Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Robot. AI, 20 August 2019
Sec. Humanoid Robotics
Volume 6 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2019.00073

INTRApersonal Synchrony as Constituent of INTERpersonal Synchrony and Its Relevance for Autism Spectrum Disorder

Carola Bloch1,2* Kai Vogeley1,3 Alexandra L. Georgescu4 Christine M. Falter-Wagner2,5*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Faculty, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Faculty, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany
  • 3Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM3), Research Center Jülich, Jülich, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Science, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany

INTERpersonal synchrony leads to increased empathy, rapport and understanding, enabling successful human-human interactions and reciprocal bonding. Research shows that individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) exhibit difficulties to INTERpersonally synchronize but underlying causes are yet unknown. In order to successfully synchronize with others, INTRApersonal synchronization of communicative signals appears to be a necessary prerequisite. We understand INTRApersonal synchrony as an implicit factor of INTERpersonal synchrony and therefore hypothesize that atypicalities of INTRApersonal synchrony may add to INTERpersonal synchrony problems in ASD and their interaction partners. In this perspective article, we first review evidence for INTERpersonal dissynchrony in ASD, with respect to different approaches and assessment methods. Second, we draft a theoretical conceptualization of INTRApersonal dissynchrony in ASD based on a temporal model of human interaction. We will outline literature indicating INTRApersonal dissynchrony in ASD, therefore highlighting findings of atypical timing functions and findings from clinical and behavioral studies that indicate peculiar motion patterns and communicative signal production in ASD. Third, we hypothesize that findings from these domains suggest an assessment and investigation of temporal parameters of social behavior in individuals with ASD. We will further propose specific goals of empirical approaches on INTRApersonal dissynchrony. Finally we present implications of research on INTRApersonal timing in ASD for diagnostic and therapeutic purposes, what in our opinion warrants the increase of research efforts in this domain.

1. INTERpersonal Dissynchrony

Terms such as interactional synchrony, non-verbal mirroring, shared rhythmicity, motor mimicry or chameleon effect embrace the underlying dimension of coordination between two or more individuals in the domain of nonverbal action” (Ramseyer and Tschacher, 2008, p.332).

Across different terminology INTERpersonal synchrony describes the phenomenon that people automatically align behavior while interacting. This is thought to strengthen their social bond by means of increased rapport (LaFrance, 1979; Tickle-Degnen and Rosenthal, 1990; Lakin and Chartrand, 2003; Vacharkulksemsuk and Fredrickson, 2012), mutual affiliation (Hove and Risen, 2009), enhanced mentalizing (Baimel et al., 2018), successful joint action (Valdesolo et al., 2010; Lorenz et al., 2014), as well as empathy (Behrends et al., 2012; Koehne et al., 2016). Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is defined as a neurodevelopmental disorder that entails difficulties in social communication and interaction together with repetitive behaviors and restricted interests (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and there exists evidence for INTERpersonal dissynchrony of individuals with ASD with interaction partners.

INTERpersonal synchrony as a dependent variable in groups of healthy control persons was measured in reference to parameters in a dynamical model of human movements dynamical model of human movements (Haken et al., 1985). Those studies measured coordinated movements between two individuals in terms of reduced changes in relative phase angles between reference points in two oscillating systems (Richardson et al., 2007; Schmidt and Richardson, 2008; Romero et al., 2015). With respect to individuals with ASD one study found less coordination of movements measured by the alignment of phase angles between two rocking chairs (Marsh et al., 2013). Similarly, individuals with ASD synchronized pendulum swings less with their parents (Fitzpatrick et al., 2016).

Fitzpatrick et al. (2017a) separately investigated performance in intentional vs. spontaneous synchrony tasks and found lower INTERpersonal coherence scores in both domains for children with ASD. Additionally, the authors found distinct cognitive mechanisms underlying both kinds of alignment problems (Fitzpatrick et al., 2017b). In a naturalistic setting, spontaneous INTERpersonal synchrony was measured by coherence of body motion of two interaction partners in predefined regions of interest, thereby not focusing on external oscillators or specific limbs, rather on general body motion (Ramseyer and Tschacher, 2011; Romero et al., 2015). So-called Motion Energy Analysis (MEA) (Ramseyer and Tschacher, 2011) calculates cross-correlation time series of pixel changes from video-recorded interactions as an indicator for coordinated movements. Noel et al. (2018) used MEA in their study and showed that individuals with ASD exhibited less INTERpersonal synchrony and less complex movements in an interview setting.

