Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 December 2018
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Refugee Mental Health View all 15 articles

A Longitudinal Investigation of Moral Injury Appraisals Amongst Treatment-Seeking Refugees

  • 1School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2Department of Consultation-Liason Psychiatry and Psychosomatic Medicine, University Hospital Zurich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

There is currently an unprecedented number of forcibly displaced people worldwide. Understanding psychological mechanisms that contribute to the mental health of refugees and asylum-seekers is important for informing the development of effective interventions for these populations. Moral injury appraisals represent an important potential cognitive mechanism that may contribute to psychological symptoms following exposure to persecution, war, and displacement. In the current study, we investigated the longitudinal association between moral injury appraisals related to one's own perceived transgressions (moral injury-self), others' perceived transgressions (moral injury-other), and PTSD and depression symptoms. Participants in this study were 134 refugees receiving treatment at two outpatient clinics in Switzerland who completed survey measures investigating these concepts. Of these, 71 were followed up 2 to 4 years later. Path analyses revealed that greater depression symptoms were associated with subsequent increases in moral injury-self appraisals (β = 0.25, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [0.11, 0.43], p = 0.002). In contrast, greater moral injury-self appraisals were associated with subsequent decreases in PTSD symptoms (β = −0.23, SE = 0.11, 95% CI = [−0.44, −0.31], p = 0.035). Findings suggest that different types of moral injury appraisals may be associated with differential psychological outcomes. These results have important potential implications for policy and treatment of refugees and asylum-seekers, highlighting the importance of targeting cognitive factors in the maintenance and treatment of psychological distress, and considering the post-migration context when working with refugees.

Introduction

The number of people who have fled their homes as a result of war and persecution internationally is currently over 68 million (1). Compared to the general population in host countries, refugees report elevated rates of psychological disorders including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression (2, 3). The development of targeted interventions to reduce the psychological burden of refugees would be greatly aided by understanding the processes underlying psychopathology in these groups. To date, however, relatively little is known about the psychological mechanisms underpinning the mental health of refugees.

One psychological mechanism that has been consistently demonstrated to impact posttraumatic mental health both cross-sectionally and longitudinally is cognitive appraisals (47). Models of PTSD posit that the way in which the trauma survivor interprets their experiences, symptoms, and the broader context influences the development of symptoms in the aftermath of a potentially traumatic event [PTE; (810)]. The types of PTEs to which refugees are typically exposed (e.g., prolonged, repeated, human-instigated) are likely to engender cognitive change regarding the self and broader society, which may, in turn, contribute to psychopathology (11, 12). Despite a strong theoretical rationale, however, there is relatively little available evidence regarding cognitive appraisals that may arise from refugee experiences nor how these interact with mental health.

One framework that may be useful in conceptualizing the cognitive impact of the refugee experience is moral injury (13, 14). Moral injury can be defined as “the lasting psychological, biological, spiritual, behavioral, and social impact of …… bearing witness to acts that transgress deeply held moral beliefs and expectations” (15). This construct was initially developed to describe the effects of being exposed to moral transgressions in the context of combat (15); however, it is increasingly being applied to non-military settings (1620). While moral injury is often conceptualized in terms of the nature of a specific event (e.g., killing in the context of warfare, betrayal by a superior officer) (15, 21), we posit that it can also be considered as a cognitive interpretation relating to refugee experiences, whereby the appraisal of an experience as morally injurious might give rise to psychological symptoms (13). This is broadly consistent with the construct of “moral pain,” proposed to describe the cognitive and emotional response to potentially morally injurious events (22). For example, in a refugee context, one individual being forced to leave behind a family member may consider this action a serious transgression of importan values; while to another, it may be perceived as a necessary step in moving one's immediate family to safety. Consistent with cognitive models of post-trauma mental health, these appraisals may then give rise to different psychological outcomes, with the former potentially contributing to psychological symptoms, and the latter to better post-trauma adaptation (8).

Research conducted in military settings has focused on two types of moral injury that may be especially relevant to refugees- that related to one's own actions (which we term moral injury-self) and that related to the actions of others (moral injury-other) (23, 24). Mixed findings have emerged regarding the association between these two types of moral injury and psychological outcomes in military samples. Currier et al. (24) found that both types of moral injury were associated with greater PTSD and depression symptoms. Although Bryan et al. (23) also found that moral injury-other was linked to higher levels of PTSD symptoms, only moral injury-self was associated with higher levels of hopelessness and anger. Held et al. (25) found that negative beliefs about others, negative beliefs about the self and self-blame mediated the association between moral injury-self (but not moral injury-other) and post-traumatic symptomatology in treatment-seeking veterans. Overall the differential association between different types of morally injurious events, appraisals, and psychological outcomes remains unclear.

To date, there have been two empirical investigations of moral injury appraisals in refugees. The first found that higher levels of moral injury-other were associated with greater PTSD and depression symptoms, explosive anger, and lower mental health-related quality of life in treatment-seeking refugees, over, and above the impact of PTE exposure, post-migration living difficulties (14). This study did not investigate moral injury-self. The second study investigated both moral injury-self and moral injury-other in a sample of 222 resettled refugees from diverse backgrounds (13). This study found that moral injury-self and moral injury-other were both predicted by greater trauma exposure, and associated with more severe anger and depression. While moral injury -other was associated with greater PTSD symptoms across all symptom clusters, moral injury -self was related to lower intrusive symptoms. Taken together, these findings indicate that that moral injury -self and moral injury -other may represent distinct constructs in a refugee context, and are differentially associated with psychological outcomes. To date, however, moral injury appraisals in refugees have only been investigated cross-sectionally. Without understanding how these types of moral injury appraisals relate to one another and psychological symptoms over time, it is difficult to ascertain the mechanistic role they may play in psychopathology in refugee groups.

