Next Article in Journal
The Association Between Physical Activity, Mental Status, and Social and Family Support with Five Major Non-Communicable Chronic Diseases Among Elderly People: A Cross-Sectional Study of a Rural Population in Southern China
Next Article in Special Issue
An Examination of Electronic Cigarette Content on Social Media: Analysis of E-Cigarette Flavor Content on Reddit
Previous Article in Journal
Spatio-Temporal Trends and Identification of Correlated Variables with Water Quality for Drinking-Water Reservoirs
Previous Article in Special Issue
Predictors of Successful Quitting among Thai Adult Smokers: Evidence from ITC-SEA (Thailand) Survey
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Social Interactions Sparked by Pictorial Warnings on Cigarette Packs

1
Department of Health Behavior, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina, Rosenau Hall CB7440, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
2
Lineberger Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
3
School of Media and Journalism, University of North Carolina, Carroll Hall, CB 3365, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12(10), 13195-13208; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121013195
Submission received: 30 July 2015 / Revised: 2 October 2015 / Accepted: 15 October 2015 / Published: 21 October 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Tobacco Control 2015)

Abstract

:
The Message Impact Framework suggests that social interactions may offer smokers the opportunity to process pictorial warnings on cigarette packs more deeply. We aimed to describe adult smokers’ social interactions about pictorial cigarette pack warnings in two longitudinal pilot studies. In Pilot Study 1, 30 smokers used cigarette packs with one of nine pictorial warnings for two weeks. In Pilot Study 2, 46 smokers used cigarette packs with one of five pictorial warnings for four weeks. Nearly all smokers (97%/96% in Pilot Study 1/2) talked about the warnings with other people, with the most common people being friends (67%/87%) and spouses/significant others (34%/42%). Pilot Study 2 found that 26% of smokers talked about the warnings with strangers. Discussions about the health effects of smoking and quitting smoking were more frequent during the first week of exposure to pictorial warnings than in the week prior to beginning the study (both p < 0.05). Pictorial warnings sparked social interactions about the warnings, the health effects of smoking, and quitting smoking, indicating that pictorial warnings may act as a social intervention reaching beyond the individual. Future research should examine social interactions as a potential mediator of the impact of pictorial warnings on smoking behavior.

1. Introduction

Tobacco use is the leading cause of preventable morbidity and mortality worldwide, causing nearly six million deaths each year [1]. A large body of observational and experimental research suggests that pictorial cigarette pack warnings are a promising policy solution for communicating information about the health risks of smoking and increasing quit intentions [2,3,4]. Based on strong evidence of their superiority over text warnings, the World Health Organization Framework Convention on Tobacco Control calls for large pictorial warnings to be placed on cigarette packs [5]. As of 2015, 77 countries and jurisdictions—representing 50% of the world’s population—had implemented policies requiring pictorial warnings [6].
Social interactions are a key mechanism through which health communication campaigns, including pictorial cigarette pack warnings, may exert their effects [7,8,9,10,11]. However, researchers are only beginning to study social interactions in the context of pictorial warnings [12,13]. While definitions of interpersonal communication and social interactions vary [8,14], we define social interactions as an exchange between two or more people, in this case sparked by a health message. This conceptualization extends beyond in-person conversations to include non-verbal interactions (e.g., showing someone the message) and virtual interactions (e.g., posting about the message on social media). In addition to the role of social interactions in transmitting messages, communications research has explored the potential for social interactions to serve as a mediator or amplifier of campaign effects [8,9,15]. Similarly, our Message Impact Framework [3] suggests that social interactions triggered by messages can facilitate change in smoking attitudes and beliefs.
Social interactions may be particularly important in the context of anti-smoking communication campaigns because smoking is a social behavior, heavily influenced by peer and social networks [16,17]. Smokers are more likely to know other smokers, and smoking behaviors often have ripple effects within social networks [18]. Moreover, smoking behavior often occurs in social settings, affording opportunities for conversations to take place. Studies of anti-smoking campaigns have found associations between campaign-related interpersonal communication and both smoking behavior and its predictors [19,20,21,22,23,24,25]. Unlike traditional anti-smoking mass-media campaigns, pictorial cigarette pack warnings appear on smokers’ cigarette packs, heightening exposure to the warning and providing a unique opportunity to spark social interactions during the act of smoking.
Few studies have examined the role of social interactions about pictorial warnings, and these studies have examined the frequency, rather than the content, of these interactions [12,13]. Moreover, these studies focused on conversations about warnings, rather than other types of interactions (e.g., non-verbal, virtual) [12,13]. A deeper examination of the nature of social interactions around pictorial warnings may shed light on how smokers communicate about these warnings with others in their social network. To that end, we sought to describe the frequency, content, and nature of adult smokers’ social interactions about pictorial cigarette pack warnings in two pilot studies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Pilot Study 1. From February to March 2013, we recruited 30 smokers, ages 18 or older, who we observed smoking cigarettes in public places in North Carolina, USA, or who received referrals from study participants. We defined current smoking as having smoked at least 100 cigarettes in their lifetime and now smoking every day or some days, and we excluded pregnant women, people who smoke only roll-your-own cigarettes, and cigarillo-only smokers.
Pilot Study 2. In June and July 2014, we recruited 48 smokers using the same eligibility criteria as Pilot Study 1. We used a variety of recruitment methods, including newspaper advertisements, flyers, and email lists.