Relevant for understanding INTERpersonal synchrony are also joint action paradigms, in which participants have to conduct actions that require consideration of another person's perspective and movement affordances in the course of motion planning. When assessing the motor anticipation of a partner's grip comfort when passing objects, participants with ASD showed more variable grip positions indicating atypical social motor planning (Gonzalez et al., 2013). Moreover, with increasing severity of ASD traits, participants modulated grip movements less in adaption to a partner's movements, but performed well in a non-social replication task indicating deficits only for the social domain (Curioni et al., 2017). Other studies found less grip-to-reach positions that enhanced end-state comfort for the partner (Scharoun and Bryden, 2016; Studenka et al., 2017) and more variable reaction times, slower movements and more movement dissynchrony with an interaction partner (Fulceri et al., 2018).

Besides investigations of alignments of whole body or limb movements, mutual gaze and the establishment of joint attention are of particular interest for INTERpersonal coupling processes (Emery, 2000; Senju and Johnson, 2009). Gaze behavior is the first non-verbal source for the coordination of behavior between newborn and parent and therefore a driving force for the development of non-verbal reciprocity (Feldman, 2007). Empirical evidence for atypical gaze behavior and atypical processing of gaze cues in individuals with ASD is now overwhelming, in particular early aversion of social gaze (Jones and Klin, 2013), altered attention preferences for social cues in form of gaze avoidance (Madipakkam et al., 2017) and less contact or involvement evoked by direct gaze (Schwartz et al., 2010). Gaze idiosyncrasies were already found in children with ASD (Jones and Klin, 2013) and are still present in adults (Schwartz et al., 2010; Georgescu et al., 2013; Madipakkam et al., 2017; Caruana et al., 2018) and are not caused by oculomotor disfunctions (Caruana et al., 2018). In conclusion, empirical evidence from several domains indicate reduced body motion alignment, less anticipation of other persons' kinematics in motor planning as well as atypical social gaze as features of individuals with ASD that contribute to INTERpersonal dissynchrony (see Table 1 for an overview).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies on INTERpersonal synchrony in ASD.

2. INTRApersonal Dissynchrony in ASD

In their social entrainment model, McGrath and Kelly (1986) consider social interaction in terms of temporal patterns or rhythms in behavior. This model states that endogenous (i.e. individual) rhythms in behavior become temporally aligned in phase and period in the course of interaction. This implies the emergence of systematic temporal patterns of verbal and non-verbal turn-taking during INTERpersonal encounters. Based on this, one can assume that there exist temporal windows of signal production that are critical for communication efficiency and INTERpersonal alignment. From an individual perspective, communication signals are composed of various non-verbal sources (e.g., gaze and gestures). These need to be coordinated with each other and with verbal output to achieve the intended communicative effects. We define INTRApersonal synchrony as the temporal coordination of communication signals in a socially informative manner. In the following, we will review evidence of atypical temporal processing and movement patterns in ASD. We will then introduce the idea that those peculiarities may be related to individuals with ASD missing the assumed temporal windows for producing socially effective communication signals.

2.1. Temporal Processing in ASD

Temporal processing of sensory input seems to be altered in individuals with ASD. For instance, in a perceptual simultaneity task individuals with ASD judged the presentation of two visual stimuli to be temporally asynchronous for smaller stimulus onset asynchronies compared to typically developed (TD) control participants (Falter et al., 2012a). Further empirical evidence shows an enhanced temporal parsing of auditory (Jones et al., 2009) and visual events (Falter et al. 2013; but see Isaksson et al. 2018), lower hit rates for the detection of differences in temporal intervals between auditory signals (Falter et al., 2012b), atypical judgment and reproduction of durations (Szelag et al., 2004) and wider multisensory temporal binding windows for simultaneity judgments (Noel et al., 2018). All of these findings support the notion of atypical temporal processing in ASD, possibly associated with a detail-focused, less holistic cognitive style as postulated in the ‘Weak Central Coherence Theory’ of autism (Happé and Frith, 2006). Atypical temporal processing also manifests in higher level processes, such as the subjective experience of time. Allman et al. (2014) in this context argue that stereotypical behavior patterns and behavioral routines serve the structuring of subjective time experience in ASD which compensates for atypical internal timing functions. In line with that are results of a high tendency in ASD to rely on self-structured routines and repetitive behavior to control bottom-up perceptual input, thereby generating experiences of timelessness or “flow” (Vogel et al., 2018a,b).

Atypical temporal processing in ASD most likely influences behavior as well, given that sensorimotor frameworks propose feedback loops of sensory and motor systems (Wolpert et al., 2003; Torres et al., 2013). In this line, Gowen and Miall (2005) found atypical motor timing in an ASD sample, namely faster and more variable responses in a finger tapping task and results were replicated by Isaksson et al. (2018). In addition to this evidence for altered motor timing, behavioral research underpins the assumption that individuals with ASD exhibit atypical movement patterns, as shown in the following literature.