The current study builds upon the Nickerson et al. study (14) to conduct the first longitudinal investigation of moral injury in refugees, with participants in this study being assessed 2 to 4 years after the first study. In addition, in this study both MI-other and MI-self were examined to elucidate their interrelationship as well as their relationship with important psychological outcomes. Understanding how moral injury appraisals might change over time, and their association to psychological symptoms, affords a unique opportunity to investigate the potentially mechanistic role of moral injury in the development and maintenance of psychological disorders.

In this study, we employed path analyses to investigate the association between moral injury appraisals and mental health over time. We hypothesized that:

(1) Greater exposure to PTEs and post-migration living difficulties would predict higher moral injury-self and moral injury-other appraisals. We based this prediction on the strong association between PTEs, post-migration living difficulties and psychological symptoms in the literature (26, 27), and the findings from Hoffman et al. (13) that greater trauma exposure was associated with higher moral injury-self and other appraisals.

(2) Higher moral injury-other appraisals at Time 1 would be associated with higher moral injury-self appraisals at Time 2, and vice versa. We based this prediction on findings from Hoffman et al. (13) that MI-self and MI-other were positively related cross-sectionally.

(3) Higher moral injury-other appraisals at Time 1 would be associated with greater PTSD and depression symptoms at Time 2. We based this prediction on findings from both Nickerson et al. (14) and Hoffman et al. (13) that MI-other was related to higher PTSD and depression cross-sectionally.

(4) Higher moral injury-self appraisals at Time 1 would be associated with greater depression (but not PTSD symptoms) at Time 2. We based this prediction on the finding from Hoffman et al. (13) that moral injury-self was associated with greater depression cross-sectionally, but not related to PTSD symptom sub-scales of avoidance, negative alterations in cognition and mood or hyperarousal.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants in this study were 134 refugees who, at the first time-point, were receiving psychological treatment at two outpatient clinics in Zurich and Bern, Switzerland. Exclusion criteria included current psychotic symptoms, severe dissociative symptoms, or active suicidality, which were assessed upon intake into the clinic by the treating psychiatrist or psychologist. At Time 1, participants in this study comprised 105 males (78.4%), with a mean age of 42.44 years (SD = 9.8%). Marital status of the sample was as follows: single n = 40 (29.9%), in a relationship/married n = 78 (58.2%) and divorced or widowed n = 16 (11.9%). The highest level of educational attainment was as follows: not completed primary school n = 17 (12.7%), completed primary school n = 43 (22.1%), completed high school n = 31 (23.1%), competed a Bachelor's Degree or technical college diploma n = 22 (23.9%) and completed a postgraduate degree n = 10 (7.5%). The employment status of the sample was as follows: engaged in full-time employment n = 9 (6.7%), engaged in part-time employment n = 17 (12.6%), unemployed n = 82 (61.2%) and retired or homemaker n = 23 (17.1%). At Time 1, participants had been in Switzerland for a mean of 9.01 years (SD = 6.7). Participants at Time 1 were from a number of ethnic backgrounds, including Turkey (n = 71, 53%, with N = 58, 43.3% being Kurdish), Iran (n = 15, 12%), Sri Lanka (n = 11, 8%), Bosnia (n = 6, 5%), Iraq (n = 6, 5%), Afghanistan (n = 5, 4%), and other (n = 20, 13%). At Time 1, participants had received treatment for a mean of 37.67 months (SD = 28.5). Inclusion criteria for this study comprised: (a) aged 18 years or older, and (b) speaking one of the study languages (German, Turkish, Arabic, Farsi, or Tamil).

Time 2 data collection commenced 2 years after Time 1, and 71 participants completed the second survey a mean of 2.81 (SD = 0.42) years after the Time 1. The distribution of ethnicities at Time 2 were as follows: Turkey (n = 42, 59.2%), Iran (n = 6, 8.5%), Sri Lanka (n = 6, 8.5%), Iraq (n = 4, 5.6%), and other (n = 13, 18.3%). Differences in the groups who completed the survey at Time 1 only, compared to those who completed the survey at Time 1 and Time 2 are presented in Table 1. Compared to those who completed only Time 1, participants who completed both time-points were significantly older [t(132) = −3.13, p = 0.002], had lived in Switzerland longer [t(132) = −2.65, p = 0.01] and included proportionally more males than females [x2(1) = 4.24, p = 0.04], than participants who only completed Time 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Differences between participants who completed time 1 and time 2 survey measures, and participants who completed only time 1 survey measures (t-tests).

Measures

Gold-standard translation and back-translation procedures were implemented for measures in this study (28). Measures were first translated from English into the study languages (German, Turkish, Arabic, Farsi, or Tamil) by accredited translators, and were then translated back into English by translators who had not seen the original versions of the questionnaires. Discrepancies, in the form of minor language inconsistencies, were rectified jointly by the research team and bilingual individuals with experience working with mental health-related information.

