Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 30 September 2022
Sec. Neuropsychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Interpersonal Synchrony in the Social World: Neuropsychological, Psychological and Neurobiological Mechanisms View all 8 articles

Trait mindful awareness predicts inter-brain coupling but not individual brain responses during naturalistic face-to-face interactions

  • 1Psychology Department, New York University, New York City, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
  • 3Department of Clinical Psychology, Free University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Max Planck - NYU Center for Language Music and Emotion, New York University, New York City, NY, United States

In recent years, the possible benefits of mindfulness meditation have sparked much public and academic interest. Mindfulness emphasizes cultivating awareness of our immediate experience and has been associated with compassion, empathy, and various other prosocial traits. However, neurobiological evidence pertaining to the prosocial benefits of mindfulness in social settings is sparse. In this study, we investigate neural correlates of trait mindful awareness during naturalistic dyadic interactions, using both intra-brain and inter-brain measures. We used the Muse headset, a portable electroencephalogram (EEG) device often used to support mindfulness meditation, to record brain activity from dyads as they engaged in naturalistic face-to-face interactions in a museum setting. While we did not replicate prior laboratory-based findings linking trait mindfulness to individual brain responses (N = 379 individuals), self-reported mindful awareness did predict dyadic inter-brain synchrony, in theta (~5–8 Hz) and beta frequencies (~26-27 Hz; N = 62 dyads). These findings underscore the importance of conducting social neuroscience research in ecological settings to enrich our understanding of how (multi-brain) neural correlates of social traits such as mindful awareness manifest during social interaction, while raising critical practical considerations regarding the viability of commercially available EEG systems.

Introduction

Recent years have seen an increase in popular interest in the benefits of mindfulness. As a personality trait, mindfulness refers to attending to the present moment experience without judging occurring feelings or thoughts (Bishop et al., 2006), and has been associated with prosocial behaviors and traits (Donald et al., 2019): Multiple psychometric studies have shown that trait mindfulness is correlated with agreeableness (Thompson and Waltz, 2007), empathy (Dekeyser et al., 2008), and conscientiousness (Thompson and Waltz, 2007; Giluk, 2009); and mindfulness-based interventions and training programs are found to effectively enhance compassion and empathy (Kreplin et al., 2018; Campos et al., 2019).

Some prevailing frameworks for understanding mindfulness theorize that the practice improves attentional control, self-awareness, metacognition, and emotional control (Lutz et al., 2007; Vago and Silbersweig, 2012). Indeed, mindfulness-related changes in brain and behavior have been observed various socially relevant behavioral paradigms, such as the Affective Stroop Task (Allen et al., 2012), pain perception tasks (Grant et al., 2011; Lutz et al., 2013; Mascaro et al., 2013), emotional provocation (Taylor et al., 2011), and prosocial decision-making (Kirk et al., 2016). These and related findings have led researchers to suggest that people who practice mindfulness develop self-regulation capacities (Chiesa et al., 2011), which in turn leads to the ability to increase the awareness of others in social settings (Schindler and Pfattheicher, 2021). Taken together, these findings suggest that by training their self-regulation functions, mindful individuals may be better able to observe and alter their social responses and emotional awareness, and engage in prosocial behaviors (Tang et al., 2015; Berry et al., 2020). This is indirectly supported by neuroimaging studies of mindfulness meditators: A meta-analysis found meditators, compared to novices, exhibited consistent changes in regions that have been associated with self-awareness (Craig, 2004), higher-order self-processing (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), and metacognition (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Fleming et al., 2010; McCaig et al., 2011). Electroencephalogram (EEG) studies, in turn, have identified lower frontal gamma activity in long-term mindfulness meditators during meditation (Lutz et al., 2004).

Crucially, however, despite mindfulness being linked to prosocial traits and to various affective modalities in controlled laboratory studies, few studies to date have investigated the neural correlates of trait mindfulness during naturalistic social interaction (Kaplan et al., 2018 ). Neural correlates of mindfulness-related prosociality have been observed using fMRI in controlled tasks like viewing emotional images (Taylor et al., 2011) and playing an Ultimatum Game (Kirk et al., 2016). In the case of naturalistic behaviors, mindfulness-related prosociality has been measured using self-report. For example, one survey study showed that trait mindfulness is associated with a heightened perceptual focus in conversations, but not daily behavioral patterns that exhibit prosocial orientation (Kaplan et al., 2018). Critically, while mindfulness meditation has been theorized to support social brain function, to our knowledge this has yet to be validated in naturalistic social settings: We are unaware of any studies that have linked neural activity during naturalistic interaction to either trait mindfulness or mindfulness-based training. To fill this gap, we recorded brain activity from dyads during face-to-face communication, and asked whether mindfulness-related traits (Brown and Ryan, 2003) predict neural responses during social interaction similar to those in laboratory-based tasks.

To answer this question, we adopted a “crowdsourcing” approach to collect naturalistic inter-brain synchrony data in a participatory art installation Mutual Wave Machine, an interactive multi-brain neurofeedback installation that translates the real-time correlation of pairs’ EEG activity into light patterns (described in detail in Dikker et al., 2021; also see wp.nyu.edu/mutualwavemachine). Pairs of museumgoers were invited to interact naturally with each other while receiving audio-visual feedback of their inter-brain synchrony. We recorded their EEG activities for the neurofeedback and for offline analyses. In the offline analyses, our group has previously found that inter-brain coupling is linked to social closeness, personal distress, and shared social attention (Dikker et al., 2017, 2021), demonstrating scientific validity of the paradigm. Here, we use the same paradigm (Chen et al., 2021) to investigate trait mindful awareness, with the following differences: we used a four-channel instead of a 14-channel portable EEG headset, adopted a different synchrony metric (Circular Correlation Coefficient, see Data Analysis), and asked different research questions.