2.2. Motor Production in ASD

Clinically, “clumsiness” in motor production is a major feature of autism (Asperger, 1944). Although still a secondary criterion for diagnosis, Parma and de Marchena (2015) argue that atypical motor patterns in ASD need to be further investigated as they occur across the spectrum and may constitute a possible diagnostic marker. In this context a study by Anzulewicz et al. (2016) successfully discriminated children with ASD from TD children by a machine learning algorithm that deployed motor variables from a gaming task with a touch screen. Children with ASD exhibited significantly faster movements with peculiar pressure patterns. Other studies further highlight jerky limb movements (Cook et al., 2013), atypical gait (Barrow et al., 2011; Kindregan et al., 2015; Dufek et al., 2017; Eggleston et al., 2017), enhanced postural sway (Gowen and Miall, 2005; Doumas et al., 2016) and enhanced variability in motor output (Brincker and Torres, 2013; Gowen and Hamilton, 2013; Parma and de Marchena, 2015; Kaur et al., 2018). A meta-analysis by Fournier et al. (2010) included 41 studies on motor coordination, motor impairment, arm movement, gait, or postural stability. They found a significant effect indicating weaker motor performance in ASD individuals, independent from symptom severity. A review by Gowen and Hamilton (2013) systematically inspected approaches on motor abilities in ASD on the background of a computational model that postulates intermediate cognitive steps of motor processing. The authors suggest poorer integration of sensory input for motor planning as well as increased variability in motor output or “motor noise” as integral characteristics in ASD.

Taken together, there is cumulative evidence for atypical movement patterns in terms of reduced coordination and greater variability in motor production. Together with evidence for peculiarities in motor timing (Gowen and Miall, 2005; Isaksson et al., 2018), movement aberrations may influence INTERpersonal communication because communicative signals as motor acts dissociate from typical signal production with respect to temporal emergence.

2.3. INTRApersonal Dissynchrony in Interactions

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) as a standard diagnostoc tool targets the coordination of communication channels as a symptom of ASD (Lord et al., 2000). Regarding social contexts there exists research in atypical gesture production in individuals with ASD. In a study by de Marchena and Eigsti (2010), the authors counted gesture usage and coded types of gestures in a narrative task with ASD and TD adolescents. They found no differences in frequency and the kind of gesture used but atypical timing of gestures related to co-occurring speech led to reduced ratings of communication quality in naive observers. In another study on gesture usage in infants with ASD, Colgan et al. (2006) also found no differences in the frequency of gestures, but infants with ASD showed a reduced variety of gestures compared to TD control participants. That is in line with findings of less complexity in non-verbal behavior found by Noel et al. (2018). These results indicate that it is not the quantity of communicative signals that leads to the known communication difficulties but the quality of signals and how they fit in the interactional flow. In the social context it is noteworthy to mention, that the temporal thresholds of perceptual simultaneity (that indicated enhanced temporal parsing of sensory events) in ASD were significantly correlated with difficulties in the communications domain, especially when difficulties were assessed with items encompassing the use of communicative gestures and social imitation (Falter et al., 2012b). Isaksson et al. (2018) likewise found an association of enhanced temporal parsing and symptom severity in communication and social interaction. Noel et al. (2018) further demonstrated that multisensory temporal binding windows correlated with INTERpersonal synchrony in TD participants but not in participants with ASD, indicating distinctive associations in the multisensory temporal domain.

Those findings imply that temporal processing in ASD may be associated with the reduced INTERpersonal alignments. If this association is mediated by atypical social signal timing, is targeted by our proposed perspective on INTRApersonal dissynchrony in ASD.

3. Perspective on Future Research

The temporal model of social interactions by McGrath and Kelly (1986) implies that synchronous alignments require mutual responsiveness and coordinated signal production. Individuals with ASD exhibit atypical temporal processing and motor patterns, what most likely disrupts the emergence or maintenance of systematic INTERpersonal coupling. In line with that, we argue that future research needs to extend findings of deviant motor timing (Gowen and Miall, 2005; Barakova and Chonnaparamutt, 2009; Isaksson et al., 2018) to the domain of socially expressive behavior and investigate the impact of INTRApersonal dissynchrony on interactions.