Exposure to Potentially Traumatic Events

We indexed exposure to PTEs using a measure developed for this study. The trauma event lists of two standardized questionnaires, the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (29) and the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (30) were amalgamated, and the final scale comprised 23 items which indexed exposure to PTEs typically experienced by refugees. Example events included witnessing the death of loved ones, lack of food and water, and torture. PTE exposure was indexed by a count of the number of types of traumatic events experienced by each participant.

Post-migration Stressors

Post-migration stressors were assessed using the Post-Migration Living Difficulties Checklist (31, 32), which was adapted to the Swiss context. This scale comprised 17 items detailing common difficulties experienced by refugees in their host society (e.g., language difficulties, financial difficulties). Participants indicated the extent to which each of these stressors had been a problem over the past 12 months on a five point scale (0 = not a problem to 4 = a very serious problem). Items that scored 2 (a moderately serious problem) or more were considered significant stressors, and a total count of living difficulties was derived.

PTSD Symptoms

We measured PTSD symptoms using the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (30). As data collection commenced prior to the release of the updated version of this scale, four items were added to encompass new DSM-5 criteria for PTSD and one item from the obsolete DSM-IV criteria removed, resulting in a 20-item scale (33). Participants rated items on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all/ only once to 3 = five or more times a week/ almost always). We used a total sum score to represent PTSD symptom severity. This scale has been used to assess PTSD in refugees (3436), and has strong psychometric properties (37). The internal consistency for this scale was α = 0.94.

Depression Symptoms

Depression was measured using the 15-item depression subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (38). Items were measured on a 4-point scale (1 = not at all to 4 = extremely), and a total sum score used to represent depression symptom severity. The HSCL has been used on numerous occasions with refugees (36, 39), and has strong psychometric properties (38). Internal consistency for this scale was α = 0.89.

Moral Injury Appraisals in Refugees

A 6-item scale indexing moral injury was developed for this study (14). This scale comprised two subscales, namely moral injury relating to one's own actions (MI-Self) and moral injury relating to others' actions (MI-Other). All items were rated on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all, 3 = very much). For each subscale, a mean of items was taken to represent the subscale score. The MI-Self subscale comprised two items relating to the individual being troubled because of actions they took or failed to take that transgressed their morals. As Cronbach's alpha provides biased estimates for reliability for two-item scales, a Spearman-Brown coefficient was calculated for this scale, ρ = 0.71 (40). The MI-Other subscale comprised four items relating to the individual being troubled by the actions of others that transgressed their morals. The internal consistency for this scale was α = 0.77.

Procedure

Ethics approval was received for this study from the Ethics Committees of the Cantons of Zurich and Bern. All eligible individuals who were receiving treatment at the Zurich and Bern clinics were invited to take part in the study by their treating therapist or a study team member. Participants were informed that their decision to participate or not would have no impact on the treatment they received at these clinics. Participants attended a 1 to 2 h long research session where they first provided written informed consent. Participants completed questionnaires in their own language using a therapist-assisted computer-based assessment tool (MAPSS) (41). Self-report questionnaires were presented on a tablet in written and auditory form. Psychiatrists, clinical psychologists or masters-level students of clinical psychology supervised the research session. Interpreters were used for sessions where required. Participants were reimbursed CHF 40 (approx. USD 40) for participation. Two to four years after completion of the first session, participants were contacted by the study team to invite them to complete the second survey. The same procedure was followed for the second session.

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were generated using SPSS Version 23.0. Path analysis, conducted in Mplus version 7.4, was used to investigate the relationship between demographics, refugee experiences, moral injury subscales, and PTSD, and depression symptoms at Time 1 and Time 2. There was 47.0% attrition at Time 2 due to participants being uncontactable (n = 44) or declining to participate (n = 19). A full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimator was implemented. This estimator is ideal for models where there is substantial missing data as it uses other variables in the model to account for missing values (42). Variables on which individuals who dropped out of the study differed from those who completed both time-points were included in the model to create unbiased parameter estimates. The model was initially specified as follows: Step 1: age, gender, length of time in Switzerland, trauma exposure and post-migration living difficulties (all measured at Time 1); Step 2: (MI-self, MI-other, PTSD symptoms, and depression symptoms (measured at Time 1); Step 3: MI-self, MI-other, PTSD symptoms, and depression symptoms. All variables were permitted to influence variables at the subsequent step. Residual error variance was allowed to covary between variables entered at the same step.

Next, model trimming was employed and paths with p < 0.10 were removed from the model. Indirect effects were tested using bootstrapping (500 draws), which yields 95% confidence intervals. Model fit criteria included a non-significant chi-square test χ2 test, a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) <0.06, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥0.90, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) ≥0.90, and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) <0.05 (43).

Results

Exposure to Potentially Traumatic Events and Post-migration Living Difficulties

Participants had experienced a mean of 12.25 types of PTEs (SD = 4.54). The most frequent types of events experienced were torture (85.1%), imprisonment (76.9%), and enforced isolation from others (76.9%). Participants had also experienced a mean of 9.77 types of living difficulties (SD = 4.16). The most frequent types of living difficulties experienced were loneliness, boredom, or isolation (84.3%), worries about family back home (80.6%), and being unable to return to their home country in an emergency (75.4%).

