Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 October 2020
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): Psychological Reactions to the Pandemic View all 66 articles

Extraversion Moderates the Relationship Between the Stringency of COVID-19 Protective Measures and Depressive Symptoms

  • 1Erasmus Happiness Economics Research Organization, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Erasmus School of Health and Policy Management, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Erasmus School of Economics, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands

From the start of the COVID-19 pandemic, psychologists are theorizing that, as compared to introverts, extraverts experience more profound negative social consequences from protective measures (e.g., travel restrictions and bans on public gatherings). As the empirical evidence for this claim is lacking, this study tested the hypothesis that extraversion moderates the relationship between the stringency of COVID-19 protective measures and depressive symptoms. Our results were based on survey data from 93,125 respondents collected in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic (March 20–April 6, 2020) across 47 countries and publicly available data on measure stringency. Findings demonstrate that extraversion moderates the relationship between measure stringency in the early days of the pandemic and depressive symptoms. For introverts, measure stringency has a negative effect on depressive symptoms, while for extraverts, it has a positive, but non-significant effect on depressive symptoms. This study suggests that, although stringent measures generally help people to worry less and feel safer, the lifestyle associated with such measures feels more natural to introverts than to extraverts.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has a profound negative effect on the world population’s physical and mental health (Dong et al., 2020; Van Bavel et al., 2020; World Health Organization, 2020). In varying degrees, governments all over the world imposed protective measures to contain the spread of the virus (Anderson et al., 2020; Hale et al., 2020). For instance, the Belarusian government dismissed the global coronavirus pandemic and imposed only a handful of measures. The Swedish government installed a larger number of measures but refrained from imposing a lockdown. Governments from China and Italy swiftly installed a total lockdown of the entire country.

Recent research during the early stages of the pandemic suggests that stringent measures also function as safeguard of mental health; they cause citizens to worry less and feel safer (Fetzer et al., 2020). This does not mean that protective measures bring nothing but benefits. An increasing degree of stringency of COVID-19 protective measures is typically accompanied by increased social distancing, the limiting of face-to-face contact with others by keeping space between oneself and other people outside of one’s home. Inherent by-products of social distancing are increased feelings of loneliness, frustration, worry and boredom—negative emotional states that, if left unattended, could lead to mental illness (Brooks et al., 2020; Folk et al., 2020; Galea et al., 2020).

Yet, it is unlikely that the effects of social consequences of measure stringency on mental illness are universal across all people. Drawing from pre-pandemic research, psychologists are proposing that extraversion—a personality trait characterized by sociability, assertiveness and high energy levels (John et al., 1991; Soto and John, 2017)—is one individual characteristic that could moderate the negative relationship between measure stringency and mental illness (e.g., Brogaard, 2020; Brooks and Moser, 2020; Smillie and Haslam, 2020; Steele, 2020). More specifically, they argue that there are potential advantages to being an introvert and potential disadvantages to being an extravert in countries where stringent measures are in place. The lifestyle associated with social distancing would feel more unnatural to extraverts than to introverts, as it inhibits extraverts to satisfy their strong urges to seek out social engagement (Woodcock et al., 2013), to experience pleasure and excitement (Kämpfe and Mitte, 2009), and to live in new and exciting surroundings (Oishi and Choi, 2020). Introverts, in contrast, would fare better, as the lifestyle allows them to shamelessly be alone more often and decide when and where to connect with others.