To record participants’ brain activity we used Muse, a 4-channel EEG headband commercialized as a neurofeedback tool for mindfulness-based stress reduction training (MBSR; Hashemi et al., 2016). As a neurofeedback tool, Muse and its accompanying app have been reported to be effective in reducing stress in breast-cancer patients (Millstine et al., 2019), and improving well-being and attention (Bhayee et al., 2016). The validity of Muse-collected data was demonstrated in a couple of studies: an ERP study showed the pooled average of TP9 and TP10 electrodes successfully captured the N200, P300 responses (Krigolson et al., 2017); EEG data signatures such as power spectral density (PSD), the individual alpha frequency (IAF) and the frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA) measures computed from Muse data were consistent with those from a research-grade EEG system (Cannard et al., 2021); researchers successfully classified perceived mental stress level using the theta-band PSD from Muse (Arsalan et al., 2019). However, mindfulness-related EEG research using the Muse headset has generated mixed results. For example, one study using the Muse observed a significant increase in beta and gamma frequencies in the post-meditation sessions compared to pre-meditation (Karydis et al., 2018). Another study using the “calm score” computed by the Muse app (a proposed proxy for mindfulness), however, failed to observe “calm score” changes in participants after a 1-month meditation intervention. Additionally, some researchers have reported findings where the “calm score” did not reflect participants’ increased trait mindfulness (Acabchuk et al., 2020).

Portable, wireless EEG headsets are increasingly used to conduct social neuroscience research in naturalistic settings, and specifically in so-called hyperscanning studies—studies that simultaneously measure the brain activity of multiple people interacting with each other. Using a range of metrics to quantify inter-brain connectivity (Ayrolles et al., 2021), inter-brain coupling has been linked to a variety of factors during both verbal and non-verbal social tasks (Czeszumski et al., 2020). Inter-brain coupling has been associated with prosociality in various contexts. For example, inter-brain coupling studies using EEG have shown higher synchrony for couples than strangers in natural conversation and motor coordination tasks (Kinreich et al., 2017; Djalovski et al., 2021), in social coordination and cooperation (Mu et al., 2017; Barraza et al., 2020), and in teams with better collective performance (Reinero et al., 2021). During social interactions outside of laboratory environments, our group has previously found that inter-brain coupling is linked to social closeness, personal distress, and shared social attention (Dikker et al., 2017, 2021).

In sum, the first aim of the present study was to investigate whether laboratory findings on the neural correlates of mindful awareness replicate during naturalistic social interaction in an EEG device that has been explicitly associated with mindfulness meditation. Specifically, we asked if more mindful individuals exhibited enhanced EEG alpha (8–12 Hz) and theta (4–8 Hz) power during face-to-face social engagement (Takahashi et al., 2005). The second aim of this research was to capture possible “multi-brain” neural correlates of mindful awareness during naturalistic interaction. We investigated whether inter-brain coupling correlates with mindful awareness during naturalistic social settings, building on a growing body of research on mindfulness on the one hand, and social neuroscience research using portable EEG systems on the other.

Materials and methods

Participants

We collected data from participants who partook in the Mutual Wave Machine exhibition at Espacio Telefónica in Madrid, Spain (2019; see Study Setup). 554 individuals participated in the study, including 271 females, 245 males, and three individuals who identified as “other.” Participants’ ages ranged from 12 to 81 years, with an average of 33.8 years. After removing data with poor quality, we ended up with 379 participants for the PSD analysis, and 62 dyads (124 individuals) for inter-brain analysis (see Data Analysis). For inter-brain analysis, we retained 22% of participants, which is a lower retention rate than the previous iteration’s 39% (Dikker et al., 2021). This could be explained by the employment of the 4-channel EEG headset rather than 16 channels in the previous study (see Discussion).

Participants completed the questionnaires and consent forms in Spanish. They were informed that the primary purpose of participation was the art experience, but their data would be used for research in the future. Participation was voluntary and without monetary compensation. Individual written informed consent was obtained before the session.

Study setup

This study was conducted as part of the participatory art installation Mutual Wave Machine where pairs of participants interacted naturally in a museum setting. This setup allowed us to study real-world face-to-face social interactions in a large population of participants recruited outside of the traditional research subject pool.

Museum visitors freely interacted with each other while their EEG was recorded using the Muse, a four-electrode wireless EEG system (Krigolson et al., 2017). In the current study, we recruited visitors at the Mutual Wave Machine exhibition at Espacio Telefónica in Madrid, Spain. Participants could participate either in pairs or individually to be paired with others. The artwork featured two shell-like structures enclosing the two participants facing each other, with visual projection on the shells and auditory feedback. EEG headsets were applied while participants completed a consent form and pre-experiment questionnaire. They were told the purpose of the work is to investigate whether being on the same “brain wavelength” related to their subjective feelings of “being in sync,” and that the brightness of the visual feedback reflected their synchrony level in real time. They were encouraged to try different strategies to achieve more synchrony. Note that there is evidence in past iterations that suggests being well-informed about the experiment increases pairs’ synchrony (Dikker et al., 2021). This increase, however, persisted even when the neurofeedback signal was a sham, suggesting the “placebo effect” of the knowledge about the experiment. We thus adopted this protocol to observe synchrony in the most encouraging setting.

The interaction typically lasted 10 min. Real-time inter-brain power correlations were calculated and used to generate visual feedback for the participants as part of the 10-min experience (Chen et al., 2021). Specifically, EEG data collected from the pair was processed in 6-s windows in real time. Both data streams were filtered into four frequency bands using FFTW (www.fftw.org; delta: 1–4 Hz; theta: 4–7 Hz; alpha: 7–12 Hz; beta: 12–30 Hz), and then Hilbert transformed to derive their instantaneous spectral power. Inter-brain synchrony was then calculated as the Pearson Correlation coefficient of the pairs’ instantaneous spectral power of particular frequency bands. Note that in some of the previous iterations, we took the effort to validate the EEG signal through eyes open/closed and up/down control experiments, which we did not perform in the current study due to practical limitations. Instead, we adopted strict data exclusion criteria semi-automatically (see EEG preprocessing).