Path Analysis

The initial model in which all paths were specified yielded adequate fit χ(20)2 = 23.90, p = 0.247, RMSEA = 0.04 (95% CI = 0.00 to 0.09), CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.95, SRMR = 0.05. After the removal of paths with p < 0.10, good model fit was retained χ(31)2 = 29.63, p = 0.537, RMSEA = 0.00 (95% CI = 0.00 to 0.06), CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.01, SRMR = 0.06. Standardized parameter estimates of direct effects are presented in Figure 1 and Table 2. Standardized indirect effects are presented in Table 3.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Longitudinal association between refugee experiences, moral injury appraisals and psychological symptoms in traumatized refugees. LDC, Postmigration living difficulties; PTE, Potentially Traumatic Events; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2; MI-O, Moral injury Other; MI- S, Moral Injury Self. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Standardized coefficients for direct effects in path model.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized indirect effects for path model.

Hypothesis 1: Association Between Refugee Experiences and MI Appraisals

Greater exposure to PTEs and living difficulties at Time 1 significantly predicted higher MI-Other but not MI-Self appraisals at Time 2.

Hypothesis 2: Association Between MI Appraisals at Time 1 and Time 2

Higher MI-Other appraisals at Time 1 were associated with higher MI-Self appraisals at Time 2. Higher MI-Self appraisals at Time 1 were associated with higher MI-Other appraisals at Time 2.

Hypothesis 3: Association Between MI-Other Appraisals at Time 1 and Psychological Symptoms at Time 2

Contrary to our hypotheses, MI-Other appraisals at Time 1 were not associated with psychological symptoms at Time 2.

Hypothesis 4: Association Between MI-Self Appraisals at Time 1 and Psychological Symptoms at Time 2

Higher MI-Self appraisals at Time 1 were associated with lower PTSD symptoms at Time 2. Notably, higher depression symptoms at Time 1 were associated with greater MI-Self appraisals at Time 2.

Indirect Effects

There was a significant indirect effect of trauma on MI-Self appraisals at Time 2 via MI-Other appraisals at Time 1. There was a significant indirect effect of living difficulties on PTSD at Time 2 via depression symptoms at Time 1. There was a significant indirect effect of living difficulties on MI-Other appraisals at Time 2 via MI-Other appraisals at Time 1. There were significant indirect effects of living difficulties on MI-Self appraisals on Time 2 via MI-Other appraisals at Time 1, and depression symptoms at Time 1.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study represents the first longitudinal investigation of moral injury appraisals amongst refugees. The key finding from this study was that moral injury-other and moral injury-self appraisals were differentially associated with psychological outcomes in treatment-seeking refugees. Specifically, we found that depression at baseline predicted greater moral injury-self appraisals at Time 2, and moral injury-self appraisals at Time 1 predicted lower PTSD symptoms at Time 2. These findings suggest that higher depression symptoms may give rise to increased appraisals that the individual has transgressed their own morals. This is consistent with cognitive models of depression that highlight the powerful role of negative cognitions about the self in this disorder (44, 45). Our finding, that moral injury appraisals at Time 1 did not predict depression at Time 2, is contrary to traditional cognitive models of depression which have posited that maladaptive cognitions contribute to the development of psychological symptoms (44, 46). Other theoretical and empirical accounts, however, have suggested that the relationship between depression and negative cognitions is bidirectional (47), or that negative cognitions arise from, rather than precede, depression symptoms (48, 49). Accordingly, theorists have posited a reciprocal relationship between negative cognitions and depression, in which a cycle of self-recrimination, rumination, and depressed mood prolongs, and increases symptoms (5052). Consistent with this, in a trial testing prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD, changes in depression symptoms preceded changes in self-blame cognitions (53). In this study, participants with high levels of depression may have engaged in rumination regarding their previous actions, thus intensifying their distress about perceived moral transgressions. It is important to note, however, that participants in this study were treatment-seeking, and thus these processes may have been influenced by treatment strategies and/or symptom reduction. Further research is required to elucidate the relationship between depressed mood, moral injury-self appraisals, and other cognitive processes such as rumination.

Our results revealed that higher levels of moral injury-self cognitions were associated with subsequent decreases in PTSD symptoms in this sample of treatment-seeking refugees. This is partly consistent with the findings from Hoffman et al. (13) that higher levels of moral injury-self cognitions were associated with lower intrusive (but not avoidance, cognitions and mood and hyperarousal symptoms). This is in contrast to cross-sectional research with military samples indicating that moral injury related to one's own actions is linked to higher PTSD symptoms (15, 21), although it should be noted that Bryan et al. (23) found that moral injury-self was not associated with PTSD in a military sample. While cognitive models of PTSD posit that higher levels of negative trauma-related appraisals give rise to more severe PTSD symptoms (810), evidence regarding the relationship between PTSD and negative appraisals of one's own actions is mixed. Studies conducted with survivors of various types of trauma have documented both a positive relationship between self-blame and PTSD symptoms (5456), and no relationship between these variables (5759). Further, consistent with the current findings, Startup et al. (60) found that higher levels of self-blame were associated with lower PTSD symptomatology in survivors of various types of traumatic events. The present findings can be interpreted in multiple ways. Firstly, as our sample were treatment-seeking, it may be that the observed reduction in PTSD symptom severity between Time 1 and 2 was related to psychotherapy received during this period. For example, if participants reported greater cognitions related to moral injury-self at Time 1, this may have led the therapist to focus the treatment on these appraisals, potentially producing a flow-on effect that resulted in reduced PTSD symptoms at Time 2. These findings should be replicated in a non-treatment seeking sample to determine whether these results represent an artifact of treatment. Second, it is possible that these findings point to a specific etiological pathway for the development and maintenance of PTSD. Models of PTSD highlight the protective role of perceived control during the traumatic event against the development of PTSD symptoms (8, 61, 62). Accordingly, in a study with accidental injury survivors, greater perceived responsibility for a traumatic event was associated with lower PTSD symptoms 6 months later (63). It is possible that, in this study, the appraisal that one's actions were transgressive was related to the perception that one could have acted differently during the event. This may have led to greater perceived control during the situation, and lower resulting levels of PTSD symptoms. Alternatively, the appraisal that one has acted against one's moral code may lead the traumatic memory to be experienced in a manner that is qualitatively distinctive. For example, individuals with higher levels of moral injury-self appraisals may experience lower levels of avoidance due to intentionally engaging with the traumatic memory. This may lead to the memory being processed differently, contributing to a unique symptom presentation. These explanations are highly speculative, however, and further longitudinal and experimental work is required to replicate this finding, and determine the mechanism by which it operates.

Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that moral injury-other appraisals did not predict subsequent psychological symptoms, despite being associated with PTSD and depression at baseline in both this study and our previous investigations (13, 14). These results suggest that the extent to which an individual perceives that they transgressed their own moral code may have a stronger association with PTSD and depression over time than the perception that the individual was transgressed against. One possible explanation for this result is that, while the appraisal that others have violated one's moral code is associated with high levels of concurrent symptomatology, this relationship may weaken over time. Another possibility is that moral injury-other appraisals are associated with differential reactions in the long-term, for example, higher levels of humiliation, hostility or mistrust of others. This is supported by findings from a longitudinal study conducted with survivors of persecution and war in Timor-Leste, which indicated that a sense of injustice relating to past events strongly predicted explosive anger reactions (in addition to PTSD and general psychological distress) (64). Further, Stein et al. (65) found that experiencing morally injurious events perpetrated by others was associated with feelings of humiliation in active duty service members. More research is required to determine the longitudinal psychological and behavioral effects of perceived moral transgressions enacted by others in refugees.

Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that moral injury-other, and moral injury-self were positively associated over time. Accordingly, the extent to which an individual perceives that they has transgressed and been transgressed against appear to be related, and contribute to the strengthening of these beliefs over time. This is consistent with the findings of Hoffman et al. (13), and research indicating that negative cognitions relating to traumatic events cluster together (6668).

Also in accordance with our hypothesis, we found that greater moral injury-other appraisals were associated with greater exposure to PTEs and post-migration living difficulties. These findings are consistent with a large body of literature which shows a dose-response relationship between PTEs and post-migration living difficulties and negative mental health outcomes in refugees (26, 27, 69). This finding suggests that the more types of traumatic events to which an individual is exposed, the greater they sense of having been transgressed against. This is consistent with results suggesting that greater exposure to trauma was associated with greater perceptions of injustice in survivors of persecution and war (64). The positive association between exposure to post-migration living difficulties and moral injury-other appraisals is notable, as it provides preliminary evidence to suggest that moral injury appraisals may be associated with experiences beyond those that are traumatic in nature. This may be especially relevant to the case of refugees. For example, a refugee may, upon fleeing his or her country, hope that they will be warmly received into the host society, is able to obtain permanent residency, find employment, and support they family financially. If, for instance, the individual is subject to discrimination, temporary visa status, and is unable to get qualifications recognized, they may feel that the host society or representative government has acted in a way that transgresses his or her moral code, giving rise to moral injury-self appraisals. Further research is required to elucidate this finding. However, these results indicate that it may be important to look beyond traditionally conceptualized traumatic events when considering moral injury in refugees.

Limitations

The current study was subject to several limitations. First and foremost, the measure used to index moral injury was preliminary in that it has not been validated with other populations. The two-item scale representing MI-Self was also limited in the extent to which it comprehensively captured MI-self appraisals, and the use of a two-item measure is not optimal from a statistical perspective at its reliability cannot be ascertained. Future research focusing on the development of a psychometrically valid measure of moral injury appraisals relating to refugees would be useful. Second, our sample size and, in particular, the number of individuals followed-up from the initial time-point, was small. This may have limited our statistical power in uncovering relationships between variables. Third, participants in this study were treatment-seeking. While this provides an ecologically-valid sample of individuals likely to experience psychological distress arising from moral injury, it is not possible to determine whether the relationships reported in this study were due to naturalistic change over time, as they were likely also influenced by therapeutic processes. Future longitudinal investigation of these variables in a community sample would be valuable. Fourth, there were a number of potential confounding factors that were not indexed in the current study, including the use of psychotropic medication which may have affected psychological symptoms, the varying length of time between baseline assessment and follow-up, which may have influenced results, and the type and frequency of psychological interventions received by participants during the follow-up period. Fifth, due to the small sample size it was not possible to evaluate the psychometric properties of each scale for each language group. Finally, there were a number of constructs of interest that were not investigated in this study. Examining the extent to which psychological processes such as rumination are related to moral injury, and potential outcomes beyond psychological symptoms (i.e., trust, hostility, humiliation) represents a future research direction.