To date, however, the assumption that the social consequences of measure stringency are negative for extraverts and positive for introverts, remains largely untested. The first empirical tests based on data collected during the pandemic are inconclusive, with studies reporting negative (Płomecka et al., 2020), positive (Folk et al., 2020), or insignificant (Elmer et al., 2020; Weinstein and Nguyen, 2020) associations between extraversion and mental illness. In this study, drawing on publicly available survey data from over 90,000 respondents across 47 countries (Fetzer et al., 2020), we therefore empirically test the hypothesis that extraversion moderates the relationship between measure stringency and depressive symptoms. By looking at moderation effects, we aimed to further nuance Fetzer et al.’s (2020) finding that measure stringency leads to reduced depressive symptoms. We also address a general calls for research on the mental health effects of COVID-19 protective measures (Holmes et al., 2020; Van Bavel et al., 2020) and more specific calls for investigations on the interplay between personality, the experience of social distancing and mental health (Folk et al., 2020; Oosterhoff et al., 2020).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

In this study, we utilized Fetzer et al.’s (2020) data. They used online snowball sampling to recruit respondents in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic (March 20–April 6, 2020), a period in which the pandemic spread rapidly, and many consequential policy decisions were made. In total, 112,136 respondents from 175 countries filled out the survey. Following recommendations by Fetzer et al. (2020), we only included the countries in which more than 200 people participated, resulting in 47 countries and a sample of 93,125 respondents. In our sample, 44% was male, with an average age of 39.1 years (SD = 13.0) and average of 16.4 years of education (SD = 4.7). More details on the countries, the number of observations per day and respondents can be found in Table 1, Figure 1, and Table 2, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of the countries in the analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of observations per day.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and Pearson correlation matrix (N = 93,125).

Measures

Descriptive statistics on all variables and a description of all variables and can be found in Tables 2, 3, respectively.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Description of variables.

Measure Stringency

The stringency of measures across country and time was measured using the COVID-19 Government Response Stringency Index (GRSI), that is up to date as of April 6, 2020 (Hale et al., 2020). The GRSI is comprised of sub-indexes on nine categories of protective measures: workplace closings, cancelation of public events, restrictions on gathering size, closing of public transport, stay at home requirements, restrictions on internal movement, restrictions on international travel and presence of public information campaigns. All sub-indexes differed in their scaling. For example, cancelation of public events had three categories: 0 (no measure), 1 (recommend cancelling) and 2 (require cancelling) and school closings had four categories: 0 (no measures), 1 (recommended closing), 2 (require closing, only some levels or categories) and 3 (require closing, require closing all levels or categories). Therefore, all sub-index scores were re-coded onto a 1–100 scale. These scores were then averaged into a single aggregate score ranging from 1 (no measures) to 100 (total lockdown). For interpretability purposes, Hale et al.’s (2020) original scale was recoded into a continuous scale from 0 to 1.

Extraversion

Extraversion was measured using the two-item measure from the Ten-Item Personality Inventory [TIPI; Spearman’s rho (ρ) = 0.53, Gosling et al., 2003]. The two items represent both poles of the extraversion dimension: “I see myself as extraverted, enthusiastic” and “I see myself as reserved, quiet.” Answer categories ranged from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). The measure was constructed by reverse coding the score on the “Reversed, quiet” item and computing an average score of the two items.

Depressive Symptoms

Depressive symptoms were measured using the average score respondents scored on the 8-item Personal Health Questionnaire (PHQ-8), e.g., “How often have you been bothered by the following over the past 2 weeks?…Little interest or pleasure in doing things” (α = 0.86, ω = 0.88, Kroenke et al., 2001; for validation in the general population, see Martin et al., 2006). Answer categories ranged from 1 (not at all) to 4 (nearly every day).

Covariates

We included several covariates that could confound the relationship between the stringency of measures, extraversion and depressive symptoms. In addition to typical demographic variables like age, gender, monthly household income, marital status and years of education, we also considered the 2-item TIPI measures of neuroticism, openness, conscientiousness and agreeableness (ρs ranging from 0.18 to 0.52), trust in government, health problems, household composition, and participation in social gatherings over the past 5 days as covariates. At the country-day level, we controlled for the number of and day-to-day change in COVID-19 cases and the number of deaths per capita (see Dong et al., 2020).