Before and after the session, participants were asked to complete questionnaires for their affective traits and states (see Materials).

Materials

All participants were asked to complete short questionnaires both before and after the session, addressing their relationship to each other, mood, and personality traits. Relationship measures include questions about relationship duration and social closeness. Affective personality trait measurement consisted of (a) a revised 14-item version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index after the session (Davis and Others, 1980), including the subscales Personal Distress (e.g., “When I see someone who badly needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces”) and Empathic Concern (e.g., “I often have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me”). Internal reliability for each scale of the IRI was (<0.90), but it is in line with the literature: PD with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.74 and EC with 0.74; and (b) the MAAS (Brown and Ryan, 2003), consisting of 15 items measuring one’s awareness of what is taking place at the present (e.g., “I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later.”). Both questionnaires were answered on a five-point Likert scale ranging from “Does not describe me well” to “Describes me very well.” Social closeness was assessed using the Inclusion of the Other in the Self (IOS) Scale, a pictorial measure of closeness with two overlapping circles representing the self and the other (Aron et al., 1992). Participants also completed a shortened version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS-X; (Watson and Clark, 1994), which was not analyzed here because the purpose of the study was to investigate the (inter-brain) neural correlates of mindful awareness.

This study focuses on mindful awareness, one facet of the self-report trait mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006). We used the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), a standardized questionnaire designed to assess the open awareness of the present moment (Brown and Ryan, 2003). The MAAS has been widely applied and shown to successfully probe specific aspects of mindfulness, such as acting with awareness (Coffey and Hartman, 2008), perceived inattention (Van Dam et al., 2010), and burnout and engagement (Kotzé and Nel, 2016). Due to the limitation of the setup, we used the short MAAS questionnaire (7 out of the 10 questions) instead of more comprehensive mindfulness measures such as the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2006) which contains 38 questions, longer than what we could fit in during the limited session each person had in the museum. Since MAAS only focuses on the awareness factor of the FFMQ (other factors are observing, describing, non-judging and nonreactivity), our finding addresses only mindful awareness, rather than mindfulness in general.

Before the session, participants completed Relationship measures, Empathic Concern part of the IRI, the MAAS scale, and the IOS scale. After the session, participants completed the Empathic Concern as well as the Personal Distress parts of the IRI, the IOS scale, and the PANAS-X questionnaire. Note that there was a risk of demand characteristics since MAAS was measured beforehand. However, the inter-brain synchrony was measured when participants were already encouraged to connect, so we believe demand characteristics due to MAAS testing is only secondary to such effects, if they exist.

Data analysis

Personality metrics

After removing incomplete and incorrect data entries, 475 individuals’ answers were preserved. For the purpose of this study, the following metrics were analyzed: MAAS score, social closeness scale, Personal Distress, Empathic Concern, sex, age. To investigate which trait measure was related to mindful awareness, we constructed a multiple linear regression analysis using Personal Distress, Empathic Concern, age, and social closeness scale as predictors, and the MAAS score as the predicted variable.

EEG preprocessing

The initial dataset consisted of 277 pairs of ~10-min recordings. First, EEG data files were removed if files were not readable (4 pairs), the two EEG files were misaligned (119 pairs), or subjects’ self-reported information was missing (57 pairs). Each individual EEG dataset was then bandpass filtered from 0.1 to 30 Hz and segmented into 1-s epochs (“pseudo trials”). Bad channels were manually rejected upon visual inspection: since frontal channels (Fp1, Fp2) were noisier and more often removed than temporal channels (TP9, TP10), our data is mainly driven by the temporoparietal channels. We used the Python package “autoreject” (Jas et al., 2017) to remove epochs with movement artifacts and eye blinks, followed by manually checking the automatic selection and correction procedure. This resulted in the additional exclusion of 20 pairs due to poor data quality. Note that for inter-subject connectivity analyses, we only preserved temporally overlapping epochs that “survive” the preprocessing for both participants in each pair: unmatched epochs were removed. For individuals’ spectral power analyses, we used all the artifact-free epochs, regardless of the participants’ partners’ data, and removed participants with less than 60 clean epochs (60 s). Lastly, datasets with less than 50 remaining epochs (50 s) after these preprocessing steps were excluded from further analysis (15 pairs removed). These preprocessing steps resulted in 62 pairs (124 individuals) for the intersubject connectivity analysis, and 379 individuals for the spectral power analysis.

After preprocessing, we performed the short-time Fourier transform on the 1-s epochs, using a Hanning window with a one-sample step size, resulting in complex spectral coefficients of 1 Hz resolution from 1 to 30 Hz.

Individual PSD analysis

Individual PSD was computed from the preprocessed, epoched data (see previous section). We applied Welch’s method to estimate PSD per epoch from 1 to 30 Hz and averaged the result across all epochs and channels. The result was one PSD value per frequency for each participant. We then used cluster-based permutation analysis to investigate whether the PSD values significantly correlated with participants’ mindful awareness.

Inter-brain coupling analysis

Inter-brain coupling was calculated using Circular Correlation Coefficient (CCorr), which is a phase synchrony measure that is argued to be robust to spurious synchronization (Burgess, 2013; Goldstein et al., 2018). CCorr was computed between corresponding channels in the dyad, and then averaged across channel pairs.