Clinical and Policy Implications

Results from this study may have important treatment and policy implications. The finding that moral injury-self appraisals were negatively associated with subsequent PTSD symptoms, and positively associated with prior depression symptoms raises important clinical considerations. It may be that these appraisals do not respond optimally to front-line exposure-based interventions for PTSD. This is consistent with findings from a clinical trial indicating that self-blame cognitions show less change during trauma-focused treatments than other cognitions (70), although another study found that reductions in depression symptoms in prolonged exposure therapy preceded self-blame cognitions (53). Nevertheless, moral injury-self appraisals may be more responsive to cognitive-based interventions for depression or PTSD, or specific treatments for moral injury adapted for refugees [i.e., Adaptive Disclosure (71, 72)]. Given the longitudinal association between MI-self appraisals and depression symptoms in this study, it would be useful to investigate whether specific treatment strategies are more effective in reducing moral injury-self and moral injury-other appraisals. From a policy perspective, it is important to note the association between post-migration living difficulties and moral injury-other appraisals in this study. There is a growing body of evidence highlighting the pivotal role of post-migration stressors in influencing outcomes in resettled refugees [for a review, see Li et al. (26)]. The finding that greater exposure to these stressors is associated with a higher perception of having been transgressed against suggests that it is important for host governments to develop supportive policies and provide adequate resources for refugees upon settlement to promote better adaptation to the new country and positive social outcomes.

Conclusions

The current study represents, to our knowledge, the first longitudinal investigation of two types of moral injury appraisals (namely moral injury related to one's own and others' transgressions) in refugees. Findings indicated that these two types of appraisals are distinctive in terms of their association to refugee experiences, and to psychological outcomes over time. This research provides preliminary evidence that moral injury may be a useful framework for conceptualizing the cognitive experience of individuals who have been exposed to trauma, persecution, displacement and post-migration stress. Further research is required to elucidate the association between moral injury appraisals and psychological, social, and behavioral outcomes amongst refugees.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Cantons of Zurich (Project Nr. KEK-ZH-Nr. 2011–0495) and Bern (KEK-BE_Nr. 152/12), Switzerland, and was conducted in compliance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). All participants provided written informed consent prior to study participation.

Author Contributions

AN, MS, US, and RB were involved in the conception of the study, the interpretation of the data and the drafting, and revision of the manuscript. JH was involved in the interpretation of the data and drafting and revision of the manuscript. NM was involved in the conception of the study, data collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, and the drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Parrotia Foundation, the Swiss Federal Office for Migration (3a-12-0495), the Swiss Foundation for the Promotion of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, and the Swiss Federal Office for Health (12.005187). The funders played no role in the design and conduct (i.e., data collection, analysis, interpretation) of this study.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. UNHCR. Global Trends in Forced Displacement in 2017. Geneva: UNHCR (2017).

2. Bogic M, Njoku A, Priebe S. Long-term mental health of war-refugees: a systematic literature review BMC Int Health Hum Rights (2015) 15:29. doi: 10.1186/s12914-015-0064-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Fazel M, Wheeler J, Danesh J. Prevalence of serious mental disorder in 7000 refugees resettled in western countries: a systematic review. Lancet (2005) 365:1309–14. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)61027-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bryant RA, Salmon K, Sinclair E, Davidson P. A prospective study of appraisals in childhood posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:2502–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dunmore E, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Cognitive factors involved in the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behav Res Ther. (1999) 37:809–29. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00181-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. LoSavio ST, Dillon KH, Resick PA. Cognitive factors in the development, maintenance, and treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 14:18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. O'Donnell ML, Elliott P, Wolfgang BJ, Creamer M. Posttraumatic appraisals in the development and persistence of posttraumatic stress symptoms. J Trauma Stress (2007) 20:173–82. doi: 10.1002/jts.20198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Therapy (2000) 38:319–45. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Foa EB, Rothbaum BO. Treating the Trauma of Rape: Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for PTSD. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1998).

Google Scholar

10. Resick PA, Schnicke MK. Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1992) 60:748–56. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.5.748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Janoff-Bulman R. Shattered Assumptions: Towards a New Psychology of Trauma. New york, NY: Free Press (1992).

Google Scholar

12. Ter Heide FJJ, Sleijpen M, van der Aa N. Posttraumatic world assumptions among treatment-seeking refugees. Transcult Psychiatry (2017) 54:824–39. doi: 10.1177/1363461517741811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hoffman J, Liddell B, Bryant RA, Nickerson A. The relationship between moral injury appraisals, trauma exposure, and mental health in refugees. Depress Anxiety (2018) 35:1030–9. doi: 10.1002/da.22787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nickerson A, Schnyder U, Bryant RA, Schick M, Mueller J, Morina N. Moral injury in traumatized refugees. Psychother Psychosomat. (2015) 84:122–3. doi: 10.1159/000369353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Litz BT, Stein N, Delaney E, Lebowitz L, Nash WP, Silva C. Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: a preliminary model and intervention strategy. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:695–706. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Currier JM, Holland JM, Rojas-Flores L, Herrera S, Foy D. Morally injurious experiences and meaning in Salvadorian teachers exposed to violence. Psychol Trauma (2015) 7:24–33. doi: 10.1037/a0034092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Haight W, Sugrue E, Calhoun M, Black J. Everyday coping with moral injury: the perspectives of professionals and parents involved with child protection services. Child Youth Ser Rev. (2017) 82:108–21. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2017.09.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Levinson M. Moral injury and the ethics of educational justice Harv Educ Rev. (2015) 85:203–28. doi: 10.17763/0017-8055.85.2.203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. McCormack L, Riley L. Medical discharge from the “family”, moral injury, and a diagnosis of PTSD: is psychological growth possible in the aftermath of policing trauma. Traumatology (2016) 22:19–28. doi: 10.1037/trm0000059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Steinmetz SE, Gray MJ. Treatment for distress assocated with accurate appraisals of self-blame for moral transgressions Curr Psychiat Rev. (2015) 11:207–19. doi: 10.2174/1573400511666150629105709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Nash WP, Marino Carper TL, Mills MA, Au T, Goldsmith A, Litz BT. Psychometric evaluation of the moral injury events scale. Mil Med. (2013) 178:646–52. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Farnsworth JK, Drescher KD, Evans WD, Walser RD. A functional approach to understanding and treating military-related moral injury. J Context Behav Sci. (2017) 6:391–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bryan CJ, Bryan AO, Anestis MD, Anestis JC, Green BA, Etienne N. Measuring moral injury: psychometric properties of the moral injury events scale in two military samples. Assessment (2015) 23:557–70. doi: 10.1177/1073191115590855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Currier JM, Farnsworth JK, Drescher KD, McDermott RC, Sims BM, Albright DL. (2017). Development and evaluation of the expressions of moral injury scale-military version. Clin Psychol Psychother. 25:474–88. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