Analytical Strategy

To examine the moderating effect of extraversion on the relationship between measure stringency and depressive symptoms, we combine individual-level and country-level data and utilize a difference-in-difference analysis. Following Fetzer et al. (2020), we use the reghdfe package in Stata (Correia, 2016), which estimates linear regression models absorbing multiple levels (i.e., country-individual and time) of fixed effects. The advantage of a fixed model over a multilevel (random) effects model is that which takes out individual-specific heterogeneity (country-education and country-age-gender) at the country-level as well as (global) day-specific shocks.

We estimate the following regressions for all individuals from countries with at least 200 respondents who responded to the survey in the period March 20–April 6, 2020:

(1)DepressiveSymptomsijt=      β1*Measurestringencyjt+β2*Extraversionijt+      β3*Measurestringencyjt*Extraversionijt+γ1*Xijt+ γ2*Xit+      vj1+vj2+vt+εijt, 

where DepressiveSymptomsijt is depressive symptoms score of individual i in country j that responded to the survey on day t, Extraversionijt is an individual’s score on the extraversion index, and MeasureStringencyjt is degree of restrictions citizens have to face in country j on day t. Xijt is a vector of individual-level control variables including income-level, marital status, comorbidities, and other personality characteristics, while Xit is a vector of country-level control variables including day-to-day change in COVID-19 cases and deaths per capita and the number of COVID-19 cases and the number of deaths per capita. In addition, we include country-education (vj1), country-age-gender (vj2) and day fixed effects (vt). Accordingly, we utilize the within-variation of people with certain characteristics that live within a particular country over time.

As some respondents filled out the questionnaire before strict measures were in place and others answered after countries’ lockdown, we can gauge to what extent changes in stringency measures differently affect extraverts and introverts’ mental health. In our estimations, standard errors are clustered by country-age and gender of the respondents. Weights were included to correct for socio-demographic differences between survey respondents and the general population in each country and differences in population size between countries (also see, Fetzer et al., 2020).

Results

In line with our hypothesis and as exhibited in Table 4, extraversion moderated the relationship between measure stringency and depressive symptoms (β = 0.24, p < 0.05; Table 4, Model 2). Our conclusion holds when we control for individual-country and country-level control variables (β = 0.178, p < 0.05; Table 4, Model 3). Although extraversion is negatively related to depressive symptoms (β = −0.06, p < 0.01; Table 4, Model 1), for introverts, measure stringency has a negative effect on depressive symptoms, whereas, for extraverts, measure stringency has a positive, but not statistically significant effect depressive symptoms (see Figure 2). As an illustration, if the measure stringency index increases from 0 to 1, the depressive symptoms of extreme introverts decrease with 0.70 points (95% CI: −1.35 to −0.05), while they increase with 0.37 points for extreme extraverts (95% CI: −0.15 to 0.89). Model 3 in Table 4 also shows that being single or divorced, having health problems, having low trust in government, and having high degrees of neuroticism and conscientiousness (and to a lesser degree openness) are important correlates of reporting depressive symptoms in the early days of the pandemic.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of regression analyses.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visualization of extraversion moderating the relation between stringency measures and depressive symptoms. CI, confidence interval. Based on Estimations in Table 4, Model 3.

Discussion

Our findings provide support for the hypothesis that extraversion moderates the relationship between stringent COVID-19 protective measures and depressive symptoms. The assumption that the stringent measures are beneficial for introverts and detrimental for extraverts received only partial support. The results indicated that introverts indeed fare substantially better when living in a country that has installed stringent protective measures than in countries that did not. However, even though the lifestyle associated with social distancing seems to feel more unnatural to extraverts than to introverts, the damaging effect of living in a country where the government imposed stringent measures appears to be limited for extraverts. This conclusion is underscored by the negative association between extraversion and depressive symptoms in our sample. Indeed, psychological research indicates that, compared to introverts, extraverts are less susceptible for mental illnesses (Malouff et al., 2005), such as depression and anxiety (Spinhoven et al., 2014), and generally happier (Steel et al., 2008; Anglim et al., 2020).