To capture slower, transient information throughout the time series, the epoched complex coefficients were concatenated before the correlation (henceforth referred to as “concatenated”), resulting in two discontinuous complex series from the pair. CCorr was then computed by correlating the angular component of the two concatenated series for each pair with respect to all four channels using the Python package Astropy (Astropy Collaboration et al., 2018). The computation is demonstrated in eq. 1.1 (Circular Correlation and Regression, 2001), where X and Y are concatenated series from a certain frequency bin, and n represents the total number of time points (e.g., if 100 epochs are preserved, there are 256 Hz × 100 s = 25,600 time points). We used 1 Hz frequency bins ranging from 1 to 30 Hz. Following previous analyses of similar datasets, we also used a second metric, concatenated Projected Power Correlation (PPC; Hipp et al., 2012; Dikker et al., 2021) demonstrated in eq. 1.2–5, where X t f and Y t f are the concatenated complex coefficients at frequency f . First, the projection of Y t f on X t f is removed, leaving only the part of Y that’s orthogonal to X , i.e., Y X t f , and the same computation was done for X t f , resulting in X Y t f (eq. 1.2). Second, we computed the correlation between | X t f | and Y X t f , and | Y t f | and X Y t f respectively, and then averaged the two values as our PPC per frequency bin.

To investigate the difference between concatenating epochs versus averaging epochs, we also applied the same method to the epoched complex coefficients without concatenating (henceforth referred to as “epoched”). In such “epoched CCorr” or “epoched PPC,” the same calculation was done to individual epochs, and the result was an average of CCorr values across epochs. These exploratory results can be found in Supplementary Figures S1, S2.

r circular = i = 1 n sin x i x _ sin y i y _ i = 1 n sin x i x _ 2 i = 1 n sin y i y _ 2     (11)

Y X t f = imag Y t f X t f X t f e ̂ X     (12)

X Y t f = imag X t f Y t f Y t f e ̂ Y     (13)

e ̂ X t f = i X t f X t f     (14)

e ̂ Y t f = i Y t f Y t f     (15)

Cluster-based permutation analysis for correlation

The following procedure applies to both the individual PSD and pairs’ inter-brain coupling, in relation to either participants’ mindful awareness or pairs’ average mindful awareness, respectively. Interbrain synchrony is often computed using a variety of methods and thus is only meaningful in contrasting conditions with statistical analyses (Ayrolles et al., 2021). Therefore, we adopted the cluster-based permutation technique, a non-parametric and data-driven method to compare conditions.

To investigate the relationship between mindful awareness and inter-brain coupling, we computed the Pearson Correlation Coefficient between pairs’ MAAS score and connectivity metrics in every frequency bin, using a cluster-based nonparametric test to correct for multiple comparisons. The protocol is adapted from Dikker et al. (2021). First, Pearson correlation coefficients were computed between every frequency bin and pairs’ average MAAS scales, generating 30 correlation values. Correlation significance thresholds r_upper and r_lower were then determined by choosing the 97.5th and 2.5th percentile of the 30 correlation values, respectively. Then the random permutation procedure started with randomly shuffling the behavioral variable (e.g., MAAS scale), and computing correlation values for each frequency bin. Correlation values higher than r_upper or lower than r_lower were marked as significant in this permutation, and significant correlation values that were adjacent in frequencies were identified as clusters. For each cluster, we extracted the cluster size as our cluster statistics. When there was more than one cluster, the maximum cluster size was chosen. This random permutation procedure was repeated 2000 times, generating a distribution of cluster statistics, of which the 95th percentile was determined as the significant cluster threshold. Last, from the actual correlation values, we identified clusters using a value of p threshold of 0.1 and compared their sizes to the significant cluster threshold. The Monte-Carlo value of p was then calculated from the percentile score of the actual cluster size in the cluster statistic distribution. A Monte-Carlo value of p lower than the significant threshold 0.05 would mean the actual cluster size is larger than 95% of the random distribution of cluster sizes.

We applied the same procedure to correlate mindful awareness with individual PSD values, replacing the pairs’ average MAAS scale with individuals’ MAAS scale.

Results

Individuals’ EEG spectral power does not predict mindful awareness

Contrary to prior findings illuminating neurobiological changes related to mindfulness in laboratory contexts, i.e., a global increase in alpha and theta power during various kinds of meditations (Takahashi et al., 2005; Lomas et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2018), we found no significant correlation between individuals’ power spectral density during social interaction and their MAAS scales (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Pearson correlation coefficient between individuals’ MAAS score and their power spectral density (PSD). Cluster-based permutation analysis of the correlation coefficients showed no significant clusters (p = 0.92).

Inter-brain coupling is correlated with dyads’ mindful awareness

The cluster-based permutation analysis showed that pairs’ mindful awareness predicted inter-brain coupling (CCorr; Monte-Carlo value of p < 0.001). Specifically, as can be seen in Figure 2B, there were two clusters where the CCorr theta band (5–8 Hz) cluster shows a negative correlation between CCorr and subjects’ MAAS scales [Figure 2B, circular correlation coefficient at 7 Hz; r(62) = −0.373], and the high beta band (26–27 Hz) cluster shows a positive correlation [Figure 2C, circular correlation coefficient at 26 Hz; r(62) = 0.325]. Figure 2A shows the Pearson Correlation Coefficient between the MAAS and CCorr at every frequency from 1 to 30 Hz, with significant clusters marked with bold lines.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Correlation results between trait mindful awareness and CCorr. (A) Pearson correlation coefficients (y-axis) between pair-averaged MAAS and inter-brain coupling (CCorr), for each 1-Hz frequency bin from 1 to 30 Hz (x-axis). Two significant clusters (monte-carlo p = 0.002) are highlighted in bold. (B) Scatter plot between CCorr at 7 Hz and pair-averaged MAAS. The dotted line is the linear regression line. [r(62) = −0.373]. (C) Scatter plot between concatenated CCorr at 26 Hz and pair-averaged MAAS scale [r(62) = 0.325].