25. Held P, Klassen BJ, Zou DS, Schroedter BS, Karnik NS, Pollack MH. Negative posttrauma cognitions mediate the association between morally injurious events and trauma-related psychopathology in treatment-seeking veterans. J Trauma Stress (2017) 30:698–703. doi: 10.1002/jts.22234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Li SS, Liddell BJ, Nickerson A. The relationship between post-migration stress and psychological disorders in refugees and asylum seekers. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:82. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0723-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Mollica RF, McInnes K, Poole C, Tor S. Dose-effect relationships of trauma to symptoms of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder among Cambodian survivors of mass violence. Br J Psychiatry (1998) 173:482–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.173.6.482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. World Health Organization (n.d). Process of Translation and Adaptation of Instruments. Geneva: World Health Organization.

29. Mollica RF, Caspi-Yavin Y, Bollini P, Truong T, Tor S, Lavelle J. The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire. Validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1992) 180:111–6. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199202000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Foa EB. Posttraumatic Diagnostic Manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems (1996).

31. Silove D, Sinnerbrink I, Field A, Manicavasagar V, Steel Z. Anxiety, depression and PTSD in asylum-seekers: associations with pre-migration trauma and post-migration stressors. Br J Psychiatry (1997) 170:351–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.4.351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Steel Z, Silove D, Bird K, McGorry P, Mohan P. Pathways from war trauma to posttraumatic stress symptoms among Tamil asylum seekers, refugees and immigrants J Traumat Stress (1999) 12:421–35. doi: 10.1023/A:1024710902534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Schynder U, Mueller J, Morina N, Schick M, Bryant RA, Nickerson A. A comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder in traumatized refugees J Trauma Stress (2015) 28:267–74. doi: 10.1002/jts.22023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Herlihy J, Scragg P, Turner S. Discrepancies in autobiographical memories–implications for the assessment of asylum seekers: repeated interviews study BMJ (2002) 324:324–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7333.324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Neuner F, Schauer M, Klaschik C, Karunakara U, Elbert T. A comparison of narrative exposure therapy, supportive counseling, and psycheducation for treating posttraumatic stress disorder in an African refugee settlement. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2004) 72:579–87. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.4.579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Wind TR, van der Aa N, de la Rie S, Knipscheer J. The assessment of psychopathology among traumatized refugees: measurement invariance of the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire and the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 across five linguistic groups. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2017) 8:1321357. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1321357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K. The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the posttrauamtic diagnostic scale. Psychol Assess. (1997) 9:445–51. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Mollica RF, Wyshak G, de Marneffe D, Khuon F, Lavelle J. Indochinese versions of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25: a screening instrument for the psychiatric care of refugees Am J Psychiatry (1987) 144:497–500. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.4.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Taylor EM, Yanni EA, Pezzi C, Guterbock M, Rothney E, Harton E. Physical and mental health status of Iraqi refugees resettled in the United States. J Immigr Minor Health (2014) 16:1130–7. doi: 10.1007/s10903-013-9893-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Eisinga R, Grotenhuis M, Pelzer B. The reliability of a two-item scale: pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown? Int J Public Health (2013) 58:637–42. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0416-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Morina N, Ewers SM, Passardi S, Schnyder U, Knaevelsrud C, Muller J. Mental health assessments in refugees and asylum seekers: evaluation of a tablet-assisted screening software. Confl Health (2017) 11:18. doi: 10.1186/s13031-017-0120-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Little TD. Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2013).

Google Scholar

43. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Beck AT. Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper and Row (1967).

Google Scholar

45. Beck AT. Cognitive therapy of depression: new perspectives. In: Clayton PJ, Barrett JE, editors. Treatment of depression. New York, NY: Raven Press (1983). p. 265–90.