Limitations and Future Research

These findings should be interpreted within the limitations of this research. First, we were not able to empirically test our assumption that it is the negative social consequences of stringent measures that explain the slight increase of depressive symptoms among extraverts. We, however, believe that this is the most plausible explanatory mechanism, as research suggests that it is the loss in social connection that causes extraverts to suffer more mentally during this pandemic (Folk et al., 2020).

Second, even though our analytical strategy allowed us to take out individual-specific heterogeneity, and extraversion is a relatively stable personality trait (Damian et al., 2019), the cross-sectional nature of the survey data, collected in the early days of the pandemic, did not allow us to examine whether introverts’ and extraverts’ responses to the protective measures changed as the situation evolved. Since the period of data collection, time has not stood still. For example, after the first wave of infections was contained, countries started relaxing protective measures. A while later, many of these countries again imposed protective measures to prevent a second wave of infections to crop up. Testing our hypothesis on more recent data is an important direction for future research for, at least, two reasons. Research on well-being set-points and coping in times of crisis suggests that people have the tendency to adapt to adversity as a crisis evolves (Riolli et al., 2002; Cummins and Wooden, 2014). If, in time, extraverts find new ways to satisfy their need for social connections (e.g., virtual communication), the interaction effect could disappear. Furthermore, the charm of social distancing for introverts may be only temporal, because, if social distancing becomes the new normal, introverts may struggle with getting sufficient social support (Blue, 2020).

Third, with a broader set of measures, we would have been able to draw more robust conclusions. As extraversion is a multi-facetted construct (Soto and John, 2017) and not all facets contribute to mental health in equal degrees (Margolis et al., 2020), it could be that measure stringency only significantly interacts with one or two facets of extraversion. In a similar vein, it could be that the moderating effect of extraversion effects would have be more apparent for more fluctuant mental health constructs, such as daily positive and negative affect (Hudson et al., 2017). In addition, type of house and living situation could be interesting variables to consider, as people living in a more spacious house or more rural areas might have had more opportunity to organize social gatherings at a safe distance and maintain a high degree of personal space vis-à-vis other household members and, in turn, suffered less from the social consequences of the pandemic. Researching the role of daily time use would be a worthwhile endeavor too, as research conducted during the early days of COVID-19 shows that activities vary drastically in the extent to which they make people happy (Lades et al., 2020). Finally, we believe that studying the role of internet availability and familiarity with virtual communication media could be a fruitful research direction, as these factors could be essential for people to maintain social contacts when facing stringent measures.

Fourth, the surveying procedure may have influenced the external validity of our findings. First, Fetzer et al.’s (2020) snowballing procedure may have resulted in certain populations to be overrepresented (e.g., women) or underrepresented in our sample (e.g., individuals in lower social strata). Even though weights were used to correct for socio-demographic differences between survey respondents and the general population in each country, still some groups might be completely absent. Most notably, by administrating a web-based survey, Fetzer et al. (2020) excluded individuals that do not have access to the internet (e.g., underprivileged people) or lack the knowledge to use it (e.g., elderly people, Baltar and Brunet, 2012). It is perhaps this overlooked proportion of the population that may have been most negatively affected by the social consequences of the pandemic, as it had limited opportunity to maintain social relationships when physical contact was infeasible. Therefore, we recommend researchers to use data based on probability sampling methods and a variety of survey modes (e.g., paper or telephone survey) when replicating our study in future research.

Conclusion

All in all, our results provide empirical evidence on a popular, but mostly unsubstantiated assumption that extraverts suffer more from COVID-19 protective measures than introverts. Nevertheless, as, in the end, extraverts and introverts both have an innate need for human connection (Baumeister and Leary, 1995), it will be essential to develop and test interventions that help people to cope with the pandemic’s social consequences (Steele, 2020). It may, for instance, be worthwhile to develop public information programs that incentive citizens to adhere the COVID-19 protective measures and, at the same time, to help people maintain social relationships and stay mentally fit, e.g., combining outdoor activities with social interaction (Lades et al., 2020) and making responsible use of virtual communication tools to stay in touch (Garfin, 2020).