Personal distress predicts mindful awareness

The regression analysis showed Personal Distress as the only significant predictor among sex, age, Empathic Concern and Social Closeness [t(475) = −5.493, p < 0.001; Figure 3] for trait mindful awareness (for the full results, please see Supplementary Table S1). Individuals with lower personal distress reported higher MAAS scale.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlation between personal distress and MAAS. Individuals’ personal distress is negatively correlated with MAAS scale [t(475) = −5.493, p < 0.001].

Discussion

This study investigates the neural correlates of mindful awareness in naturalistic face-to-face social interactions. Our study investigated the relationship between mindful awareness and prosociality on both the psychometric and neurobiological level. The questionnaire results showed that trait mindful awareness was associated with lower personal distress. The EEG results showed that neural correlates of mindful awareness during social interaction were found in interbrain synchrony, but not in individual EEG power changes.

Intra- vs. inter-individual neural correlates of mindful awareness

Contrary to previous studies, we did not find a relationship between individual brain activity (power spectral density) and mindful awareness. There are a few possible explanations for this null effect. First, past studies investigating EEG power and mindfulness have been focusing on different types of mindfulness correlates from ours: power changes in individuals have been observed only in meditative states (Lutz et al., 2004; Takahashi et al., 2005), and functional changes in other controlled lab tasks are observed with fMRI (Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2007; Farb et al., 2007; Grant et al., 2011; Taylor et al., 2011; Allen et al., 2012; Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012; Lutz et al., 2013; Mascaro et al., 2013) or in event-related potentials with EEG (Brown et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2018). Second, using a four-channel portable EEG system in a noisy, less controlled setting might also have contributed to a null result. It is important to reiterate, however, that we did find inter-brain correlates of mindful awareness during social interaction.

This is not the first study to report a discrepancy between intra- and inter-brain neural correlates: other hyperscanning studies have similarly found that a multi-brain approach captures neural correlates of social behaviors that are not observed in individuals (Simony et al., 2016; Balconi et al., 2017; Davidesco et al., 2019; Dikker et al., 2021). For example, in one study an inter-brain network but not individual brain activity predicted players’ strategy in prisoner’s dilemma (Fallani et al., 2010), and inter-brain coupling but not individual alpha power nor intra-brain synchrony predicted students’ performance during lessons (Davidesco et al., 2019). Under the rationale that online mutual interaction is “a complex nonlinear system that cannot be reduced to the summation of effects in single isolated brains” (Koike et al., 2015), our study further validates a multi-brain approach in complex social tasks by extending it to a naturalistic setting and portable EEG systems.

Note that in previous iterations of the current study’s experimental setup, the Mutual Wave Machine, we found inter-brain coupling was correlated with pairs’ relationship duration and with their affective personalities (social closeness and perspective taking) using the EMOTIV portable EEG system. The current study intends to validate the easier-to-use EEG system Muse, focusing on a different affective trait, mindful awareness. Although we did not replicate previous findings about social closeness and perspective taking, it could be due to the significant difference in experimental setup and data analysis methods between the current study and past iterations using EMOTIV.

Mindful awareness and empathy measures

Our questionnaire results showed a negative correlation between trait mindful awareness and personal distress, but not between trait mindful awareness and empathic concern. In line with previous research, this result suggests that mindful awareness might alleviate the negative consequences of empathy, but not necessarily contribute to empathic feelings: Previous research has suggested a complicated relationship between mindfulness and empathy, especially when empathy is dissected as a multidimensional construct (Davis and Others, 1980). Measuring overall self-reported empathy, some studies have reported a positive correlation between trait mindfulness and empathy (Beitel et al., 2005; Dekeyser et al., 2008; Greason and Cashwell, 2009), and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) training has been shown to increase participants’ self-reported empathy (Shapiro et al., 1998). However, other studies did not find effects of mindfulness-based training on self-reported empathy, empathic concern, or emotion recognition (Galantino et al., 2005; Lim et al., 2015). Specifically, in an eight-week MBSR training for nursing students, self-reported personal distress decreased, whereas self-reported empathic concern and perspective taking did not change (Beddoe and Murphy, 2004).

Distinct from empathic concern, personal distress is a self-oriented negative feeling that is often associated with reduced perspective taking and compassion fatigue when witnessing others’ suffering, and can be unrelated to prosocial behaviors (Pulos et al., 2004; FeldmanHall et al., 2015). Thus, our finding that trait mindful awareness is negatively correlated with personal distress but not related to empathic concern, is in line with previous work. Such empirical evidence supports the theory that mindful awareness is a vital part of self-compassion, which contributes to the resiliency against emotional fatigue (Figley, 1995; Neff, 2003; Thomas, 2012).

Relationship between trait mindful awareness and inter-brain coupling

We observed a positive correlation between inter-brain coupling and mindful awareness in the beta frequency range, but a negative correlation in the theta frequency range. This finding seems puzzling in light of past studies reporting a positive relationship between inter-brain coupling and prosocial behavior and prosocial traits (Valencia and Froese, 2020). However, recent work has pushed back on the leading assumption that more synchrony is always ‘better’ (for review, see Mayo and Gordon, 2020). For example, multiple studies have found cognitive downsides of behavioral synchrony, such as insecure attachment (Feniger-Schaal et al., 2016), worse performance in cooperative problem solving (Abney et al., 2015; Wallot et al., 2016), and decreased self-regulation (Galbusera et al., 2019). Physiological research has also yielded mixed results about the link between synchrony and couples’ relationships as well as parent-infant engagement (Timmons et al., 2015; Wass et al., 2019). In their review, Mayo and Gordon (Mayo and Gordon, 2020) proposed situating synchrony in an interpersonal system that contains both collective and independent behaviors and taking into account both the synchronization and segregation aspects inherent to synchrony. While neural studies overwhelmingly report positive relationships between inter-brain synchrony and social factors, there too, some studies pointed to the complexities of such relationships. For example, Goldstein et al. (2018) found that, during hand holding, romantic partners’ inter-brain coupling (CCorr) was negatively correlated with analgesia of the target person upon pain stimulation, and positively correlated with empathic accuracy of the partner. Separate examination of the effect of pain and touch, however, suggested distinct brain-coupling components associated with the experience of pain and the empathy for pain. The relationship between inter-brain synchrony and mindful awareness, a personality trait that entails a mixture of cognitive properties, may also correspond to multiple processes and require further investigation.