Therefore we suggest approaches on INTRApersonal dissynchrony should pursue two consecutive goals. First, the aim is to quantify temporal deviations in communication behavior in ASD and to find critical temporal windows of INTRApersonal synchronous signal production. State-of-the-art techniques, such as motion capture, eye tracking and video tracking should be used to assess time series of communication behavior. This allows the investigation of peculiarities of signal timing in multiple communication contexts. Combining such techniques makes it possible to assess the temporal coordination of separate signal sources (e.g., gaze, gestures, facial expressions, speech) in terms of relational signal onsets, durations and end points. Thereby one may gain insights into the temporal composition of individual signal streams. On the background of findings of enhanced motor variability (Brincker and Torres, 2013; Gowen and Hamilton, 2013; Kaur et al., 2018) the investigation of measures of dispersion in ASD samples will be of particular interest. Furthermore, implementing perceptual timing tasks may lead to insights in functional relations of temporal processing and social motor timing in ASD. Thus, the questions if timing of communicative channels is affected by a general sensory timing deficit or by social contexts or both can be addressed by comparing task performance in social and non-social tasks of varying sensory complexity. With regard to the neurophysiological framework of predictive coding, it would be highly interesting to investigate, if INTRApersonal dissynchrony also manifests in EEG patterns with social cues produced by individuals with ASD possibly missing predictive timing windows (Arnal and Giraud, 2012). As physical arousal and stress may be enhanced in social tasks in ASD participants, further assessment of heart rate and skin conductance constitute important covariates.

A subsequent goal would be to analyze the perception of idiosyncratic communication patterns, here targeting causal effects of INTRApersonal dissynchrony on INTERpersonal outcomes. Therefore, motion capture data should be used to animate virtual characters in order to create ecologically valid and standardized stimulus material for perception studies (Bente and Krämer, 2002; Georgescu et al., 2014; Pan and Hamilton, 2018). By assessing impression, evaluation and recognition of altered signal production in ASD, one may draw causal conclusions for deficits in social interactions. Dependent variables should be included in such perception studies that are critical for the quality of the produced signal, e.g., communication efficiency and measures of INTERpersonal bonding, e.g., likeability. Creating an “autistic avatar” would allow experimental manipulation of movement parameters under high experimental control. It is of great relevance to illuminate the perspective of the interaction partner to fully understand developmental pathways and resulting communication deficits. There is evidence that TD participants show poorer performance in decoding expressive movements generated by individuals with ASD, indicating reciprocal lack of mentalization (Edey et al., 2016). On presentation of short video clips or still frames of individuals with ASD, independent raters judged individuals with ASD less favorably and reported less motivation to socially approach them (Sasson et al., 2017). An avatar that exhibits specific autistic movement patterns could therefore be employed for research into reciprocal effects of INTERpersonal dissynchrony as well as for training of staff to improve interaction with patients.

This approach on INTRApersonal dissynchrony in ASD potentially has further implications for diagnosis and therapy. In this context, measures of INTRApersonal communication signal coordination could serve as implicit measures that can be used for diagnostic purposes. Implicit diagnostic tools are strongly needed to account for symptomatical heterogeneity in ASD. Subjective observational tools are based on clinical observations or self-report with limited objectivity, especially in adults given behavioral adjustment throughout their lives. Time series data of motion patterns may be used for diagnostic purposes, e.g. supported by machine learning (Georgescu et al., 2019). Our recent work shows that automatized classification of ASD from non-ASD is possible on the mere basis of motion energy assessed using video analysis (ibid.). Specifically motion capture data of INTRApersonal movement parameters is likely to further increase classification power due to richer data retrieval.

An INTRApersonal approach has conceivable implications for the field of robotics in autism research as temporal parameters of signal production may inform models of interactive robotic behavior.

Recent research of human-robot interaction (HRI) with children with ASD revealed positive effects, as robots attract attention and elicit novel behavior while social complexity can be controlled for (Duquette et al., 2008; Scassellati et al., 2012; Srinivasan et al., 2016). The AURORA project (AUtonomous RObotic platform as a Remedial tool for children with Autism) used the robot “Robota,” which resembles a human doll and is able to exhibit interactive movements via video-, speech-, and motion-tracking (Dautenhahn and Billard, 2002). The project showed that “Robota” could serve a mediating role for eliciting joint attention in triadic human-human-robot interactions and elicited spontaneous imitation behavior (Dautenhahn and Werry, 2004; Robins et al., 2004, 2005), thereby potentially reinforcing social skills.

Amplifying joint attention via HRI is of great potential for endorsing social engagement and reciprocity in children with ASD, but the literature is not yet fully convincing. In their study, Anzalone et al. (2014) found that children with ASD were less responsive to joint attention initiatives by the social robot “Nao” and both groups responded less to the robot compared to a human therapist. Another approach investigated interactions of four children with “Nao” and again found mixed results, including facilitated joint attention only for one child (Tapus et al., 2012). Possibly, the design of the social robot with respect to its anthropomorphism may be highly important for eliciting and reinforcing social interactive behavior in children with ASD, for example a realistic eye design in joint attention paradigms (Admoni and Scassellati, 2017; Luria et al., 2018). However, the target of the intervention is not yet properly defined.

