One intriguing finding in eye movement research is that word identification processes begin before the eyes fixate a word (Dodge, 1907; Rayner, 1975; Schotter, Angele, & Rayner, 2012). Readers process the word they are fixating and, at some point during that fixation, begin processing the upcoming word to its right. Thus, word recognition during silent reading typically begins when a word appears in parafoveal view to the right of where the eye is fixated. One surprising aspect of parafoveal processing is that skilled readers activate complex linguistic information such as phonological structure, in addition to processing low-level visual information (Ashby & Martin, 2008; Ashby & Rayner, 2004). Parafoveal processes occur automatically during skilled silent reading, and readers appear to be unaware of the parafoveal information they process (Henderson, Dixon, Petersen, Twilley, & Ferreira, 1995; Lesch & Pollatsek, 1998; Pollatsek, Lesch, Morris, & Rayner, 1992; Schotter et al., 2012). Most importantly, parafoveal information enables skilled readers to process text 20–40% faster than when parafoveal information is not available (Rayner, 1998, 2009; Rayner, Liversedge, & White, 2006). Parafoveal information contributes to faster reading speed through a reduction in both the duration and number of fixations that occur during skilled silent reading (Rayner, 2009; Fitzsimmons & Drieghe, 2011).

Although there is substantial research literature examining parafoveal processing during silent reading (see Rayner, 1998, 2009; Schotter et al., 2012), little is known about the role of these processes in oral reading. In this article, we present an initial investigation into the role of parafoveal information in oral reading as compared with silent reading. Given that masking parafoveal information slows down silent reading, we asked whether the availability of parafoveal information affects oral reading in a similar fashion. To do this, we monitored eye movements as participants read sentences silently and orally in conditions in which parafoveal information was and was not available. Beyond contributing to our understanding of oral reading processes, these data provide information about the nature of parafoveal processing by examining whether parafoveal effects are vulnerable to task demands and how reading systems uptake available parafoveal information.

Oral reading provides an interesting test of parafoveal processing, since there is little strategic benefit to speeding the rate at which the eyes move when reading aloud. This is partly because of the fact that word pronunciation involves muscles in the speech tract, which operate more slowly than do cognitive processes. As a result of this timing difference, readers typically look at and process a word in the text that is two to three words to the right of the one they are pronouncing (Inhoff, Solomon, Radach, & Seymour, 2011). Given that the eyes are ahead of the voice in oral reading, readers may only need to process the fixated word, and words to the right of that might be irrelevant.

Classic research suggests a fairly straightforward relationship between silent and oral modes of reading, indicating that oral reading processes may be essentially the same as silent reading processes, with the addition of articulatory demands (Huey, 1908/1968). The few eye movement studies that have been conducted to examine oral and silent reading indicate that eye movements in both reading modes are tightly correlated within participants (Anderson & Swanson, 1937; Søvik, Arntzen, & Samuelstuen, 2000).

To investigate parafoveal processes in oral reading as compared with silent reading, we manipulated the parafoveal information available to readers as they read orally and silently by changing the size of a gaze-contingent text window (see Fig. 1) in a moving window paradigm (McConkie & Rayner, 1975; Rayner & Bertera, 1979; Rayner, Slattery, & Bélanger, 2010). In the one-word window (1 W) condition, participants saw the fixated foveal word only. The three-word window (3 W) allowed readers to obtain parafoveal information; the fixated word and the two words to its right were available in the windowFootnote 1. In both conditions, the window moved with each saccade as the eyes advanced through the sentence. As was outlined above, we anticipated three possible outcomes. First, since the eyes are ahead of the voice in oral reading, perhaps only the fixated word is necessary to maintain reading speed, and the 1 W and 3 W conditions should not differ. Second, since parafoveal processing occurs automatically during skilled reading, restricting parafoveal information should slow reading down to a comparable extent in both reading tasks. Third, reading might be faster with the 3 W window than with the 1 W window, but the amount of preview benefit might be greater in silent reading than in oral reading. Our data were largely consistent with the third prediction.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The moving window paradigm. The asterisk (*) indicates the fixation point

Method

Participants

Twenty-four undergraduates at the University of Massachusetts received either course credit or $7 to participate in the experiment. They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive concerning the experiment’s purpose.