Conclusion

This study used consumer-grade portable EEG (Muse) and asked how trait mindful awareness and prosociality manifest itself in neural responses during naturalistic dyadic face-to-face social interactions. Neural correlates of mindful awareness during social interaction were evident in inter-brain coupling, but we did not replicate previous studies showing individual EEG power changes as a function of mindful awareness using a consumer-grade EEG system. In addition, the directionality and signature of the relationship between mindful awareness and inter-brain coupling varied by frequency and by how inter-brain coupling was computed. Together, our findings are suggestive of a complex relationship between mindful awareness and inter-brain coupling, while at the same time raising a cautionary note about methodological approaches in hyperscanning research.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon request, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants themselves, or their legal guardian/next of kin for underage participants.

Author contributions

PC, SD, and UK: study conception and design. SD: data collection. PC: analysis and interpretation of results and draft manuscript preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was funded by a grant from Lundbeckfonden #R291-2018-1462 (UK), The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research grant #406.18.GO.024, and The Dutch Creative Industries Fund, and Fundación Telefónica (SD).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.915345/full#supplementary-material

References

Abney, D. H., Paxton, A., Dale, R., and Kello, C. T. (2015). Movement dynamics reflect a functional role for weak coupling and role structure in dyadic problem solving. Cogn. Process. 16, 325–332. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0648-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acabchuk, R. L., Simon, M. A., Low, S., Brisson, J. M., and Johnson, B. T. (2020). Measuring meditation Progress with a consumer-grade EEG device: caution from a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness 12, 68–81. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01497-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K. S., van Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., et al. (2012). Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. J. Neurosci. 32, 15601–15610. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2957-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsalan, A., Majid, M., Butt, A. R., and Anwar, S. M. (2019). Classification of perceived mental stress using a commercially available EEG headband. IEEE J. Biomed. Health Inform. 23, 2257–2264. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2019.2926407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astropy CollaborationPrice-Whelan, B. M., Sipőcz, H. M., Günther, P. L., Lim, S. M., Crawford, S. C., et al. (2018). The Astropy project: building an open-science project and status of the v2.0 Core package. Astron. J. 156:123. doi: 10.3847/1538-3881/aabc4f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayrolles, A., Brun, F., Chen, P., Djalovski, A., Beauxis, Y., Delorme, R., et al. (2021). HyPyP: a Hyperscanning python pipeline for inter-brain connectivity analysis. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 72–83. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Pezard, L., Nandrino, J.-L., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2017). Two is better than one: the effects of strategic cooperation on intra- and inter-brain connectivity by fNIRS. PLoS One 12:e0187652. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barraza, P., Pérez, A., and Rodríguez, E. (2020). Brain-to-brain coupling in the gamma-band as a marker of shared intentionality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:295. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beddoe, A. E., and Murphy, S. O. (2004). Does mindfulness decrease stress and Foster empathy among nursing students? J. Nurs. Educ. 43, 305–312. doi: 10.3928/01484834-20040701-07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beitel, M., Ferrer, E., and Cecero, J. J. (2005). Psychological mindedness and awareness of self and others. J. Clin. Psychol. 61, 739–750. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D. R., Hoerr, J. P., Cesko, S., Alayoubi, A., Carpio, K., Zirzow, H., et al. (2020). Does mindfulness training without explicit ethics-based instruction promote prosocial behaviors? A meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46, 1247–1269. doi: 10.1177/0146167219900418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhayee, S., Tomaszewski, P., Lee, D. H., Moffat, G., Pino, L., Moreno, S., et al. (2016). Attentional and affective consequences of technology supported mindfulness training: a randomised, active control, efficacy trial. BMC Psychol. 4:60. doi: 10.1186/s40359-016-0168-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2006). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., and Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of Attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 11483–11488. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606552104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., and Inzlicht, M. (2013). Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 93–99. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, A. P. (2013). On the interpretation of synchronization in EEG Hyperscanning studies: a cautionary note. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:881. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, D., Modrego-Alarcón, M., López-Del-Hoyo, Y., González-Panzano, M., Van Gordon, W., Shonin, E., et al. (2019). Exploring the role of meditation and dispositional mindfulness on social cognition domains: a controlled study. Front. Psychol. 10:809. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannard, C., Wahbeh, H., and Delorme, A. (2021). Validating the wearable MUSE headset for EEG spectral analysis and frontal alpha asymmetry. bioRxiv. doi: 10.1101/2021.11.02.466989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The Precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and Behavioural correlates. Brain J. Neurol. 129, 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P., Hendrikse, S., Sargent, K., Romani, M., Oostrik, M., Wilderjans, T. F., et al. (2021). Hybrid harmony: a multi-person Neurofeedback application for interpersonal synchrony. Front. Neuro. 2:687108. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2021.687108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiesa, A., Calati, R., and Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The Frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a Rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28, 168–186. doi: 10.3758/BF03331976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Circular Correlation and Regression (2001). Topics in circular statistics,” in Series on Multivariate Analysis. Vol. 5. World Scientific, 175–203.