Google Scholar

46. Abramson L, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: a theory-based subtype of depression. Psychol Rev. (1989) 96:358–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: an elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychol Bull. (2001) 127:773–96. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Figueroa CA, Ruhe HG, Koeter MW, Spinhoven P, Van der Does W, Bockting CL Cognitive reactivity versus dysfunctional cognitions and the prediction of relapse in recurrent major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry (2015) 76:e1306–12. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lewinsohn PM, Steinmetz JL, Larson DW, Franklin J. Depression-related cognitions: antecedent or consequence? J Abnorm Psychol. (1981) 90:213–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.90.3.213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cisela JA, Roberts JE. Rumination, negative cognition and their interactive effects on depressed mood. Emotion (2007) 7:555–65. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Morrow J, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1990) 58:519–27. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.3.519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Teasdale JD. Negative thinking in depression: cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship? Adv Behav Res Therapy (1983) 5:3–25. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(83)90013-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schumm JA, Dickstein BD, Walter KH, Owens GP, Chard KM. Changes in posttraumatic cognitions predict changes in posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during cognitive processing therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2015) 83:1161–6. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Britt TW, Adler AB, Sawhney G, Bliese PD. Coping strategies as moderators of the association between combat exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. J Traumat Stress (2017) 30:491–501. doi: 10.1002/jts.22221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Canton-Cortes D, Canton J, Cortes MR. The interactive effect of blame attribution with characteristics of child sexual abuse on posttraumatic stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2012) 200:329–35. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31824cc078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ullman SE, Filipas HH, Townsend SM, Starzynski LL. Psychosocial correlates of PTSD symptom severity in sexual assault survivors. J Trauma Stress (2007) 20:821–31. doi: 10.1002/jts.20290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Beck JG, Coffey SF, Palyo SA, Gudmundsdottir B, Miller LM, Colder CR. Psychometric Properties of the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): a replication with motor vehicle accident survivors. Psychol Assess. (2004) 16:289–98. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hiskey S, Ayres R, Andrews L, Troop N. Support for the location of negative posttraumatic cognitions in the diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder. Personal Indiv Diff. (2015) 74:192–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kolts RL, Robinson AM, Tracy JJ. The relationship of sociotropy and autonomy to posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptomatology in trauma survivors J Clin Psychol. (2004) 60:53–63. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Startup M, Makgekgenene L, Webster R. The role of self-blame for trauma as assessed by the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): a self-protective cognition? Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:395–403. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Foa EB, Zinbarg R, Rothbaum BO. Uncontrollability and unpredictability in post-traumatic stress disorder: an animal model. Psychol Bull. (1992) 112:218–38. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Frazier AL, Bergman M, Steward J. Perceived control and posttraumatic stress: a temporal model. Appl Prevent Psychol. (2002) 10:207–23. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(01)80015-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Schnyder U, Wittmann L, Friedrich-Perez J, Hepp U, Moergeli H. Posttraumatic stress disorder following accidental injury: rule or exception in Switzerland? Psychother Psychosom (2008) 77:111–8. doi: 10.1159/000112888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Silove D, Mohsin M, Tay AK, Steel Z, Tam N, Savio E, Rees S. Six-year longitudinal study of pathways leading to explosive anger involving the traumas of recurrent conflict and the cumulative sense of injustice in Timor-Leste. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2017) 52:1281–94. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1428-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Stein NR, Mills MA, Arditte K, Mendoza C, Borah AM, Resick PA. A scheme for categorizing traumatic military events. Behav Modif. (2012) 36:787–807. doi: 10.1177/0145445512446945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Blain LM, Galovski TE, Elwood LS, Meriac JP. How does the posttraumatic cognitions inventory fit in a four-factor posttraumatic stress disorder world? An initial analysis. Psychol Trauma (2013) 5:513–20. doi: 10.1037/a0030251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Daie-Gabai A, Aderka IM, Allon-Schindel I, Foa EB, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): psychometric properties and gender differences in an Israeli sample. J Anxiety Disord. (2011) 25:266–71. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Davis LW, Leonhardt BL, Siegel A, Brustuen B, Luedtke B, Vohs JL. Metacognitive capacity predicts severity of trauma-related dysfunctional cognitions in adults with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 237:182–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Porter M, Haslam N. Predisplacement and postdisplacement factors associated with mental health of refugees and internally displaced persons: a meta-analysis. J Am Med Assoc. (2005) 294:602–12. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kumpula MJ, Pentel KZ, Foa EB, LeBlanc NJ, Bui E, McSweeney LB. Temporal Sequencing of Change in Posttraumatic Cognitions and PTSD symptom reduction during prolonged exposure therapy. Behav Ther. (2017) 48:156–65. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Gray MJ, Schorr Y, Nash W, Lebowitz L, Amidon A, Lansing A. Adaptive disclosure: an open trial of a novel exposure-based intervention for service members with combat-related psychological stress injuries. Behav Ther. (2012) 43:407–15. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Litz BT, Lebowitz L, Gray MJ, Nash WP. Adaptive Discosure: A New Treatment for Military Trauma, Loss and Moral Injury. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2017).

Google Scholar

Keywords: moral injury, refugees, posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, trauma

Citation: Nickerson A, Hoffman J, Schick M, Schnyder U, Bryant RA and Morina N (2018) A Longitudinal Investigation of Moral Injury Appraisals Amongst Treatment-Seeking Refugees. Front. Psychiatry 9:667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00667

Received: 16 May 2018; Accepted: 19 November 2018;
Published: 18 December 2018.

Edited by:

Jutta Lindert, University of Applied Sciences Emden Leer, Germany

Reviewed by:

Solvig Ekblad, Karolinska Institute (KI), Sweden
Daniel Bressington, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong

Copyright © 2018 Nickerson, Hoffman, Schick, Schnyder, Bryant and Morina. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Angela Nickerson, a.nickerson@unsw.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.