2.2. Procedures

Pilot Study 1. A research assistant explained the study to participants and obtained written informed consent. We provided participants with an $80 cash incentive and accompanied them to a nearby store, where we asked them to purchase the amount of cigarette packs they would normally smoke over two weeks as determined in the initial screening. We randomly assigned participants to receive one of nine pictorial warnings that the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) had proposed in 2011 for implementation in the U.S. (Figure 1) (A lawsuit later challenged these warnings, preventing the FDA from implementing them in the U.S. [26].) Then, research staff removed the package cellophane and applied the same pictorial warning labels to the top half of the front and back panels of participants’ cigarette packs, in accordance with the proposed FDA requirements (Figure 1). As applying the labels on the top half of the front panel sealed the flip-top box shut, staff cut through the label to allow the box to open freely. Participants were instructed to smoke cigarettes only from these labeled packs for the duration of the study. Participants took a survey at baseline and then returned to the study offices to complete a follow-up survey two weeks later and received an additional $50 cash incentive. After completing the follow-up visit survey, each participant received information about a local smoking cessation program.
Pilot Study 2. Procedures for Pilot Study 2 were similar to Pilot Study 1. After obtaining written informed consent, we randomly assigned participants to receive one of five pictorial warnings, all of which we also used in Pilot Study 1. Participants visited our study offices at baseline and then weekly for four weeks, completing a survey at each visit. Smokers brought eight days’ worth of cigarettes to each of the first four appointments. While participants were taking the survey, study staff labeled their packs using the same procedures as in Pilot Study 1. Participants received a cash incentive at the end of each visit, totaling $185. At the final appointment, each participant received information about a local smoking cessation program. Two of the 48 participants withdrew from the study. The University of North Carolina Institutional Review Board approved the procedures for both studies. Brewer et al. [27] provides a more detailed description of the study protocols.
Figure 1. Example of pictorial warning label on pack.
Figure 1. Example of pictorial warning label on pack.
Ijerph 12 13195 g001

2.3. Measures

In Pilot Study 1, the baseline survey assessed demographics and the follow-up survey assessed whether participants talked to anyone about the warnings during the study and with whom they spoke. The survey then included open-ended questions about how conversations about the warning started and the content of those conversations.
The open-ended responses from Pilot Study 1 informed the closed-ended survey items used in Pilot Study 2. Prior to Pilot Study 2, we also refined the survey instrument by conducting cognitive interviews [28] with 15 adult smokers. In Pilot Study 2, participants completed a baseline survey and four follow-up surveys weekly. The surveys asked about the nature of conversations about the warning, including who started the last conversation, conversation partners, topics of conversations, and social media posts about the warnings. Finally, the survey assessed weekly frequency of conversations about the warning, the health effects of smoking, and quitting smoking.
We defined low income as being at or below 200% of the U.S. federal poverty level. The survey instruments are available upon request.