Google Scholar

Coffey, K. A., and Hartman, M. (2008). Mechanisms of action in the inverse relationship between mindfulness and psychological distress. Complement. Health Pract. Rev. 13, 79–91. doi: 10.1177/1533210108316307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2004). Human feelings: why are some more aware than others? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 239–241. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czeszumski, A., Eustergerling, S., Lang, A., Menrath, D., Gerstenberger, M., Schuberth, S., et al. (2020). Hyperscanning: a valid method to study neural inter-brain underpinnings of social interaction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:39. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidesco, I., Laurent, E., Valk, H., West, T., Dikker, S., Milne, C., et al. (2019). Brain-to-brain synchrony between students and teachers predicts learning outcomes. bioRxiv :644047.

Google Scholar

Davis, M. H., and Others. (1980). “A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.” Available at: https://www.uv.es/friasnav/Davis_1980.pdf

Google Scholar

Dekeyser, M., Raes, F., Leijssen, M., Leysen, S., and Dewulf, D. (2008). Mindfulness skills and interpersonal behaviour. Personal. Individ. Differ. 44, 1235–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Michalareas, G., Oostrik, M., Serafimaki, A., Kahraman, H. M., Struiksma, M. E., et al. (2021). Crowdsourcing neuroscience: inter-brain coupling during face-to-face interactions outside the laboratory. NeuroImage 227:117436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., et al. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 27, 1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Djalovski, A., Dumas, G., Kinreich, S., and Feldman, R. (2021). Human attachments shape interbrain synchrony toward efficient performance of social goals. NeuroImage 226:117600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donald, J. N., Sahdra, B. K., Van Zanden, B., Duineveld, J. J., Atkins, P. W. B., Marshall, S. L., et al. (2019). Does your mindfulness benefit others? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the link between mindfulness and prosocial behaviour. Br. J. Psychol. 110, 101–125. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallani, D. V., Fabrizio, V. N., Sinatra, R., Astolfi, L., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., et al. (2010). Defecting or not defecting: how to ‘Read’ human behavior during cooperative games by EEG measurements. PLoS One 5:e14187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 313–322. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Evans, D., and Mobbs, D. (2015). Empathic concern drives costly altruism. NeuroImage 105, 347–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feniger-Schaal, R., Noy, L., Hart, Y., Koren-Karie, N., Mayo, A. E., and Alon, U. (2016). Would you like to play together? Adults’ attachment and the Mirror game. Attach Hum. Dev. 18, 33–45. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2015.1109677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figley, C. R., (ed.) (1995). “Compassion fatigue: coping with secondary traumatic stress disorder in those who treat the traumatized.” Brunner/Mazel Psychological Stress Series, No. 23. 268. Available at: https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/1995-97891-000.pdf

Google Scholar

Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J., and Rees, G. (2010). Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 329, 1541–1543. doi: 10.1126/science.1191883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galantino, M. L., Baime, M., Maguire, M., Szapary, P. O., and Farrar, J. T. (2005). Association of Psychological and Physiological Measures of stress in health-care professionals during an 8-week mindfulness meditation program: mindfulness in practice. Stress Health: J. Int. Soc. Invest. Stress 21, 255–261. doi: 10.1002/smi.1062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galbusera, L., Finn, M. T. M., Tschacher, W., and Kyselo, M. (2019). Interpersonal synchrony feels good but impedes self-regulation of affect. Sci. Rep. 9:14691. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50960-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giluk, T. L. (2009). Mindfulness, big five personality, and affect: a meta-analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 47, 805–811. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, P., Weissman-Fogel, I., Dumas, G., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2018). “Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115: E2528–E2537. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., and Rainville, P. (2011). A non-elaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and pain-related cortices predicts Low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain 152, 150–156, doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greason, P. B., and Cashwell, C. S. (2009). Mindfulness and counseling self-efficacy: the mediating role of attention and empathy. Couns. Educ. Superv. 49, 2–19. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6978.2009.tb00083.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasenkamp, W., and Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:38. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashemi, A., Pino, L. J., Moffat, G., Mathewson, K. J., Aimone, C., Bennett, P. J., et al. (2016). Characterizing population EEG dynamics throughout adulthood. eNeuro 3, ENEURO.0275–ENEU16.2016. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0275-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hipp, J. F., Hawellek, D. J., Corbetta, M., Siegel, M., and Engel, A. K. (2012). Large-scale cortical correlation structure of spontaneous oscillatory activity. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 884–890. doi: 10.1038/nn.3101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jas, M., Engemann, D. A., Bekhti, Y., Raimondo, F., and Gramfort, A. (2017). Autoreject: automated artifact rejection for MEG and EEG data. NeuroImage 159, 417–429. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, D. M., Raison, C. L., Milek, A., Tackman, A. M., Pace, T. W. W., and Mehl, M. R. (2018). Dispositional mindfulness in daily life: a naturalistic observation study. PLoS One 13:e0206029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karydis, T., Langer, S., Foster, S. L., and Mershin, A. (2018). “Identification of post-meditation perceptual states using wearable EEG and self-calibrating protocols,” in Proceedings of the 11th PErvasive Technologies Related to Assistive Environments Conference. 566–569. Corfu Greece: ACM.