Data Availability Statement

The data and code used for this study can be found at https://osf.io/vgkmd/. The original data from Fetzer et al. (2020) can be found at https://osf.io/3sn2k/.

Ethics Statement

The data collection procedure was reviewed and approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (reference: E-206, see Fetzer et al., 2020). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

IW wrote most of the manuscript and verified the results. SS wrote a part of the manuscript and verified the results. MB ran most of the analyses and came up with the research question. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) (Grant No. 652.001.003).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anderson, R. M., Heesterbeek, H., Klinkenberg, D., and Hollingsworth, T. D. (2020). How will country-based mitigation measures influence the course of the COVID-19 epidemic? Lancet 395, 931–934. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30567-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anglim, J., Horwood, S., Smillie, L. D., Marrero, R. J., and Wood, J. K. (2020). Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146:279. doi: 10.1037/bul0000226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltar, F., and Brunet, I. (2012). Social research 2.0: virtual snowball sampling method using Facebook. Intern. Res. 22, 57–74. doi: 10.1108/10662241211199960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117:497. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blue, A. (2020). Do Introverts Have the Edge in Social Distancing? Maybe Not, Psychologist Says. Available online at: https://uanews.arizona.edu/story/do-introverts-have-edge-social-distancing-maybe-not-psychologist-says (accessed March 26, 2020).

Google Scholar

Brogaard, B. (2020). The Introvert Advantage During Lockdown. Available online at: https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/the-mysteries-love/202003/the-introvert-advantage-during-lockdown (accessed March 30, 2020)

Google Scholar

Brooks, C., and Moser, J. (2020). Surviving or Thriving? Enduring COVID-19 as an Introvert and Extrovert. Available online at: https://msutoday.msu.edu/news/2020/surviving-or-thriving-enduring-covid-19-as-an-introvert-and-extrovert/ (accessed April 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, S. (2016). A Feasible Estimator for Linear Models with Multi-Way Fixed Effects. Avaialable online at: http://scorreia.com/research/hdfe.pdf (accessed June 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A., and Wooden, M. (2014). Personal resilience in times of crisis: the implications of SWB homeostasis and set-points. J. Happ. Stud. 15, 223–235. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9481-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, R. I., Spengler, M., Sutu, A., and Roberts, B. W. (2019). Sixteen going on sixty-six: a longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117:674. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, E., Du, H., and Gardner, L. (2020). An interactive web-based dashboard to track COVID-19 in real time. Lancet Infect. Dis. 20, 533–534. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30120-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elmer, T., Mepham, K., and Stadtfeld, C. (2020). Students Under Lockdown: Assessing Change in Students’ Social Networks and Mental Health During the COVID-19 Crisis. Available online at: https://psyarxiv.com/ua6tq/ (accessed June 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Fetzer, T., Witte, M., Hensel, L., Jachimowicz, J., Haushofer, J., Ivchenko, A., et al. (2020). Global Behaviors and Perceptions in the COVID-19 Pandemic. Availavle online at: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3kfmh (accessed June 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Folk, D., Okabe-Miyamoto, K., Dunn, E., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2020). Did social connection decline during the first wave of COVID-19?: the role of extraversion. Collabra Psychol. 6:37. doi: 10.1525/collabra.365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galea, S., Merchant, R. M., and Lurie, N. (2020). The mental health consequences of COVID-19 and physical distancing: the need for prevention and early intervention. JAMA Intern. Med. 180, 817–818. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2020.1562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfin, D. R. (2020). Technology as a coping tool during the COVID-19 pandemic: implications and recommendations. Stress Health doi: 10.1002/smi.2975 [Epub ahesd of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., and Swann, W. B. Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Pers. 37, 504–528. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, T., Petherick, A., Phillips, T., and Webster, S. (2020). “Variation in government responses to COVID-19,” in Blavatnik School of Government Working Paper, 31. Available online at: www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/covidtracker (accessed June 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., O’Connor, R. C., Perry, V. H., Tracey, I., Wessely, S., Arseneault, L., et al. (2020). Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 547–560. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudson, N. W., Lucas, R. E., and Donnellan, M. B. (2017). Day-to-day affect is surprisingly stable: a 2-year longitudinal study of well-being. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 8, 45–54. doi: 10.1177/1948550616662129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). “Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues,” in Handbook of personality: Theory and research, eds O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 114–158.