2.4. Data Analysis

We present descriptive statistics for the two studies. For Pilot Study 2 data, we used planned contrasts within a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare frequencies of social interactions about the warnings (conversations about the warnings, showing someone the warnings) during week 1 to each subsequent follow-up week. We repeated the analyses for social interactions about smoking (harms of smoking, quitting smoking), comparing baseline to each subsequent week. Analyses for Pilot Study 2 excluded two participants who withdrew from the study. Thirteen Pilot Study 2 participants did not receive questions about conversation partners or topics due to a survey malfunction. Analyses used SPSS version 22 and Stata/IC version 13.1. We set critical alpha to 0.05 and used two-tailed statistical tests.

3. Results

Participants’ mean age was 30 years in Pilot Study 1 and 43 years in Pilot Study 2 (Table 1). Most smokers were at or below 200% of the US federal poverty level. About 10% of smokers identified as gay or bisexual. The mean number of cigarettes smoked per day was 12 (standard deviation (SD) = 9) in Pilot Study 1 and 11 (SD = 8) in Pilot Study 2.
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
CharacteristicPilot Study 1 (n = 30)
%
Pilot Study 2 (n = 46)
%
Age
  18–24 years404
  25–39 years3337
  40–54 years2733
  55+ years026
  Mean (SD)30 (11)43 (12)
Female4057
Gay or bisexual1011
Hispanic 1013
Race
  Asian74
  Black2735
  White4344
  Other/Multiracial 2317
Education
  High school degree or less3320
  Some college5050
  College graduate1024
  Graduate or professional degree77
Low income (≤200% of poverty level)6959
Cigarettes smoked per day, mean (SD)12 (9)11 (8)
Social interactions about the pictorial warnings were a nearly universal experience in both studies. Twenty-nine of 30 (97%) Pilot Study 1 participants and 44 of 46 (96%) Pilot Study 2 participants talked with others about the warnings at least once. In response to an open-ended question, many participants (60%) in Pilot Study 1 reported that they started conversations about the warning by showing the warning to someone else. Half of Pilot Study 1 smokers (50%) mentioned that conversations began when others noticed and asked about the warning on their pack. Similarly, in Pilot Study 2, when asked how the last conversation about the warning started, 66% of participants reported that they initiated the last conversation about the warning, whereas 34% said that someone else initiated the conversation.
Smokers in both studies talked about the warning with a wide variety of people, as shown in Figure 2 (Pilot Study 1 n = 29; Pilot Study 2 n = 31 after accounting for missing data for 13 people). Most participants reported talking about the warning with a friend (67% in Pilot Study 1 and 87% in Pilot Study 2). Some talked with significant others/spouses, other family members, and co-workers (between 33% and 58% of participants). Few participants talked with a medical professional about the warning (3% in Pilot Study 1 and 10% in Pilot Study 2). Ten percent of Pilot Study 2 participants talked to children, and 26% talked to someone they did not previously know; Pilot Study 1 did not assess these categories.
Figure 2. Partners in conversations about warnings, Pilot Study 1/2. Among Pilot Study 1/2 smokers (n = 29/31) who had at least one conversation about the warnings. Data were missing for 13 Pilot Study 2 participants; -- not assessed.
Figure 2. Partners in conversations about warnings, Pilot Study 1/2. Among Pilot Study 1/2 smokers (n = 29/31) who had at least one conversation about the warnings. Data were missing for 13 Pilot Study 2 participants; -- not assessed.
Ijerph 12 13195 g002
The remainder of the findings in our paper reflect data only from Pilot Study 2 participants. Some Pilot Study 2 participants reported talking about the warning with only smokers (17%) or mostly smokers (34%; Table 2). However, conversations with non-smokers were also common, with about half of participants (48%) talking about the warning with a mix of smokers and non-smokers, mostly non-smokers, or only non-smokers. Conversations with non-smokers about the health effects of smoking and quitting smoking were even more common; 70% of smokers reported talking about the health effects of smoking with a mix of smokers and non-smokers, mostly non-smokers, or only non-smokers. Likewise, 63% of participants talked about quitting smoking with a mix, mostly non-smokers, or only non-smokers.