Apparatus

Materials were presented on a 22-in. monitor attached to a Pentium 166 MHZ computer interfaced with an SR Research EyeLink 1000 eye tracking system with high spatial resolution and a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. Participants read sentences binocularly, but only the right eye was recorded. Viewing distance was approximately 60 cm (3.8 letters = 1° visual angle). Custom designed software ensured that the display change occurred within 6–12 ms; the window appeared to the participants to move in synchrony with their eye movements.

Materials

Participants read 80 sentences from Rayner, Castelhano, and Yang (2009) presented in Courier, a monospace font. On average, these sentences contained 11.2 words and were 63.5 letters long (including interword spaces). Average word length was 4.8 letters. Average window size (including interword spaces) was 5.8 and 15.5 letter spaces in the 1 W window and 3 W window, respectively. Twenty sentences were followed by a yes/no comprehension question, which participants answered with a button press (mean accuracy = 94% with no difference across conditions).

Design and procedure

Each participant read a block of 40 sentences silently and a block of another 40 sentences aloud in this within-participants, fully counterbalanced design. Sentences with 1W and 3W windows were randomly interleaved in both blocks. Every participant read one quarter of the sentences in each condition in this 2 (1 W or 3 W) × 2 (silent or oral) design. Across participants, every sentence appeared in all four reading conditions. To create the window conditions, letters outside the viewing window were replaced by an x mask for every word in order to preserve low-level parafoveal information about the interword spaces. Example sentences appear in Fig. 1.

Initially, participants were calibrated by fixating on three points randomly presented across the horizontal midline of the screen. At the start of each trial, a square (15 pixels wide) appeared on the left side of the screen, with its left edge aligned with the first letter. The sentence replaced this square on the screen once a stable fixation was detected. Participants were instructed to read for comprehension (either silently or orally) and to press a keypad button when finished. Presentation order within each block (silent or oral) was randomized for each participant, and the order of blocks was counterbalanced across participants. Following the eight practice sentences, participants read 80 experimental sentences (20 in each of the four experimental conditions).

Results

Fixations shorter than 80 ms and longer than 800 ms were eliminated (.9% of fixations). Trials in which there was a blink or track loss during reading were removed prior to analysis (10% of trials), as were trials in which the mean eye movement measure fell 2.5 SD beyond the participants’ mean for a given window size (2.6% of trials). Trial exclusion affected all conditions similarly (F < 1).

To be consistent with previous moving window experiments, we first report reading rate, or words read per minute (wpm), which was calculated as the sum of fixation times from the beginning of the first fixation to the end of the last fixation in a sentence. Because reading rate is a composite of fixation durations and number of fixations, we also report average fixation duration, average number of fixations per sentence, and the average length of forward saccades. We chose these measures in order to identify which eye movement behaviors contributed to differences in reading rate. In addition, the probability of first fixation and the probability of refixation were examined in order to determine whether an effect found in number fixations was due to these measuresFootnote 2.

In all eye movement measures, we found the expected main effects of reading mode and window size (all ps < .001) reported in previous articles (see Tinker, 1936; Rayner, 2009), and the F values for these appear in Table 1 Footnote 3. In addition to main effects, Table 1 presents tests of interactions between reading mode and window size that suggest that the availability of parafoveal information had larger effects in silent versus oral reading. Therefore, we describe the follow-up t tests in the following text.

Table 1 Results of significance tests for main effects of reading mode and window size as well as the interactions. Tests that were nonsignificant at an alpha of .05 are noted by an asterisk (*)

Reading rate

As Table 2 illustrates, having parafoveal information available to the right of fixation increased reading speed more in the silent than in the oral reading conditions. However, paired t tests confirmed a significant preview effect in both silent reading, t 1 (23) = 8.10, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 8.80, p < .001, and oral reading, t 1 (23) = 5.18, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 4.93, p < .001.