Our perspective suggests that individuals with ASD exhibit social interaction in different ways (e.g. peculiar temporal parameters of communicative signal production). Socially interactive robots generally need to be able to recognize communicative signals and exhibit appropriate reactions (Breazeal et al., 2016). Thus, models of robotic behavior could be adjusted to temporal parameters of signal production in ASD in order to reinforce reciprocity, similar to computational approaches in Admoni and Scassellati (2014) or Barakova and Chonnaparamutt (2009). Such an adjustment may enhance compliance and responsiveness of individuals with ASD toward the robotic interaction partner. Furthermore, given that Gowen and Hamilton (2013) suggest intact motor learning in ASD, parameters of typical signal timing may be used for robotic interventions that aim to train proper timing in communicative signal coordination, thereby providing a possible quantitative outcome measure of treatment success. Building upon findings of the AURORA project, the creation of HRI scenarios in which human-like robots serve as interactive tutors for training specific communicative skills (e.g. joint attention) are promising. Creating game-based robot interactions that prompt spontaneous imitation of properly coordinated signals could be a great opportunity to support children in their development of non-verbal skills.

There exist a number of aspects that need to be considered when planning approaches on INTRApersonal dissycnhrony in ASD. One potentially confounding factor when measuring INTRApersonal synchrony lies in the distinction between spontaneously and voluntarily produced behavior, as different cognitive processes are thought to underlie these processes (Frith and Frith, 2008; Torres et al., 2013). Thus, future studies should investigate how INTRApersonal dissynchrony differs under the impact of explicit instructions or implicit and natural task conditions. Furthermore, highly standardized study designs that strictly control sensory surroundings are crucial for studying INTRApersonal synchronization, given deviant sensory processing may contribute to behavioral variability in ASD.

Further research should broaden this approach to other psychiatric disorders that entail INTRApersonal coordination peculiarities like schizophrenia (Walther et al., 2015) or depression (Schrijvers et al., 2008). But especially for ASD, we think that a perspective on INTRApersonal dissynchrony is fundamentally relevant for understanding INTERpersonal difficulties. A quantification of temporally atypical coordination of communication signals in ASD is an important explanatory approach that potentially informs diagnosis as well as intervention programs.

Author Contributions

In accordance with theoretical discussions with CF-W and KV, CB wrote the first manuscript version. AG contributed literature and theoretical ideas. All authors read and modified the manuscript several times. All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

CB was supported by a German Research Foundation grant (granted to CF-W; FA876/3-1). CF-W was supported by a Bavarian Gender Equality Grant.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Admoni, H., and Scassellati, B. (2014). “Toward a data-driven generative behavior model for human-robot interaction,” in Proceedings of the 2014 Workshop on Mobile Augmented Reality and Robotic Technology-Based Systems (Bretton Woods, NH: ACM), 19–20.

Google Scholar

Admoni, H., and Scassellati, B. (2017). Social eye gaze in human-robot interaction: a review. J. Hum. Robot Interact. 6, 25–63. doi: 10.5898/JHRI.6.1.Admoni

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allman, M. J., Yin, B., and Meck, W. H. (2014). “Time in the psychopathological mind,” in Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality, eds V. Arstila and D. Lloyd (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 637–654.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub.

Google Scholar

Anzalone, S. M., Tilmont, E., Boucenna, S., Xavier, J., Jouen, A.-L., Bodeau, N., et al. (2014). How children with autism spectrum disorder behave and explore the 4-dimensional (spatial 3d+ time) environment during a joint attention induction task with a robot. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 8, 814–826. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anzulewicz, A., Sobota, K., and Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2016). Toward the autism motor signature: gesture patterns during smart tablet gameplay identify children with autism. Sci. Rep. 6:31107. doi: 10.1038/srep31107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnal, L. H., and Giraud, A.-L. (2012). Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cognit. Sci. 16, 390–398. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 117, 76–136. doi: 10.1007/BF01837709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baimel, A., Birch, S. A., and Norenzayan, A. (2018). Coordinating bodies and minds: behavioral synchrony fosters mentalizing. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74, 281–290. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barakova, E. I., and Chonnaparamutt, W. (2009). Timing sensory integration. IEEE Robot. Autom. Mag. 16, 51–58. doi: 10.1109/MRA.2009.933626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrow, W. J., Jaworski, M., and Accardo, P. J. (2011). Persistent toe walking in autism. J. Child Neurol. 26, 619–621. doi: 10.1177/0883073810385344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrends, A., Müller, S., and Dziobek, I. (2012). Moving in and out of synchrony: a concept for a new intervention fostering empathy through interactional movement and dance. Arts Psychother. 39, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2012.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bente, G., and Krämer, N. C. (2002). “Virtuelle Gesten: VR-Einsatz in der nonverbalen kommunikationsforschung” in Virtuelle Realitäten, Vol. 5, eds G. Bente, N. C. Krämer, and A. Petersen (Göttingen: Hogrefe), 81–107.