Google Scholar

Kämpfe, N., and Mitte, K. (2009). What you wish is what you get? The meaning of individual variability in desired affect and affective discrepancy. J. Res. Pers. 43, 409–418. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., and Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 16, 606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lades, K. L., Laffan, K., Daly, M., and Delaney, L. (2020). Daily emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Br. J. Health Psychol. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12450 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., and Schutte, N. S. (2005). The relationship between the five-factor model of personality and symptoms of clinical disorders: a meta-analysis. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 27, 101–114. doi: 10.1007/s10862-005-5384-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Margolis, S., Stapley, A. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2020). The association between Extraversion and well-being is limited to one facet. J. Pers. 88, 478–484. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Rief, W., Klaiberg, A., and Braehler, E. (2006). Validity of the brief patient health questionnaire mood scale (PHQ-9) in the general population. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 28, 71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, S., and Choi, H. (2020). Personality and space: introversion and seclusion. J. Res. Pers. 85:103933. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103933

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oosterhoff, B., Palmer, C. A., Wilson, J., and Shook, N. (2020). Adolescents’ motivations to engage in social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: associations with mental and social health. J. Adolesc. Health 67, 179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Płomecka, M. B., Gobbi, S., Neckels, R., Radziński, P., Skórko, B., Lazzeri, S., et al. (2020). Mental Health Impact of COVID-19: A Global Study of Risk and Resilience Factors. Available online at: https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.05.05.20092023v1.full.pdf (accessed June 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Riolli, L., Savicki, V., and Cepani, A. (2002). Resilience in the face of catastrophe: optimism, personality, and coping in the Kosovo crisis. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 1604–1627. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02765.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smillie, L. D., and Haslam, N. (2020). Personalities that Thrive in Isolation and What We Can All Learn from Time Alone. Parkville: The Conversation.

Google Scholar

Soto, C. J., and John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113:117. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinhoven, P., Elzinga, B. M., van Hemert, A. M., de Rooij, M., and Penninx, B. W. (2014). A longitudinal study of facets of extraversion in depression and social anxiety. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 71, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, P., Schmidt, J., and Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 134:138. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, J. (2020). Be Proactive About Mental Health During COVID Isolation, Clinical Psychologist Says. Huntsville: The University of Alabama in Huntsville.

Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N., and Nguyen, T. (2020). Motivation and preference in isolation: a test of their different influences on responses to self-isolation during the COVID-19 outbreak. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7:200458. doi: 10.1098/rsos.200458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodcock, A., Graziano, W. G., Branch, S. E., Habashi, M. M., Ngambeki, I., and Evangelou, D. (2013). Person and thing orientations: psychological correlates and predictive utility. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 4, 116–123. doi: 10.1177/1948550612444320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2020). Mental Health and Psychosocial Considerations During the COVID-19 Outbreak. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, COVID-19 protective measures, extraversion, depressive symptoms, mental health—related quality of life

Citation: Wijngaards I, Sisouw de Zilwa SCM and Burger MJ (2020) Extraversion Moderates the Relationship Between the Stringency of COVID-19 Protective Measures and Depressive Symptoms. Front. Psychol. 11:568907. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568907

Received: 02 June 2020; Accepted: 08 September 2020;
Published: 02 October 2020.

Edited by:

Joanna Sokolowska, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Poland

Reviewed by:

Valerio Capraro, Middlesex University, United Kingdom
Urszula Wolski, Buckinghamshire New University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Wijngaards, Sisouw de Zilwa and Burger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Indy Wijngaards, wijngaards@ese.eur.nl; wijngaards@eshpm.eur.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.