Table 2 Participant means and standard errors for the one word (1 W) and three word (3 W) windows in the silent and oral reading conditions

Following Rayner et al. (2010), we did a median split of the data according to participants’ silent reading rates in the 3 W condition, yielding a fast group (mean wpm = 328 wpm) and a slow group (mean = 228 wpm). In addition to finding that the pattern of effects noted above held for each group, statistical tests confirmed a three-way interaction, F 1(1, 22) = 18.38, p < .001; F 2(1, 61) = 20.91, p < .001, driven by stronger preview benefits in silent reading for the faster readers than the slower readers (see Fig. 2). In addition, follow-up tests of window size were significant for both groups in both conditions: faster readers during silent reading, t 1 (11) = 7.34, p < .001; t 2 (78) = 9.73, p < .001, and oral reading, t 1 (11) = 2.59, p < .05; t 2 (77) = 2.11, p < .05; slower readers during silent reading, t 1 (11) = 9.60, p < .001; t 2 (73) = 8.18, p < .001, and oral reading, t 1 (11) = 5.05, p < .001; t 2 (69) = 3.43, p < .001.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Words read per minute (WPM) by fast and slow readers during silent reading (one word window [1 W] and three word window [3 W]) and oral reading (1 W and 3 W)

Fixation duration

As seen in Table 2, the average fixation duration decreased when parafoveal information was available, as compared with when it was not, in both the silent and oral reading conditions (a difference of 30 ms and 18 ms, respectively). This effect was significantly larger in silent than in oral reading (Table 1), but follow-up paired t tests confirmed a significant preview effect in both silent reading, t 1 (23) = 9.60, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 11.69, p < .001, and oral reading, t 1 (23) = 6.14, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 7.55, p < .001. Table 3 displays the means for the fast and slow readers; there was no three-way interaction.

Table 3 Participant means for the faster readers and slower readers

Number of fixations

On average, readers made fewer fixations per sentence when parafoveal information was available than when it was not (see Table 2). This effect was significantly larger in silent versus oral reading (Table1); paired t tests confirmed a significant preview effect in silent reading, t 1 (23) = 6.41, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 5.81, p < .001, whereas the effect in oral reading was barely marginal by participants, t 1 (23) = 1.73, p = .098, and was significant by items, t 2 (79) = 2.34, p < .05.

Statistical tests of data from faster and slower readers, as defined above, confirmed a three-way interaction, F 1(1, 22) = 7.16, p < .05, F 2(1, 61) = 6.24, p < .05, driven by a larger decrease in the number of fixations made by fast readers when parafoveal information was available, as compared with when it was not. Follow-up tests of parafoveal preview effects were significant for both groups only in the silent reading condition: faster readers, t 1 (11) = 5.29, p < .001; t 2 (78) = 7.19, p < .001, and slower readers, t 1 (11) = 5.16, p < .001; t 2 (73) = 3.65, p < .001. In contrast, preview availability did not significantly affect the number of fixations made during oral reading by faster readers, ts < 1, or slower readers, t 1 (11) = 1.53, p = .16, t 2 (69) = 1.14, p > .2.

Probability of first-pass fixation

On average, readers were more likely to fixate a word when parafoveal information for that word was unavailable (see Table 2). This effect was significantly larger in silent versus oral reading (Table 1); however, paired t tests confirmed a significant preview effect in silent reading, t 1 (23) = 9. 70, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 9.12, p < .001, and in oral reading, t 1 (23) = 6.68, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 8.73, p < .001. Table 3 presents the means by skill group. Paired t tests confirmed parafoveal preview effects for the faster readers in silent reading, t 1 (11) = 6.50, p < .001; t 2 (78) = 6.63, p < .001, and in oral reading, t 1 (11) = 6.11, p < .001; t 2 (77) = 6.15, p < .001. Slower readers also showed a preview effect in silent reading, t 1 (11) = 6.98, p < .001; t 2 (73) = 9.53, p < .001, and in oral reading, t 1 (11) = 3.67, p < .01; t 2 (69) = 6.10, p < .001, with no three-way interaction.

Probability of refixation

On average, readers were more likely to refixate a word when parafoveal information for that word was unavailable (see Table 2). A nonsignificant interaction failed to indicate any difference in effect size for silent versus oral reading, and there was no interaction with reading skill (Fs < 1).

Forward saccade length

As seen in Table 2, forward saccades during silent reading were one character shorter on average when parafoveal letter information was unavailable than when it was available. During oral reading, forward saccade length was .71 characters shorter when parafoveal letter information was unavailable. The interaction was significant by participants, but not by items (p = .17). Follow-up paired t tests confirmed significant preview effects in silent reading, t 1 (23) = 7.96, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 6.81, p < .001, and in oral reading, t 1 (23) = 6.63, p < .001; t 2 (79) = 7.99, p < .001. A similar pattern held for the fast and slow readers, and there was no significant three-way interaction (Fs < 1).