Breazeal, C., Dautenhahn, K., and Kands, T. (2016). “Social robotics.” in Springer Handbook of Robotics, eds B. Siciliano and O. Khatib (New York, NY: Springer International Publishing), 1935–1972.

Google Scholar

Brincker, M., and Torres, E. B. (2013). Noise from the periphery in autism. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:34. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caruana, N., Stieglitz Ham, H., Brock, J., Woolgar, A., Kloth, N., Palermo, R., et al. (2018). Joint attention difficulties in autistic adults: an interactive eye-tracking study. Autism 22, 502–512. doi: 10.1177/1362361316676204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colgan, S. E., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., and Baranek, G. T. (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychol. 12, 307–319. doi: 10.1080/09297040600701360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, J. L., Blakemore, S.-J., and Press, C. (2013). Atypical basic movement kinematics in autism spectrum conditions. Brain 136, 2816–2824. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curioni, A., Minio-Paluello, I., Sacheli, L. M., Candidi, M., and Aglioti, S. M. (2017). Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior. Mol. Autism 8:23. doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0141-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dautenhahn, K., and Billard, A. (2002). “Games children with autism can play with Robota, a humanoid robotic doll,” in Universal Access and Assistive Technology, eds S. Keates, P. M. Langdon, P. J. Clarkson, and P. Robinson (London, UK: Springer), 179–190.

Google Scholar

Dautenhahn, K., and Werry, I. (2004). Towards interactive robots in autism therapy: background, motivation and challenges. Pragmat. Cognit. 12, 1–35. doi: 10.1075/pc.12.1.03dau

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Marchena, A., and Eigsti, I.-M. (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Res. 3, 311–322. doi: 10.1002/aur.159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doumas, M., McKenna, R., and Murphy, B. (2016). Postural control deficits in autism spectrum disorder: the role of sensory integration. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 853–861. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2621-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dufek, J. S., Eggleston, J. D., Harry, J. R., and Hickman, R. A. (2017). A comparative evaluation of gait between children with autism and typically developing matched controls. Med. Sci. 5:1. doi: 10.3390/medsci5010001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duquette, A., Michaud, F., and Mercier, H. (2008). Exploring the use of a mobile robot as an imitation agent with children with low-functioning autism. Autonom. Robots 24, 147–157. doi: 10.1007/s10514-007-9056-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Johnson, M. H., Bird, G., and Press, C. (2016). Interaction takes two: typical adults exhibit mind-blindness towards those with autism spectrum disorder. J. Abn. Psychol. 125:879. doi: 10.1037/abn0000199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggleston, J. D., Harry, J. R., Hickman, R. A., and Dufek, J. S. (2017). Analysis of gait symmetry during over-ground walking in children with autism spectrum disorder. Gait Posture 55, 162–166. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 581–604. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falter, C. M., Braeutigam, S., Nathan, R., Carrington, S., and Bailey, A. J. (2013). Enhanced access to early visual processing of perceptual simultaneity in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1857–1866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1735-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falter, C. M., Elliott, M. A., and Bailey, A. J. (2012a). Enhanced visual temporal resolution in autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE 7:e32774. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falter, C. M., Noreika, V., Wearden, J. H., and Bailey, A. J. (2012b). More consistent, yet less sensitive: interval timing in autism spectrum disorders. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 2093–2107. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.690770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony: biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 16, 340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, P., Frazier, J. A., Cochran, D. M., Mitchell, T., Coleman, C., and Schmidt, R. (2016). Impairments of social motor synchrony evident in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 7:1323. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J. L., Duncan, A., Barnard, H., Richardson, M. J., et al. (2017a). Evaluating the importance of social motor synchronization and motor skill for understanding autism. Autism Res. 10, 1687–1699. doi: 10.1002/aur.1808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, P., Romero, V., Amaral, J. L., Duncan, A., Barnard, H., Richardson, M. J., et al. (2017b). Social motor synchronization: insights for understanding social behavior in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 2092–2107. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3124-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2008). Implicit and explicit processes in social cognition. Neuron 60, 503–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fulceri, F., Tonacci, A., Lucaferro, A., Apicella, F., Narzisi, A., Vincenti, G., et al. (2018). Interpersonal motor coordination during joint actions in children with and without autism spectrum disorder: the role of motor information. Res. Dev. Disabil. 80, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgescu, A. L., Koehler, J. C., Weiske, J., Vogeley, K., Koutsouleris, N., and Falter-Wagner, C. (2019). Machine learning approaches to study social interaction difficulties in ASD. Front. Robot. AI.

