-pitch oddballs, regardless of whether the near- and far-pitch oddballs were higher in pitch or lower in pitch than the standard. Observing the same pitch distance effect regardless of pitch direction is important because it argues against the possibility that the pitch effect we observed is either an effect of absolute pitch height or associated with differences in perceived loudness. Second, likelihood was found to enhance the pitch effect whereby far-pitch oddballs that occurred 25 % of the time were perceived to be longer than far-pitch oddballs that occurred 75 % of the time, while the reverse was true for near-pitch oddballs. Third, oddballs that occurred in later sequence positions were perceived to be longer than oddballs that occurred in earlier sequence positions. Fourth, the pattern of RTs observed for the oddball detection task in Experiment 2 generally paralleled the pattern of PSEs observed in Experiment 1; moreover, how quickly participants were able to detect the oddball in Experiment 2 predicted PSEs obtained in Experiment 1. Finally, discrimination thresholds were slightly lower for far-pitch than for near-pitch oddballs and lower for oddballs in later serial positions than in earlier serial positions. In the remainder of the discussion, we consider the implication of the present findings for different theoretical accounts.

Tse et al. (2004) proposed that deviant “oddball” stimuli receive enhanced attention and, thereby, should be perceived to be longer in duration than they would otherwise, on the basis of the assumption that the amount of attention increases the effective accumulation of temporal information. In the context of the present experiments, the enhanced-attention account proposed by Tse et al. would explain the pitch effect we observed but would have difficulty accounting for the mediating effect of likelihood. Overall, the enhanced-attention account can be interpreted to predict that the more unexpected an oddball stimulus is, the more attention it should attract. Thus, 25 % likely oddballs should be perceived to be longer than 75 % likely oddballs, which is consistent with our finding for far-pitch oddballs. However, inconsistent with the enhanced-attention account, the reverse was observed for near-pitch oddballs, which were perceived to last longer when presented on 75 % of the trials than when presented on 25 % of the trials.

Overall, these findings can be at least partly accounted for by the repetition suppression explanation of the oddball effect (Pariyadath & Eagleman, 2007, 2012; Schindel et al., 2011). On this view, the temporal expansion of the deviant oddball event is a by-product of reduced neural activity to the repeated or more generally predictable standard stimulus. Thus, the oddball effect is the result of perceived shortening of the standard duration, rather than perceived lengthening of the oddball. Both the position effect and pitch effect are consistent with this view. Repetition suppression accounts for the position effect because more repetitions of the standard occur for oddballs that are in later sequence positions. Here, we also observed a reliable effect of position on temporal discrimination thresholds, but see Matthews (2011) for a null effect of repetition on temporal discrimination thresholds.

Repetition suppression can account for the pitch effect because, at least in some neural models of repetition suppression, the magnitude of the oddball effect would also be expected to scale with degree of physical stimulus difference: The more dissimilar the oddball is in pitch from the standard, the greater the neural activity in response to the oddball (see Grill-Spector, Henson, & Martin, 2006, for a general review of repetition suppression). Like the enhanced-attention account, however, the repetition suppression account would have some difficulty accounting for the mediating effect of likelihood. In general, the rarer (25 % likely) oddball should have been perceived to be longer than the more frequent (75 % likely) oddball, regardless of pitch, which is not what we observed.

Some potential insight about the opposite effects of likelihood in near and far pitch conditions may be gleaned from the work of Jones and colleagues on dynamic attending theory and the development of expectations about pitch and time structure in auditory sequences (Jones, 1976; Jones, Moynihan, MacKenzie, & Puente, 2002; Large & Jones, 1999; McAuley & Jones, 2003). Most relevant for this present investigation is Jones, Johnston, and Puente (2006), who showed that the pitch and time structure of a context tone sequence produces expectations in the listener that guide their attentional focus for upcoming target tones. Specifically, pitch discrimination of a target tone was found to be better when the target tone (1) was presented within the expected pitch range of the auditory context and (2) occurred at an expected time point based on an extrapolation of the context rhythm. Applied to the present finding of a pitch-dependent effect of likelihood (in the 25 % condition, pitch effects were greater), one possibility is that successive trials establish the context for the development of pitch expectancies, such that when an oddball occurs with a 75 % probability, the pitch of that oddball, together with the repeated standard tone, establishes an expected oddball pitch. Thus, when the near-pitch oddball occurs on 75 % of the trials, the unlikely far-pitch oddball is a more deviant oddball and, thus, may be more detectable. Conversely, when the far-pitch oddball occurs on 75 % of trials, the unlikely near-pitch oddball is a less deviant oddball and, thus, may be less detectable. RT data bear out this asymmetric pattern. This perspective refines the concept of oddball saliency to mean the degree to which a to-be-judged stimulus fits/misfits with the pitch expectancy for an oddball, which is dynamically determined by ongoing context.

Although the finding of longer perceived durations for later position oddballs provides support for a repetition suppression account, the present study has provided some initial evidence that the effect of position could also be due to the increase in temporal preparation that is possible for later position oddballs. As the elements of the sequence unfold, participants can be more and more certain that an oddball will occur; that is, they can develop a temporal expectation for the occurrence of the oddball stimulus. Shorter observed RTs in the detection task (Experiment 2) for later position oddballs are consistent with this interpretation, which are also reminiscent of a variable foreperiod effect on RTs (Klemmer, 1956; Niemi & Näätänen, 1981).