Results summary

We found significant main effects of window size and reading mode across all measures, which suggests that parafoveal information plays a role in eye movement control during oral reading as well as silent reading. Significant interactions between reading mode and window size indicated that the effects of parafoveal information availability were larger in silent reading than in oral reading for most measures. The availability of parafoveal information did affect reading rate, fixation duration, and probability of first fixation in oral reading, but less so than in silent reading. Analyses with reading skill as a factor suggested that parafoveal information availability improved reading rate for faster readers during silent reading more than any other condition, and that this benefit for the fast readers was derived mainly from a reduced number of fixations.

Discussion

In this experiment, we examined parafoveal processing during silent and oral reading. Participants read sentences aloud and silently in conditions in which parafoveal information was available (3 W) and in which it was not (1 W). Parafoveal information facilitated reading speed in both reading modes; text was read faster during silent and oral reading when parafoveal information was available than when it was not. In this respect, oral reading processes seem to parallel silent reading processes, as was suggested by previous research (Anderson & Swanson, 1937; Tinker, 1936). However, the data also revealed stronger parafoveal information effects in silent than in oral reading. In terms of overall reading speed, the availability of parafoveal information improved silent reading speed by 59 wpm, which was more than three times the size of that effect in oral reading (16 wpm). The parafoveal effect on fixation durations was somewhat larger in silent (30 ms) than in oral (18 ms) reading. The parafoveal effect on the number of fixations was substantially larger in silent reading (1.25 fewer fixations per sentence) than in oral reading (.33 fewer fixations per sentence). Together, these data suggest that the availability of parafoveal information affected both the when and the where systems of eye movement control (e.g., Findlay & Walker, 1999; Rayner & McConkie, 1976; Rayner & Pollatsek, 1981).

The data pattern is consistent with previous studies that established the role of parafoveal processes in facilitating silent reading (Rayner, 2009; Rayner et al. 2006). These data indicate that parafoveal information benefits silent reading by reducing the duration and number of fixations. In addition, in this study, we demonstrated that parafoveal information contributes directly to decreases in fixation duration during oral as well as silent reading. In contrast, the number of fixations on a sentence was reduced when parafoveal information was available during silent reading, but not oral reading. Therefore, the data indicate that most of the benefit of parafoveal information to oral reading speed stems from reduced fixation durations. As a consequence, the overall parafoveal preview benefit was smaller when reading aloud than when reading silently.

We propose three possible interpretations of the reduced effect of parafoveal information in oral reading. First, it may be that the additional phonological working memory processes recruited in oral reading to maintain words awaiting pronunciation (and the addition of production processes) deplete the resources available for parafoveal processing (see Inhoff, Connine, Eiter, Radach, & Heller, 2004). Second, parafoveal processes might operate similarly in silent and oral reading, but the benefits of parafoveal information could be mitigated downstream. For example, the phonological representations held in working memory for articulation could interfere more with recently activated phonological representations and thereby reduce the parafoveal information benefit during oral reading. However, this does not explain why the availability of parafoveal information had a relatively small effect on the number of fixations in oral reading. Finally, another possible account is that parafoveal information is processed similarly in the two reading modes, but readers do not utilize it to program fixation location during oral reading.

Our finding that parafoveal information speeded silent reading more for faster readers than for slower readers is consistent with Rayner et al. (2010) and with Chace, Rayner, and Well (2005). The latter study reported larger parafoveal phonological effects for more skilled readers than for less skilled readers. On the basis of the foveal load hypothesis (Henderson & Ferreira, 1990; Kennison & Clifton, 1995; Rayner, 1986; Schroyens, Vitu, Brysbaert, & d'Ydewalle, 1999; White, Rayner, Liversedge, 2005), Chace et al. proposed that slower readers devote more resources to foveal processing and therefore have fewer resources available for parafoveal processing than do faster readers. In the present study, the slow readers were less able than the fast readers to process words in the parafovea to support word skipping during silent reading.

In summary, we found that the availability of parafoveal information facilitated oral as well as silent reading. However, parafoveal information speeded silent reading rate more than oral, because of different effects of preview information on when the eyes move and how often. Lastly, the difference in the parafoveal effects on silent and oral reading rate was larger for faster readers than for slower readers.