Table 2. Conversations sparked by warnings in Pilot Study 2, Week 1.
Table 2. Conversations sparked by warnings in Pilot Study 2, Week 1.
Conversation PartnersAbout Warnings
n (%)
About Health Effects of Smoking
n (%)
About Quitting Smoking
n (%)
Only smokers5 (17)3 (9)6 (18)
Mostly smokers10 (34)7 (21)6 (18)
Mix of smokers and non-smokers11 (38)17 (52)15 (45)
Mostly non-smokers2 (7)3 (9)1 (3)
Only non-smokers1 (3)3 (9)5 (15)
n293333
Notes: Data from people who had at least one conversation about these topics. Data were missing for 13 people in “about warnings” column.
When asked about the content of their conversations, most participants discussed the perceived effectiveness of the warning, including whether the warning would encourage other smokers to quit smoking (77%), would make them want to quit smoking (71%), or would discourage people from starting to smoke (68%; Table 3). Many smokers also discussed the research study (71%) or whether warnings like this should be on all cigarette packs (65%). Few participants reported making fun of the warning (13%). Participants or their conversation partners described the warning as scary (71%), informative/useful (55%), gross (55%), depressing/gloomy (52%), or interesting/engaging (42%; Table 3). Few described the warnings as stupid/pointless (19%) or judgmental/controlling (10%). Two participants reported posting about the warning on Facebook, but no one reported posting on other social media platforms.
Pilot Study 2 participants had about four conversations about the warning during the first week of the study (Figure 3). Smokers had fewer conversations at weeks 2, 3, and 4 compared to week 1 (all decreases p < 0.05), but continued to have almost two conversations per week by the end of the study. Participants showed someone else the warning about three times during the first week of the study, but this number also declined over time (p < 0.05).
The number of conversations about the health effects of smoking increased from baseline to week 1 (p < 0.05; Figure 4). The number of conversations about quitting smoking also increased from baseline to week 1 (p < 0.05; Figure 4). However, the number of conversations about health effects and quitting was not different at weeks 2, 3, and 4 compared to baseline.
Table 3. Content of conversations in Pilot Study 2, across all weeks (n = 31).
Table 3. Content of conversations in Pilot Study 2, across all weeks (n = 31).
Content of Conversationsn (%)
Topics discussed in conversations
  Whether warning would encourage smokers to quit smoking24 (77)
  Whether warning would make me want to quit smoking22 (71)
  Discussed this research study22 (71)
  Whether warning would discourage people from starting to smoke21 (68)
  Whether warnings like this should be on cigarette packs20 (65)
  Made fun of warning4 (13)
Descriptions of warnings in conversations
  Scary22 (71)
  Informative, useful17 (55)
  Gross17 (55)
  Depressing, gloomy16 (52)
  Interesting, engaging13 (42)
  Stupid, pointless6 (19)
  Judgmental, controlling3 (10)
Note: Data from people who had at least one conversation about the warning. Data were missing for 13 people.
Figure 3. Social interactions about warnings (Pilot Study 2, n = 46), * p < 0.05, compared to week 1; (A) Conversations about the warning; (B) Showed someone the warning.
Figure 3. Social interactions about warnings (Pilot Study 2, n = 46), * p < 0.05, compared to week 1; (A) Conversations about the warning; (B) Showed someone the warning.
Ijerph 12 13195 g003
Figure 4. Social interactions about smoking (Pilot Study 2, n = 46), * p < 0.05, compared to baseline; (A) Conversations about health effects of smoking; (B) Conversations about quitting smoking.
Figure 4. Social interactions about smoking (Pilot Study 2, n = 46), * p < 0.05, compared to baseline; (A) Conversations about health effects of smoking; (B) Conversations about quitting smoking.
Ijerph 12 13195 g004

