Skip to main content
Log in

Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism

  • Brief Reports
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous work has shown that recalling information from long-term memory can impair the long-term retention of related representations—a phenomenon known as retrieval-induced forgetting (Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994). We report an experiment in which the question of whether retrieval is necessary to induce this form of impairment was examined. All the subjects studied six members from each of eight taxonomic categories (e.g.,fruit orange). In the competitive practice condition, the subjects practiced recalling three of the six members, using category-stem cues (e.g.,fruit or____). In the noncompetitive practice condition, the subjects were reexposed to these same members for the same number of repetitions but were asked to recall the category name by using the exemplar and a stem as cues (e.g.,fr____orange). Despite significant and comparable facilitation of practiced items in both conditions, only the competitive practice subjects were impaired in their ability to recall the nonpracticed members on a delayed cued-recall test. These findings argue that retrieval-induced forgetting is not caused by increased competition arising from the strengthening of practiced items, but by inhibitory processes specific to the situation of recall.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

References

  • Allen, G. A., Mahler, W. A., &Estes, W. K. (1969). Effects of recall tests on long-term retention of paired associates.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,8, 463–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., &Bjork, R. A. (1994). Mechanisms of inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.),Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 265–325). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., &Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 1063–1087.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., &McCulloch, K. C. (1999). Integration as a general boundary condition on retrieval-induced forgetting.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,25, 608–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., &Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in memory retrieval. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.),Memory: Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.; pp. 237–313). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., &Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case.Psychological Review,102, 68–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., &Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.),Contemporary development in mathematical psychology: Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 243–293). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Domains of recollection.Psychological Review,89, 708–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, J. M., &Underwood, B. J. (1959). “Fate” of first-list associations in transfer theory.Journal of Experimental Psychology,58, 95–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basden, D. R., &Basden, B. H. (1995). Some tests of the strategy disruption interpretation of part-list cuing inhibition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 1656–1669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Battig, W. F., &Montague, W. E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut norms [Monograph].Journal of Experimental Psychology,80, 1–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bäuml, K.-H. (1996). Revisiting an old issue: Retroactive interference as a function of the degree of original and interpolated learning.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,3, 380–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bäuml, K.-H. (1997). The list-strength effect: Strength-dependent competition or suppression?Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,4, 260–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bäuml, K.-H. (1998). Strong items get suppressed, weak items do not: The role of item strength in output interference.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,5, 459–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier. In R. L. Solso (Ed.),Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaxton, T. A., &Neely, J. H. (1983). Inhibition from semantically related primes: Evidence of a category-specific inhibition.Memory & Cognition,11, 500–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bousfield, W. A., &Barclay, W. D. (1950). The relationship between order and frequency of occurrence of restricted associated responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology,40, 643–647.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, M., &Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention.Memory & Cognition,20, 633–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ciranni, M. A., &Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,25, 1403–1414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H., &Barnhardt, T. M. (1990). Inhibitory semantic priming of lexical decisions due to failure to retrieve weakly activated codes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 328–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • DaPolito, F. J. (1966).Proactive effects with independent retrieval of competing responses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University.

  • Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I. M., &Bleasdale, F. A. (1973). Retrieval difficulty and subsequent recall.Memory & Cognition,1, 213–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 513–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. V. (1989). Back to Woodworth: Role of interlopers in the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon.Memory & Cognition,17, 69–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karchmer, M. A., &Winograd, E. (1971). Effects of studying a subset of familiar items on recall of the remaining items: The John Brown effect.Psychonomic Science,25, 224–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kučera, H., &Francis, W. (1967).Computational analysis of presentday American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence.Psychological Review,8, 252–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mensink, G.-J. M., &Raaijmakers, J. G. (1988). A model of interference and forgetting.Psychological Review,95, 434–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1984). Retrieval inhibition from part-set cuing: A persisting enigma in memory research.Memory & Cognition,12, 531–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L. (1971). Transfer, interference and forgetting. In J. W. Kling & L. A. Riggs (Eds.),Woodworth and Schlosberg’s Experimental psychology (3rd ed., pp. 1019–1132). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T., &Lucas, D. (1984). Using cognitive diaries to investigate naturally occurring memory blocks. In J. E. Harris & P. Morris (Eds.),Everyday memory actions and absent-mindedness (pp. 53–70). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III (1973). Inhibition in recall from cueing with recall targets.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 644–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III (1974). Inhibiting effects of recall.Memory & Cognition,2, 261–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, &Neely, J. H. (1982). Retrieval blocks in episodic and semantic memory.Canadian Journal of Psychology,36, 213–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, &Schmidt, S. R. (1980). Output interference in the recall of categorized and paired associate lists.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,6, 91–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rundus, D. (1973). Negative effects of using list items as recall cues.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 43–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Fiore, S. M., &Brandimonte, M. A. (1997). At a loss from words: Verbal overshadowing of perceptual memories.Psychology of Learning & Motivation,37, 291–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. S., III,Bjork, R. A., &Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,2, 249–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J. (1972). The question of associative growth in the learning of categorized materials.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 324–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J. (1975). Intralist cueing of recognition.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,14, 630–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A., Bower, G. H., &Roher, D. (1991). Congruency effects in part-list cuing inhibition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 974–982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. D. (1971). Output interference and organized recall from longterm memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,10, 400–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. D. (1973). Input order and output interference in organized recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,100, 147–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., &Arbuckle, T. Y. (1963). Sources of intratrial interference in paired-associate learning.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,1, 321–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., &Hastie, R. (1972). Inhibition effects of intralist repetition in free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,92, 297–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wixted, J. T., Ghadisha, H., &Vera, R. (1997). Recall latency following pure and mixed-strength lists: A direct test of the relative strength model of free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 523–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1938).Experimental psychology. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael C. Anderson.

Additional information

The research reported herein was supported in part by startup funds from the University of Oregon. We thank Ted Bell, Geeta Shivde, Bobbie Spellman, Chad Dodson, Mike Ciranni, and three reviewers for comments on an early draft of this paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Anderson, M.C., Bjork, E.L. & Bjork, R.A. Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychon Bull Rev 7, 522–530 (2000). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214366

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214366

Keywords

Navigation