Some converging support for a temporal preparation interpretation of the position effect comes from several studies (Grondin & Rammsayer, 2003; Mo & George, 1977). Grondin and Rammsayer found longer perceived duration of intervals presented after longer foreperiods when the duration of the foreperiod interval was variable from trial to trial. This is analogous in the present investigation to having the sequential position of the oddball stimulus randomly vary from trial to trial. Oddballs in later sequential positions have a longer foreperiod than do oddballs in earlier sequential positions. Mo and George also found that foreperiod influenced temporal reproductions and RTs to a visual stimulus equivalently. Specifically, longer foreperiods (when the length of the foreperiod was unpredictable) resulted in both longer temporal reproductions and shorter RTs.

More broadly, the consistent relationship between the RTs in Experiment 2 and PSEs in Experiment 1 as a function of oddball pitch distance, likelihood, and position suggests a common explanatory source of the observed temporal distortions—namely, in how quickly individuals are able to initiate (start) timing in response to the oddball onset. A number of predictions follow from this hypothesis. Generally speaking, conditions that influence RTs in the detection of an event (as in the foreperiod literature discussed above) should also affect perceived duration in a similar manner. For example, previous studies examining effects of foreperiods on RTs have shown that increasing either occurrence uncertainty or temporal uncertainty of a target stimulus tends to produce longer RTs for detecting the target (Drazin, 1961; Gordon, 1967; Klemmer, 1956; see Niemi & Näätänen, 1981, for a review of foreperiod effects on RTs). Considered in the context of a temporal preparation account of the effect of position on perceived duration reported here and by Pariyadath and Eagleman (2012), including “catch” trials on which no oddball occurs on some proportion of trials should (1) produce longer PSEs (an overall shortening of perceived durations) and (2) reduce, or even eliminate, the effect of position.

One final issue that deserves some comment concerns the large discrepancies observed in the overall magnitude of the oddball effect across studies. The magnitude of the oddball effect we observed in Experiment 1 (i.e., 5 % overestimation to 7 % underestimation) was much smaller than in previous studies and in some cases (e.g., near-pitch oddballs), we observed shortening rather than lengthening. Notably, Tse et al. (2004) found that overestimation of the oddball duration was as large as 60 % but also centered the oddball duration in the direction of oddball overestimation (i.e., participants judged durations that were close to the PSE). Most studies, however, have not revealed the same magnitude of distortions in perceived duration as that observed in Tse et al. Pariyadath and Eagleman (2007) found oddball overestimation of only about 12 % in a visual oddball paradigm. In a cross-modal oddball paradigm with temporally predictable oddballs (i.e., oddballs presented in the same position every trial), van Wassenhove et al. (2008) found oddball overestimation of roughly 8 % for expanding disk and increasing pitch oddballs. Schindel et al. (2011) also found approximately 10 % overestimation of a visual oddball, relative to standards.

Seifried and Ulrich (2010) recently provided evidence in a visual oddball experiment that estimates of PSE are strongly biased by the distribution of to-be-judged oddball durations, thus highlighting the context sensitivity of the observed effects. They reported that if the distribution of oddball durations is centered at a value that is less than the standard duration (i.e., in the direction of the oddball effect), then estimated PSEs will shift toward the mean oddball duration; this yields greater overestimation (i.e., a larger oddball effect) than when oddball durations are centered at a value that is equal to the standard duration. In an additional study (not reported here), we replicated the Seifried and Ulrich findings for the auditory paradigm used in the present set of experiments and also showed that shortening (rather than lengthening) is found when oddball durations are centered on a value that is longer than the standard duration. Thus, these supplementary findings extend the results of Seifried and Ulrich both to the auditory modality and to conditions that consider whether the oddball effect can be reversed (i.e., reveal underestimation) when the distribution of oddball durations is shifted to a value that is longer than the standard.

Conclusions

Two experiments have shown that for an auditory oddball paradigm, oddball pitch distance, likelihood, and position all influence the perceived duration of the deviant oddball stimulus. Oddballs that are farther in pitch from the standard and in later sequence positions are perceived to be longer than oddballs that are closer in pitch to the standard or in earlier sequence positions, with a larger difference between near- and far-pitch oddballs in low-likelihood conditions. The same pattern of duration distortions is observed regardless of whether the oddballs were higher or lower in pitch than the standard, ruling out the possibility that the pitch effect is one of absolute pitch height or attributable to differences in perceived loudness. The observed interaction between the pitch effect and oddball likelihood provides little direct support for either a repetition suppression account or an enhanced-attention account. Moreover, the positive relationship between the time required to detect an oddball stimulus and PSEs across all conditions provides some initial evidence that the oddball effect is at least partly driven by the latency to initiate timing following oddball onset. This interpretation is consistent with a temporal preparation account of the position effect. Thus, oddball stimuli that are more perceptually salient or more temporally expected are perceived to be longer in duration because they are more quickly detected.