4. Discussion

Interacting with others about pictorial cigarette pack warnings was a nearly universal experience in both studies, indicating that pictorial warnings exert influence in a fundamentally social manner. Our study found that the pictorial warnings sparked conversations not only about the warnings themselves, but also about the health effects of smoking and about quitting smoking. In other words, pictorial warnings generated meaningful conversations that covered important topics beyond simply talking about the warning itself. We previously reported that most smokers noticed these warnings and about half reported that the warnings caused them to think about the harmful effects of smoking [27]. Moreover, we previously reported that endorsement of positive prototypes of smokers decreased and intention to quit smoking increased between baseline and follow-up. Thus, pictorial warnings may motivate both social interactions and changes in attitudes and intentions. While our study design did not allow us to examine social interactions as a mechanism of change, several theories and frameworks, including the Message Impact Framework [3], highlight social interactions as a possible part of the attitude and behavior change process [3,8,9,10,29]. Two previous observational studies provide further support for the importance of social interactions as a mechanism of attitude and behavior change. A longitudinal study of Australian students ages 7–12 found that frequency of talking about pictorial cigarette pack warnings was associated with lower intentions to smoke, after controlling for individual and parental smoking behavior [13]. Similarly, longitudinal data collected from panels of adult smokers in Canada, Australia, and Mexico found that frequency of talking about pictorial warnings on cigarette packs predicted subsequent quit attempts in adjusted analyses [12]. Our findings, taken together with these previous studies, suggest that social interactions are an integral part of the way that smokers experience pictorial warnings on cigarette packs.
Smokers in our study talked about the warnings with friends, family members, coworkers, medical professionals, and even strangers. Thus, the reach of pictorial cigarette pack warnings extended beyond study participants to others in their social network. Moreover, many participants reported talking with both smokers and non-smokers about the warnings, the health effects of smoking, and quitting smoking. Indirectly influencing non-smokers through health communication campaigns may be a promising way to shift social norms and discourage smoking [30]. For instance, an evaluation of a national anti-smoking campaign in the US, “Tips from Former Smokers,” found that the campaign resulted in millions more non-smokers talking about the dangers of smoking and recommending cessation services to friends or family [30]. Future pictorial warning studies should consider exploring conversations with non-smokers in greater depth, both qualitatively and quantitatively, to determine how these conversations influence subsequent changes in smoking attitudes and behavior.
The frequency of social interactions peaked at the one-week follow-up visit. This early spike in social interactions may represent a special opportunity for complementary tobacco control campaigns and interventions. However, we found that the frequency of social interactions declined over the four-week period after this initial spike. Given that our study placed only a single warning on smokers’ packs and did not place warnings on the packs of other smokers in a social network, it is possible that the effects of our warnings on social interactions were prematurely attenuated. Previous studies have found that cigarette pack warnings are most effective when newly implemented and that responses to the warnings tend to partially “wear out” over the course of months or years [31,32,33,34]. Studies have found that some smokers actively avoid looking at pictorial warnings [34,35,36] and may become habituated to seeing them over time [34], both of which may contribute to wear-out effects. Thrasher et al. (2015) found that the percentage of smokers talking about cigarette pack warnings was relatively high (50%) immediately following the implementation of new pictorial warnings in Canada, but declined at the next survey wave four months later. Several countries, including Australia, Belgium, New Zealand, Mexico, and Trinidad and Tobago, mandate that the content of cigarette pack warnings must change on a regular basis [6]. Research has found that periodic rotation of cigarette pack warnings can help to minimize wear-out effects [32,37]. Indeed, the frequency of talking about cigarette pack warnings has remained relatively stable over time in Mexico, where pictorial warnings rotate every six months [12].
Social media may also offer an opportunity to enhance the reach and impact of pictorial warnings [38]. While we found few unprompted interactions about the warnings via social media, campaigns could encourage sharing and interacting about them through social media. In addition to naturally-occurring social media engagement, mass and social media campaigns implemented in conjunction with pictorial warnings may amplify the effects of pictorial warnings [39]. A study of adult smokers in Mexico found that exposure to a complementary mass media campaign was associated with greater attention to pictorial warnings and cognitive elaboration (i.e., the extent to which the warnings made participants think about health risks and about quitting) [40]. Similarly, two studies in Australia found evidence that pictorial warnings and television advertisements worked in a synergistic manner in which the advertisements heightened the effect of pictorial warning exposure on knowledge of the health effects of smoking [41]. Future studies should examine potential synergistic effects of mass media and social media campaigns that complement pictorial warnings.
Strengths of our study include the use of cognitively-tested measures about social interactions, as well as a naturalistic pack labeling protocol that exposes smokers to warnings on their actual cigarette pack [27]. Moreover, the longitudinal study design allowed us to assess trends over time in Pilot Study 2. However, the study took place in the US, where pictorial warnings are not currently on cigarette packs, potentially heightening the immediate novelty of the warnings and thus sparking conversations about the warnings. The external validity of these findings for smokers in other settings and over a longer period of time has yet to be established. The small sample size may have limited our statistical power to detect trends in the frequency of interactions over time.