Georgescu, A. L., Kuzmanovic, B., Roth, D., Bente, G., and Vogeley, K. (2014). The use of virtual characters to assess and train non-verbal communication in high-functioning autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:807. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgescu, A. L., Kuzmanovic, B., Schilbach, L., Tepest, R., Kulbida, R., Bente, G., et al. (2013). Neural correlates of “social gaze” processing in high-functioning autism under systematic variation of gaze duration. Neuroimage Clin. 3, 340–351. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, D. A., Glazebrook, C. M., Studenka, B. E., and Lyons, J. (2013). Motor interactions with another person: do individuals with autism spectrum disorder plan ahead? Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:23. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gowen, E., and Hamilton, A. (2013). Motor abilities in autism: a review using a computational context. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 323–344. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gowen, E., and Miall, R. C. (2005). Behavioural aspects of cerebellar function in adults with asperger syndrome. Cerebellum 4, 279–289. doi: 10.1080/14734220500355332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haken, H., Kelso, J. A., and Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol. Cybernet. 51, 347–356. doi: 10.1007/BF00336922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cognit. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaksson, S., Salomäki, S., Tuominen, J., Arstila, V., Falter-Wagner, C. M., and Noreika, V. (2018). Is there a generalized timing impairment in autism spectrum disorders across time scales and paradigms? J. Psychiatr. Res. 99, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. R., Happé, F., Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., et al. (2009). Auditory discrimination and auditory sensory behaviours in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47, 2850–2858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, W., and Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504:427. doi: 10.1038/nature12715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaur, M., Srinivasan, S. M., and Bhat, A. N. (2018). Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony between children with and without autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Res. Dev. Disabil. 72, 79–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kindregan, D., Gallagher, L., and Gormley, J. (2015). Gait deviations in children with autism spectrum disorders: a review. Autism Res. Treat. 2015:741480. doi: 10.1155/2015/741480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., and Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition 146, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Soc. Psychol. Q. 42, 66–70. doi: 10.2307/3033875

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakin, J. L., and Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol. Sci. 14, 334–339. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 205–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, T., Vlaskamp, B. N., Kasparbauer, A.-M., Mörtl, A., and Hirche, S. (2014). Dyadic movement synchronization while performing incongruent trajectories requires mutual adaptation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:461. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, M., Forlizzi, J., and Hodgins, J. (2018). “The effects of eye design on the perception of social robots,” in 2018 27th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) (Nanjing: IEEE), 1032–1037.

Google Scholar

Madipakkam, A. R., Rothkirch, M., Dziobek, I., and Sterzer, P. (2017). Unconscious avoidance of eye contact in autism spectrum disorder. Sci. Rep. 7:13378. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13945-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Helt, M., Verbalis, A. D., Schmidt, R., et al. (2013). Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:4. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGrath, J. E., and Kelly, J. R. (1986). Time and Human Interaction: Toward a Social Psychology of Time. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Noel, J.-P., De Niear, M. A., Lazzara, N. S., and Wallace, M. T. (2018). Uncoupling between multisensory temporal function and nonverbal turn-taking in autism spectrum disorder. IEEE Trans. Cognit. Dev. Syst. 10, 973–982. doi: 10.1109/TCDS.2017.2778141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, X., and Hamilton, A. F. C. (2018). Why and how to use virtual reality to study human social interaction: the challenges of exploring a new research landscape. Br. J. Psychol. 109, 395–417. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parma, V., and de Marchena, A. B. (2015). Motor signatures in autism spectrum disorder: the importance of variability. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1081–1084. doi: 10.1152/jn.00647.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramseyer, F., and Tschacher, W. (2008). “Synchrony in dyadic psychotherapy sessions,” in Simultaneity: Temporal Structures and Observer Perspectives, eds S. Vrobel, O. E. Rössler, and T. Marks-Tarlow (Singapore: World Scientific), 329–347.

Google Scholar

Ramseyer, F., and Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 79:284. doi: 10.1037/a0023419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R., and Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 867–891. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, B., Dautenhahn, K., Te Boekhorst, R., and Billard, A. (2005). Robotic assistants in therapy and education of children with autism: can a small humanoid robot help encourage social interaction skills? Univ. Access Inform. Soc. 4, 105–120. doi: 10.1007/s10209-005-0116-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, B., Dickerson, P., Stribling, P., and Dautenhahn, K. (2004). Robot-mediated joint attention in children with autism: a case study in robot-human interaction. Interact. Stud. 5, 161–198. doi: 10.1075/is.5.2.02rob

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero, V., Amaral, J., Fitzpatrick, P. A., Schmidt, R. C., and Richardson, M. (2015). “Capturing social motor coordination: a comparison of the microsoft kinect, video-motion analysis and the polhemus latus motion tracking system,” in Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Pasadena, CA).

Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., Faso, D. J., Nugent, J., Lovell, S., Kennedy, D. P., and Grossman, R. B. (2017). Neurotypical peers are less willing to interact with those with autism based on thin slice judgments. Sci. Rep. 7:40700. doi: 10.1038/srep40700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scassellati, B., Admoni, H., and Matarić, M. (2012). Robots for use in autism research. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 14, 275–94. doi: 10.1146/annurev-bioeng-071811-150036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharoun, S. M., and Bryden, P. J. (2016). Anticipatory planning in children with autism spectrum disorder: an assessment of independent and joint action tasks. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 10:29. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. C., and Richardson, M. J. (2008). “Dynamics of interpersonal coordination,” in Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics, eds A. Fuchs and V. Jirsa (Heidelberg: Springer), 281–308.

Google Scholar

Schrijvers, D., Hulstijn, W., and Sabbe, B. G. (2008). Psychomotor symptoms in depression: a diagnostic, pathophysiological and therapeutic tool. J. Affect. Disord. 109, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, C., Bente, G., Gawronski, A., Schilbach, L., and Vogeley, K. (2010). Responses to nonverbal behaviour of dynamic virtual characters in high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 100–111. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0843-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A., and Johnson, M. H. (2009). The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development. Trends Cognit. Sci. 13, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, S. M., Eigsti, I.-M., Gifford, T., and Bhat, A. N. (2016). The effects of embodied rhythm and robotic interventions on the spontaneous and responsive verbal communication skills of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a further outcome of a pilot randomized controlled trial. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 27, 73–87. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studenka, B. E., Gillam, S. L., Hartzheim, D., and Gillam, R. B. (2017). Motor and verbal perspective taking in children with autism spectrum disorder: changes in social interaction with people and tools. Res. Dev. Disabil. 66, 64–79. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szelag, E., Kowalska, J., Galkowski, T., and Pöppel, E. (2004). Temporal processing deficits in high-functioning children with autism. Br. J. Psychol. 95, 269–282. doi: 10.1348/0007126041528167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapus, A., Peca, A., Aly, A., Pop, C., Jisa, L., Pintea, S., et al. (2012). Children with autism social engagement in interaction with nao, an imitative robot: a series of single case experiments. Interact. Stud. 13, 315–347. doi: 10.1075/is.13.3.01tap

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tickle-Degnen, L., and Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychol. Inq. 1, 285–293. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, E. B., Brincker, M., Isenhower III, R. W., Yanovich, P., Stigler, K. A., Nurnberger Jr., J. I., et al. (2013). Autism: the micro-movement perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:32. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vacharkulksemsuk, T., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 399–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., and DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 693–695. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, D., Falter-Wagner, C. M., Schoofs, T., Krämer, K., Kupke, C., and Vogeley, K. (2018a). Interrupted time experience in autism spectrum disorder: empirical evidence from content analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 22–33. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3771-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, D. H., Falter-Wagner, C. M., Schoofs, T., Krämer, K., Kupke, C., and Vogeley, K. (2018b). Flow and structure of time experience–concept, empirical validation and implications for psychopathology. Phenomenol. Cognit. Sci. 18, 1–24. doi: 10.1007/s11097-018-9573-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther, S., Stegmayer, K., Sulzbacher, J., Vanbellingen, T., Müri, R., Strik, W., et al. (2015). Nonverbal social communication and gesture control in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 338–345. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 593–602. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: human-human interaction, INTERpersonal synchrony, INTRApersonal synchrony, timing, non-verbal behavior, autism spectrum disorder

Citation: Bloch C, Vogeley K, Georgescu AL and Falter-Wagner CM (2019) INTRApersonal Synchrony as Constituent of INTERpersonal Synchrony and Its Relevance for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Robot. AI 6:73. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2019.00073

Received: 22 March 2019; Accepted: 30 July 2019;
Published: 20 August 2019.

Edited by:

Cigdem Beyan, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Chung Hyuk Park, George Washington University, United States
Nicola Vanello, University of Pisa, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Bloch, Vogeley, Georgescu and Falter-Wagner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carola Bloch, carola.bloch@med.uni-muenchen.de; Christine M. Falter-Wagner, christine.falter@med.uni-muenchen.de

Download