Google Scholar

Kinreich, S., Djalovski, A., Kraus, L., Louzoun, Y., and Feldman, R. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Sci. Rep. 7:17060. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, U., Xiaosi, G., Sharp, C., Hula, A., Fonagy, P., and Read Montague, P. (2016). Mindfulness training increases cooperative decision making in economic exchanges: evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage 138, 274–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koike, T., Tanabe, H. C., and Sadato, N. (2015). Hyperscanning neuroimaging technique to reveal the ‘two-in-one’ system in social interactions. Neurosci. Res. 90, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotzé, M., and Nel, P. (2016). The psychometric properties of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS) and Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI) as measures of mindfulness and their relationship with burnout and work engagement. SA J. Ind. Psychol. 42, 1–11. doi: 10.4102/sajip.v42i1.1366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreplin, U., Farias, M., and Brazil, I. A. (2018). The limited Prosocial effects of meditation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 8:2403. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20299-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krigolson, O. E., Williams, C. C., Norton, A., Hassall, C. D., and Colino, F. L. (2017). Choosing MUSE: validation of a Low-cost, portable EEG system for ERP research. Front. Neurosci. 11:109. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D. J., Kulubya, E., Goldin, P., Goodarzi, A., and Girgis, F. (2018). Review of the neural oscillations underlying meditation. Front. Neurosci. 12:178. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, D., Condon, P., and DeSteno, D. (2015). Mindfulness and compassion: an examination of mechanism and scalability. PLoS One 10:e0118221. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., and Fu, C. H. Y. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 401–410. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2007). “Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: an introduction,” in The Cambridge handbook of consciousness. eds. P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 499–552.

Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., and Davidson, R. J. (2004). “Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 16369–16373. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407401101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., McFarlin, D. R., Perlman, D. M., Salomons, T. V., and Davidson, R. J. (2013). Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of painful stimuli in expert meditators. NeuroImage 64, 538–546. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mascaro, J. S., Rilling, J. K., Negi, L. T., and Raison, C. L. (2013). Pre-existing brain function predicts subsequent practice of mindfulness and compassion meditation. NeuroImage 69, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayo, O., and Gordon, I. (2020). In and out of synchrony-behavioral and physiological dynamics of dyadic interpersonal coordination. Psychophysiology 57:e13574. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCaig, R. G., Dixon, M., Keramatian, K., Liu, I., and Christoff, K. (2011). Improved modulation of Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex using real-time fMRI training and meta-cognitive awareness. NeuroImage 55, 1298–1305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millstine, D. M., Bhagra, A., Jenkins, S. M., Croghan, I. T., Stan, D. L., Boughey, J. C., et al. (2019). Use of a wearable EEG headband as a meditation device for women with newly diagnosed breast cancer: a randomized controlled trial. Integr. Cancer Ther. 18:153473541987877. doi: 10.1177/1534735419878770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, Y., Han, S., and Gelfand, M. J. (2017). The role of gamma interbrain synchrony in social coordination when humans face territorial threats. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1614–1623. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self Iden.: J. Int. Soc. Self Iden. 2, 223–250. doi: 10.1080/15298860309027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulos, S., Elison, J., and Lennon, R. (2004). The hierarchical structure of the interpersonal reactivity index. Soc. Behav. Pers. 32, 355–359. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2004.32.4.355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinero, D. A., Dikker, S., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2021). Inter-brain synchrony in teams predicts collective performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 43–57. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindler, S., and Pfattheicher, S. (2021). When it really counts: investigating the relation between trait mindfulness and actual prosocial behavior. Current Psychology, doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01860-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., and Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J. Behav. Med. 21, 581–599. doi: 10.1023/A:1018700829825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simony, E., Honey, C. J., Chen, J., Lositsky, O., Yeshurun, Y., Wiesel, A., et al. (2016). Dynamic reconfiguration of the default mode network during narrative comprehension. Nat. Commun. 7:12141. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M., et al. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. Int. J. Psycho. 55, 199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, V. A., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, E., Roffe-Vidal, S., et al. (2011). Impact of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. NeuroImage 57, 1524–1533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. T. (2012). Does personal distress mediate the effect of mindfulness on professional quality of life? Adv. Soc. Work 13, 561–585. doi: 10.18060/2600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, B. L., and Waltz, J. (2007). “Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation: overlapping constructs or not?” Personal. Individ. Differ. 43: 1875–1885, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmons, A. C., Margolin, G., and Saxbe, D. E. (2015). Physiological linkage in couples and its implications for individual and interpersonal functioning: a literature review. J. Fam. Psychol. 29, 720–731. doi: 10.1037/fam0000115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vago, D. R., and Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valencia, A. L., and Froese, T. (2020). What binds us? Inter-brain neural synchronization and its implications for theories of human consciousness. Neuro. Cons. 2020:niaa010. doi: 10.1093/nc/niaa010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dam,, Nicholas, T., Earleywine, M., and Borders, A. (2010). Measuring mindfulness? An item response theory analysis of the mindful attention awareness scale. Personal. Individ. Differ. 49, 805–810. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallot, S., Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., and Roepstorff, A. (2016). Beyond synchrony: joint action in a complex production task reveals beneficial effects of decreased interpersonal synchrony. PLoS One 11:e0168306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wass, S. V., Smith, C. G., Clackson, K., Gibb, C., Eitzenberger, J., and Mirza, F. U. (2019). Parents mimic and influence their Infant’s autonomic state through dynamic affective state matching. Curr. Biol. 29, 2415–22.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1994). “The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule - expanded form.” University of Iowa.

Google Scholar

Wong, K. F., Teng, J., Chee, M. W. L., Doshi, K., and Lim, J. (2018). Positive effects of mindfulness-based training on energy maintenance and the EEG correlates of sustained attention in a cohort of nurses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:80. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mindfulness, empathy, interpersonal relations, interbrain synchrony, social neuroscience, naturalistic interaction

Citation: Chen P, Kirk U and Dikker S (2022) Trait mindful awareness predicts inter-brain coupling but not individual brain responses during naturalistic face-to-face interactions. Front. Psychol. 13:915345. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.915345

Received: 07 April 2022; Accepted: 13 September 2022;
Published: 30 September 2022.

Edited by:

Michele Scandola, University of Verona, Italy

Reviewed by:

Mingming Zhang, Shanghai Normal University, China
Zhishan Hu, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
Christopher A. Was, Kent State University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Chen, Kirk and Dikker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Phoebe Chen, hc2896@nyu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.