5. Conclusions

Pictorial cigarette pack warnings sparked social interactions about the warnings as well as conversations about the health effects of smoking and quitting smoking. Nearly every participant in our two pilot studies reported talking about the warning, indicating that pictorial warnings function as a social intervention. Future research should explore whether social interactions mediate the relationship between pictorial warning exposure and smoking-related attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Moreover, future studies should examine the content of social interactions and role of conversation partners (e.g., smoker vs. non-smoker) as potential moderators of social interactions effects.

Acknowledgments

Research reported in this publication was supported by The National Cancer Institute and FDA Center for Tobacco Products (CTP) under Award Number P30CA016086-38S2. 5P50CA180907 from the National Cancer Institute and FDA Center for Tobacco Products (CTP) supported MGH’s time spent writing the paper. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or the Food and Drug Administration. The authors thank the research participants for taking part in this study. The authors also thank the Pacific Institute for Research and Evaluation for assistance with participant recruitment and data collection.

Author Contributions

Marissa G. Hall conducted statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript, with guidance from Noel T. Brewer. Kathryn Peebles assisted with analysis, background research, and writing. Authors worked together to develop the measures and protocols. All authors provided critical feedback on drafts of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

Dr. Ribisl has served as an expert consultant in litigation against cigarette manufacturers and Internet tobacco vendors. The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. World Health Organization. Who Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic, 2013: Enforcing Bans on Tobacco Advertising, Promotion and Sponsorship; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Noar, S.M.; Hall, M.G.; Brewer, N.T. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings have important effects. Am. J. Public Health 2015. [CrossRef]
  3. Noar, S.M.; Hall, M.G.; Francis, D.; Ribisl, K.M.; Pepper, J.; Brewer, N.T. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob. Control 2015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hammond, D. Health warning messages on tobacco products: A review. Tob. Control 2011, 20, 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. World Health Organization. Framework Convention on Tobacco Control. Available online: http://www.who.int/fctc/en/ (accessed on 20 April 2015).
  6. Canadian Cancer Society. Cigarette Package Health Warnings: International Status Report; Canadian Cancer Society: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  7. Katz, E.; Lazarsfeld, P.F. Personal Influence, the Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communications; Transaction Publishers: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  8. Southwell, B.G.; Yzer, M.C. The roles of interpersonal communication in mass media campaigns. Commun. Yearb. 2007, 31, 420–462. [Google Scholar]
  9. Southwell, B.G.; Yzer, M.C. When (and why) interpersonal talk matters for campaigns. Commun. Theory 2009, 19, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hornik, R.C.; Yanovitzky, I. Using theory to design evaluations of communication campaigns: The case of the national youth anti-drug media campaign. Commun. Theory 2003, 13, 204–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Noar, S.M. A 10-year retrospective of research in health mass media campaigns: Where do we go from here? J. Health Commun. 2006, 11, 21–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Thrasher, J.F.; Abad-Vivero, E.N.; Huang, L.; O’Connor, R.J.; Hammond, D.; Bansal-Travers, M.; Yong, H.H.; Borland, R.; Markovsky, B.; Hardin, J. Interpersonal communication about pictorial health warnings on cigarette packages: Policy-related influences and relationships with smoking cessation attempts. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. White, V.; Webster, B.; Wakefield, M. Do graphic health warning labels have an impact on adolescents’ smoking-related beliefs and behaviours? Addiction 2008, 103, 1562–1571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Helme, D.W.; Noar, S.M.; Allard, S.; Zimmerman, R.S.; Palmgreen, P.; McClanahan, K.J. In-depth investigation of interpersonal discussions in response to a safer sex mass media campaign. Health Commun. 2011, 26, 366–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Jeong, M.; Tan, A.S.; Brennan, E.; Gibson, L.; Hornik, R.C. Talking about quitting: Interpersonal communication as a mediator of campaign effects on smokers’ quit behaviors. J. Health Commun. 2015, 20, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Chen, P.H.; White, H.R.; Pandina, R.J. Predictors of smoking cessation from adolescence into young adulthood. Addict. Behav. 2001, 26, 517–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Powell, L.M.; Tauras, J.A.; Ross, H. The importance of peer effects, cigarette prices and tobacco control policies for youth smoking behavior. J. Health Econ. 2005, 24, 950–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Christakis, N.A.; Fowler, J.H. The collective dynamics of smoking in a large social network. N. Engl. J. Med. 2008, 358, 2249–2258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Dunlop, S.; Wakefield, M.; Kashima, Y. Can you feel it? Negative emotion, risk, and narrative in health communication. Media Psychol. 2008, 11, 52–75. [Google Scholar]
  20. Durkin, S.; Wakefield, M. Maximizing the impact of emotive antitobacco advertising: Effects of interpersonal discussion and program placement. Soc. Mark. Q. 2006, 12, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hafstad, A.; Aarø, L.; Langmark, F. Evaluation of an anti-smoking mass media campaign targeting adolescents: The role of affective responses and interpersonal communication. Health Educ. Res. 1996, 11, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hafstad, A.; Aaro, L.E.; Engeland, A.; Andersen, A.; Langmark, F.; Stray-Pedersen, B. Provocative appeals in anti-smoking mass media campaigns targeting adolescents—The accumulated effect of multiple exposures. Health Educ. Res. 1997, 12, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Van den Putte, B.; Yzer, M.; Southwell, B.G.; de Bruijn, G.J.; Willemsen, M.C. Interpersonal communication as an indirect pathway for the effect of antismoking media content on smoking cessation. J. Health Commun. 2011, 16, 470–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Schuster, D.V.; Valente, T.W.; Skara, S.N.; Wenten, M.R.; Unger, J.B.; Cruz, T.B.; Rohrbach, L.A. Intermedia processes in the adoption of tobacco control activities among opinion leaders in california. Commun. Theory 2006, 16, 91–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Dunlop, S.M.; Wakefield, M.; Kashima, Y. The contribution of antismoking advertising to quitting: Intra- and interpersonal processes. J. Health Commun. 2008, 13, 250–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Kraemer, J.D.; Baig, S.A. Analysis of legal and scientific issues in court challenges to graphic tobacco warnings. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2013, 45, 334–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Brewer, N.T.; Hall, M.G.; Lee, J.G.; Peebles, K.; Noar, S.M.; Ribisl, K.M. Testing warning messages on smokers’ cigarette packages: A standardised protocol. Tob. Control 2015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Willis, G.B. Cognitive Interviewing: A tool for Improving Questionnaire Design; Sage Publications Incorporated: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  29. Strahan, E.J.; White, K.; Fong, G.T.; Fabrigar, L.R.; Zanna, M.P.; Cameron, R. Enhancing the effectiveness of tobacco package warning labels: A social psychological perspective. Tob. Control 2002, 11, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. McAfee, T.; Davis, K.C.; Alexander, R.L., Jr.; Pechacek, T.F.; Bunnell, R. Effect of the first federally funded us antismoking national media campaign. Lancet 2013, 382, 2003–2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hammond, D.; Fong, G.T.; Borland, R.; Cummings, K.M.; McNeill, A.; Driezen, P. Text and graphic warnings on cigarette packages: Findings from the international tobacco control four country study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2007, 32, 202–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Borland, R.; Wilson, N.; Fong, G.T.; Hammond, D.; Cummings, K.M.; Yong, H.-H.; Hosking, W.; Hastings, G.; Thrasher, J.; McNeill, A. Impact of graphic and text warnings on cigarette packs: Findings from four countries over five years. Tob. Control 2009, 18, 358–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hitchman, S.C.; Driezen, P.; Logel, C.; Hammond, D.; Fong, G.T. Changes in effectiveness of cigarette health warnings over time in canada and the united states, 2002–2011. Nicotin Tob. Res. 2014, 16, 536–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Bell, K.; Dennis, S.; Robinson, J.; Moore, R. Does the hand that controls the cigarette packet rule the smoker? Findings from ethnographic interviews with smokers in canada, australia, the united kingdom and the USA. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015, 142, 136–144. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Maynard, O.M.; Attwood, A.; O’Brien, L.; Brooks, S.; Hedge, C.; Leonards, U.; Munafo, M.R. Avoidance of cigarette pack health warnings among regular cigarette smokers. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2014, 136, 170–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hardcastle, S.J.; Chan, D.C.; Caudwell, K.M.; Sultan, S.; Cranwell, J.; Chatzisarantis, N.L.; Hagger, M.S. Larger and more prominent graphic health warnings on plain-packaged tobacco products and avoidant responses in current smokers: A qualitative study. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Hammond, D. Tobacco labelling and packaging toolkit. Available online: http://www.tobaccolabels.ca/toolkit (accessed on 10 December 2014).
  38. Kornfield, R.; Smith, K.C. Earned media and public engagement with cdc’s “tips from former smokers” campaign: An analysis of online news and blog coverage. J. Med. Internet Res. 2015. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hammond, D.; Wakefield, M.; Durkin, S.; Brennan, E. Tobacco packaging and mass media campaigns: Research needs for articles 11 and 12 of the who framework convention on tobacco control. Nicotin Tob. Res. 2013, 15, 817–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Thrasher, J.F.; Murukutla, N.; Perez-Hernandez, R.; Alday, J.; Arillo-Santillan, E.; Cedillo, C.; Gutierrez, J.P. Linking mass media campaigns to pictorial warning labels on cigarette packages: A cross-sectional study to evaluate effects among mexican smokers. Tob. Control 2013, 22, e57–e65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Brennan, E.; Durkin, S.J.; Cotter, T.; Harper, T.; Wakefield, M.A. Mass media campaigns designed to support new pictorial health warnings on cigarette packets: Evidence of a complementary relationship. Tob. Control 2011, 20, 412–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hall, M.G.; Peebles, K.; Bach, L.E.; Noar, S.M.; Ribisl, K.M.; Brewer, N.T. Social Interactions Sparked by Pictorial Warnings on Cigarette Packs. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 13195-13208. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121013195

AMA Style

Hall MG, Peebles K, Bach LE, Noar SM, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. Social Interactions Sparked by Pictorial Warnings on Cigarette Packs. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015; 12(10):13195-13208. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121013195

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hall, Marissa G., Kathryn Peebles, Laura E. Bach, Seth M. Noar, Kurt M. Ribisl, and Noel T. Brewer. 2015. "Social Interactions Sparked by Pictorial Warnings on Cigarette Packs" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 12, no. 10: 13195-13208. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121013195

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop