Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 August 2020
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic The Patient’s Change: Understanding the Complexity of the Dynamics of Change and its Precursors in Psychotherapy View all 10 articles

A Mixed Methods Framework for Psychoanalytic Group Therapy: From Qualitative Records to a Quantitative Approach Using T-Pattern, Lag Sequential, and Polar Coordinate Analyses

  • 1FPCEE Blanquerna, Ramon Llull University, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Conducted within a mixed methods framework, this study focuses on the conversation-facilitation role of a lead therapist during group psychotherapy with adolescents. Conversation is an essential component of psychoanalytic psychotherapies and there is growing interest in describing and studying the impact of conversational techniques. One way to do this is to report on specific approaches, such as questioning, paraphrasing, and mentalization in intervention turns and to analyze their impact on the therapist-patient relationship. The main aim of this study was to investigate differences in communication strategies used by a lead therapist in the early and late stages of therapy with six adolescents aged 13–15 years. We employed a mixed methods design based on systematic direct observation supplemented by indirect observation. The observational methodology design was nomothetic, follow-up, and multidimensional. The choice of methodology is justified by our use of an ad hoc observation instrument for communication strategies combining a field format and a category system. We analyzed interobserver agreement quantitatively by Cohen’s kappa using GSEQ5 software. Following confirmation of the reliability of the data, we analyzed the lead therapist’s conversation-facilitation techniques in sessions 5 and 29 of a 30-session program by quantitatively analyzing what were initially qualitative data using T-pattern detection (THEME v.6 Edu software), lag sequential analysis (GSEQ5 software), and polar coordinate analysis (HOISAN v. 1.6.3.3.6. software and R software). The results show changes in the techniques used from the start to the end of therapy. Of the 28 communication strategies analyzed, three were particularly common: questioning and paraphrasing in session 5 and questioning and mentalization in session 29. This mixed methods study shows that combined use of T-pattern detection, lag sequential analysis, and polar coordinate analysis can offer meaningful and objective insights into group psychotherapy through the lens of the therapist.

Introduction

The use of mixed methods in psychotherapy research has grown in recent years (Bartholomew and Lockard, 2018; Del Giacco et al., 2019, 2020; Halfon et al., 2019; Roberts and Allen, 2019; Venturella et al., 2019). Researchers working as psychotherapists have underlined the need to integrate the objectivity of quantitative methods with the creativity of qualitative and mixed methods in order “to put the flesh of clinical meaning on the bones of quantitative outcomes” (Target, 2018, p. 18). Psychotherapy research covers a field of great complexity. Some of this flexibility can be captured and understood through the analysis of qualitative and quantitative data within a mixed methods design to shed light on what lies beneath multimodal interactions that precede change in psychotherapy. A mixed methods design offers an objective and scientifically rigorous yet flexible approach for capturing change and continuity over the course of psychotherapy. An observational methodology is perfectly suited to the study of spontaneous behavior in natural settings (Anguera et al., 2018), and as such, is ideal for analyzing the regular interactions that occur between therapists and patients in a range of psychotherapy settings (individual, group, or family therapy) and, more specifically, in psychoanalytic therapy settings (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, in press). The process for analyzing change in psychotherapy is well established and plenty of opportunities exist within this process (from the definition of the research question to the interpretation of systematically collected and recorded data) to integrate both qualitative and quantitative elements.

Conventionally speaking, mixed methods studies integrate qualitative and quantitative perspectives (Johnson et al., 2007; Creswell and Plano Clark, 2017; Onwuegbuzie et al., 2018) in drawing on all types of data, including observational datasets, narratives, in-depth interviews, survey results, and measures from physiological and other tests, including repeated measures taken over the course of a single study. The means by which these data can be linked has grown exponentially, giving rise to numerous conceptual nuances, a long list of study designs, and a future that promises to end decades of methodological confrontation.

The very essence of the observational methodology consists of seeking complementarity through the integration of qualitative and quantitative elements. A key strength of the observational methodology is that it rigorously guarantees quality through the objective analysis of rigorously collected and processed qualitative data that can be analyzed robustly and quantitatively without loss of information richness (Anguera et al., in press).

Observational methodology is thus itself a mixed methods approach (Anguera et al., 2017a). Although relatively novel, it has shown enormous promise and is growing in popularity in a range of scientific fields, including psychology. In their review of mixed methods in psychotherapy research, Bartholomew and Lockard (2018) reported that a considerable proportion (32.26%) of these studies have focused on group interventions. While most studies have been conducted in adults, some have been conducted in adolescents (Down et al., 2011) and children with emotional and behavioral problems in groups (Swank and Shin, 2015) and individual psychodynamic play therapy (Halfon et al., 2016). Our group has used observational methodology to analyze group psychotherapy in previous studies (Vaimberg, 2010; Roustan et al., 2013; Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017; Alcover et al., 2019) and in the empirical part of this article. In the latter we demonstrate the different steps involved in the QUAL-QUAN-QUAL transformation of data and show how the “connecting” method (Creswell and Plano Clark, 2017) is an ideal way to link qualitative and quantitative elements within a systematic observation framework.

There is growing interest in describing and studying the impact of psychoanalytic therapy techniques (Midgley et al., 2018) from different perspectives, with researchers continuing to search for answers to the question “what works for whom?” (Fonagy et al., 2015). Different approaches to responding to this question have been adopted: some studies have taken a microanalytical approach based on the psychophysiological responses of therapists and patients (Steffen et al., 2014; Kleinbub, 2017), while others have analyzed the impact of therapist empathy and challenge on psychophysiological responses from patients (Voutilainen et al., 2018).

From a mixed methods perspective this question can be addressed by analyzing communication and therapeutic discourse interactions using an ad hoc observation instrument (Arias-Pujol et al., 2015; Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017; Del Giacco et al., 2019, 2020). Conversation and the therapeutic alliance are essential components of psychoanalytic therapy (Manzano et al., 2018). In the case of adolescents, creating a therapeutic alliance is crucial to preventing therapy dropout (O’Keefe et al., 2018). As the therapy unfolds, an alliance is formed between the therapist and participants (Tanzilli et al., 2018) that allows the therapist to communicate increasingly complex content concerning the here-and-now of the sessions. In the early stages of therapy, this intrapsychic content may not be understood by the patient, and might even lead to resistance, especially in adolescents (Oetzel and Scherer, 2003; Lavik et al., 2017, 2018); it can only emerge once a climate of trust has been created (Sagen et al., 2013). Interventions of this type are aimed at increasing the patient’s capacity for mentalization, which is a process by which people make sense of themselves and each other (Fonagy, 1991). In group sessions, therapist interventions designed to build capacity for mentalization show that behaviors are motivated by emotions, thoughts, fantasies, and wishes. Although it is recognized that everybody has their own mind, the group participants come to see the benefits of sharing points of view and empathizing with other people’s experiences (Torras de Beà, 2013). A previous study by our group (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017) that analyzed conversation turn-taking in adolescent group therapy showed that four main roles were played by the lead therapist: (1) she did not facilitate interventions by all group members uniformly, (2) she encouraged turn-taking from more inhibited participants, (3) she facilitated conversation from the early stages of therapy, and (4) she promoted the capacity to mentalize toward the end of therapy.

The aim of this new study, conducted within a mixed methods framework, was to investigate potential differences in the communication strategies used by a lead therapist in earlier and later stages of therapy. The specific aim was to use T-pattern detection, lag sequential analysis, and polar coordinate analysis to detect changes in the communication flow between a lead therapist and her patients (in this case, adolescents), analyzing the specific techniques used and their impact on the therapist-patient relationship.

Materials and Methods

Design

The specific design was nomothetic/follow-up/multidimensional (N/F/M) (Anguera et al., 2001; Sánchez-Algarra and Anguera, 2013). It was nomothetic because we observed different participants (lead therapist, co-therapist, and group members), follow-up because we analyzed two sessions and their content (one session each from the beginning and the end of the therapy), and multidimensional because we analyzed 15 communication strategy dimensions using an ad hoc observation instrument (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017). Both direct observation (Sánchez-Algarra and Anguera, 2013) and indirect observation (Anguera et al., 2018) techniques were used. The observation was participative, given that the psychotherapist interacted with the adolescents. The recommendations of the Guidelines for Reporting Evaluations based on Observational Methodology (GREOM) (Portell et al., 2015) and the Methodological Quality Checklist for Studies based on Observational Methodology (MQCOM) (Chacón-Moscoso et al., 2019) were followed.

Participants

The group therapy sessions were conducted in the Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation (FETB) Center for Child and Adolescent Mental Health (Barcelona, Spain) with six adolescents (four boys and two girls) aged 13–15 years, an expert lead therapist, and a co-therapist. All the adolescents had difficulties with learning and interpersonal relationships.

This research forms part of a broader project involving an 8-month intervention developed to enhance the mentalization and communication capacities of adolescents, whose parents attended parallel sessions on parenting. The goal of the therapists was to facilitate interaction among all the group members by creating an atmosphere of emotional security and support (Torras de Beà, 2013).

Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of the minors in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Code of the General Council of the Spanish Official College of Psychologists. Approval by an ethics committee was not required as per applicable institutional and national guidelines and regulations. The participants were informed that they were being filmed and agreed accordingly, and were shown the location of the video cameras, positioned discretely to minimize reactivity bias. They were guaranteed that their identity and privacy would be protected at all times. For this, pseudonyms were used in the transcripts and encodings of the material. The study was approved by the head of the Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation (FETB) Research Department. Regulatory provisions regarding clinical research in humans of the European Union (Good Clinical Practice for Trials on Medicinal Products in the European Community: EEC 111/3976/88-EN) and of Spain (Royal Decree 561/1993) were applied.

Instruments

Observation Instrument

The observation instrument, developed ad hoc for a previous study, combined field format and several category systems (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017). The category systems were built on 15 dimensions proposed for analysis of communication strategies. A category system was built from each dimension (except for some single-category dimensions) that fulfilled the requirements of exhaustiveness and mutual exclusivity. The ‘turn’ dimension was observed directly, while the other 14 dimensions were observed indirectly. A total of 28 categories resulted for the dimensions (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Dimensions and category systems in the observation instrument for therapists and patients (adapted from Arias-Pujol and Anguera (2017).

Recording and Analysis Instruments

The recording instrument used was the freeware GSEQ5, v.5.21 (Bakeman and Quera, 1996, 2011), which allowed the sessions to be coded in accordance with the observation instrument. The obtained data were type II data (Bakeman, 1978), and, therefore, concurrent and event-based. GSEQ5 was also used to calculate agreement.

Regarding the analyses, T-patterns were detected using the freeware THEME v.6 Edu2 (Magnusson, 1996, 2000, 2020), lag sequential analysis was performed using GSEQ5, polar coordinates were analyzed using the freeware3 HOISAN v. 1.6.3.3.6. (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012), and vectors were graphed using R (Rodríguez-Medina et al., 2019).

Procedure

This research was part of a group psychotherapy program consisting of 30 sessions, 24 of which were transcribed to capture conversation turn-taking. To delimit the observation unit, we used interlocutor and syntactic criteria in a complementary manner (Anguera, in press; Krippendorff, 2013). As mentioned, the data were type II data (Bakeman, 1978), which materialize code matrices as obtained in the quantitizing process; these qualitative data from the recording (see vignettes in Tables 2, 3) were systematized through observation-instrument coding and computerized recording. The code matrices contain rows (a separate row for each observation unit) that show the codes for co-occurrences of simultaneous behaviors for the different dimensions of the observation instrument. Quantitizing is crucial in the mixed methods framework (Anguera et al., 2017a; Anguera, 2020; Anguera et al., in press), as it enables access to the second QUAL-QUAN-QUAL phase; the fact that the code matrices are quantitatively analyzed allows for the crucial step that connects functions and that permits the integration of qualitative and quantitative elements (Plano Clark and Sanders, 2015), in such a way that initially qualitative data can be analyzed quantitatively (Anguera et al., 2018).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sample clinical vignette for the initial session.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sample clinical vignette for the final session.

The reliability of the data was confirmed by calculating Cohen (1960, 1968); the obtained values of between 0.897 and 0.939, according to Landis and Koch(1977, p. 165), can be interpreted as “almost perfect agreement”.

Data Analysis

For the current study, we compared the content of two sessions in order to showcase an innovative methodological development in group psychotherapy in which qualitative records from the two sessions underwent a powerful quantitative analysis within a mixed methods framework.

The first session was an early session (session 5, the first with the full group), while the second one was a session from the end of therapy (session 29, held 7 months later, just before the farewell/end-of-treatment session). Once the data had been validated and transformed into code matrices, sessions 5 and 29 were analyzed in depth using three scientifically grounded and specific categorical data techniques: T-pattern detection, lag sequential analysis, and polar coordinate analysis (with the therapist as the focal subject). These quantitative techniques are highly appropriate for the analysis of qualitative data collected by direct observation (Anguera et al., 2017b) and indirect observation (Anguera et al., 2018) and suitably organized in code matrices within the framework of a mixed methods study (Anguera et al., in press). To date, the three techniques have been applied in combination in the fields of education (Santoyo et al., 2017; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2019) and sport (Tarragó et al., 2017).

T-pattern detection

T-pattern detection was proposed and developed by Magnusson (1996, 2000, 2005, 2016, 2018, 2020). T-patterns, or temporal patterns, are essentially a combination of events that occur in the same order, separated by temporal distances that remain invariant over time. The basic premise of T-pattern detection is that the interactive flow or chain of behaviors consists of structures of variable stability that can be visualized through the detection of underlying T-patterns (Suárez et al., 2018; Portell et al., 2019; Santoyo et al., 2020). As indicated by Magnusson(2020, p. 2): “As a Mixed Methods approach, T-pattern analysis […] passes repeatedly between qualitative and quantitative analyses, from data collection logging the occurrences of qualities (categories) and their real-time (quantitative) locations resulting in time-stamped data, here T-data, to the detection of T-patterns (qualities) […], typically followed by both qualitative and quantitative analyses of the detected patterns.” T-pattern analysis involves the use of an algorithm that calculates temporal distances between codes of behaviors, analyzing the extent to which the critical interval remains invariant relative to the null hypothesis. It requires the use of systematized data (usually in the form of code matrices) for which the duration of each co-occurrence has been recorded (Anguera et al., 2018). As indicated by Magnusson (1996, 2000, 2020), a T-pattern, Q, consists of m ordered components, X1m, that are recurrent, where each temporal co-occurrence of behaviors (called event-types) is a T-data. A T-pattern can be characterized as follows, considering variations in distances between consecutive behaviors (Magnusson, 2020):

Q = X1 [d1,d2]1 X2 [d1,d2]2. Xi [d1,d2]i Xi + 1. Xm 1 [d1,d2] (m 1) Xm,

where X is an event-type or a T-pattern. The general term Xi [d1, d2]i Xi + 1 means that, within occurrences of the pattern, after Xi occurring at t statistically significantly more often than expected by chance, Xi + 1 occurs within interval [t + d1, t + d2], or short [d1, d2], called a critical interval (Anguera et al., in press).

Microanalyses are also possible and very useful (Anguera, 2005). These analyses are run in THEME v. 6 Edu, which offers different settings that can be modified to obtain complementary results. Combined analysis of these results can provide a better understanding of interactive transitions over time. THEME provides all the necessary features to analyze the data and presents the results graphically as dendrograms or tree diagrams.

Two parameters necessary for each analysis are the minimum number of occurrences and the level of significance. We set the minimum number of occurrences to 30 and the significance level to p < 0.005. Note that the method applied in this research was rather unconventional, as the temporal distance parameter was set to 1 in all cases. This method was chosen because of the nature of the data (type II).

While T-pattern detection has been used in a wide range of fields, including clinical psychology (Blanchet et al., 2005; Haynal-Reymond et al., 2005; Merten and Schwab, 2005; Plumet and Tardif, 2005; Horn and Magnusson, 2016; Woods et al., 2016), its application to group therapy with adolescents is novel.

Lag sequential analysis

This technique, proposed by Bakeman (1978), aims to detect the existence of patterns of behavior within categorical data corresponding to regular behaviors that are not due to random effects. Lag sequential analysis one or more given behaviors (any that, by hypothesis, are assumed to generate or initialize a behavior pattern), one or more conditional behaviors (for which we wish to test the existence of a statistical association with a given behavior), and lags (positive, negative, or both). Behaviors with positive and negative lags occur after and before the given behavior, respectively. The number of the lag indicates the order in which it occurs.

Lag sequential analysis can operate with five types of data: event sequence data, state sequence data, timed state sequence data, interval sequence data, and multi-event sequence data. The first four were designated by Bakeman (1978) and were later slightly modified by Bakeman and Quera (1996, 2011) when building the SDIS-GSEQ software (precursor of the current GSEQ5). A minimum of 30 data items (30 code matrix rows) is required for the results to be valid (Bakeman and Gottman, 1987). Since lag sequential analysis works with code matrices (Anguera et al., 2018), it can be used to detect regularities (patterns of behavior) that show the structure of interactive episodes (Bakeman, 1978, 1991; Bakeman and Gottman, 1987; Bakeman and Quera, 1996, 2011; Quera, 2018); this is very useful in clinical psychology, especially when we want to detect regularities at different points in time.

Once the conditional behaviors and lags of interest have been defined, as per Bakeman (1978), a matching frequencies table based on the gicen behavior is generated and this is then used to generate a probability table showing expected and conditional probabilities. Expected probabilities indicate the extent of random effects, while conditional probabilities provide the residual values that indicate whether or not the relationship with the given behavior is significant that at each lag. It is recommended to apply the adjustment proposed by Allison and Liker (1982), incorporated in SDIS-GSEQ, as it expresses the results as adjusted residuals.

Once the adjusted residuals have been obtained, the pattern (or patterns) of behavior is (are) “constructed,” starting with the proposed criterion behavior in each case. Each lag (whether positive or negative) will include the conditional behavior(s) with a significant adjusted residual value: >1.96 when the relationship is activation and <−1.96 when the relationship is inhibition (for a significance level of p < 0.05).

So that researchers can consider where the structure conventionally ends, i.e., to end the interpretation purposes of the obtained structure, interpretation guidelines should be applied (a) when there are no more lags with statistically significant behaviors, (b) when there are two consecutive empty lags, or (c) when there are several statistically significant behaviors in two consecutive lags and the first of the lags is considered the MAX LAG (Anguera et al., in press; Sackett, 1979).

Lag sequential analysis can be applied, in both direct and indirect observation, to a complete session, part of a session, parts of different sessions (e.g., the first few minutes of a series of sessions), or series of complete sessions. It therefore offers enormous flexibility in addressing different research questions. It requires data for which the sequence of occurrence of concurrent behaviors has been recorded and it can be run in any of the following programs: GSEQ5 v. 5.2 (Bakeman and Quera, 2011) and GSEQ5 (Bakeman and Quera, 2011), which allow various simultaneous criterion behaviors, or HOISAN v. 1.6.3.3 (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012), which only allows one criterion behavior.

Lag sequential analysis has been successfully applied in many direct and indirect observation studies conducted over the past 25 years in clinical psychology (e.g., Martínez del Pozo, 1993; Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2004, Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2005; Roustan et al., 2013; Arias-Pujol et al., 2015; Venturella et al., 2019; Del Giacco et al., 2020).

Polar coordinate analysis

Polar coordinate analysis, an analytical technique proposed by Sackett (1980), is based on building a map that shows the statistical association between different behavior codes, and specifically between a behavior that is considered central or core, called the focal behavior, and all other behaviors, called conditional behaviors. The goal is to determine if there is a relationship, and if there is one, its type and intensity. This technique, which considers as data the adjusted residuals obtained in the lag sequential analysis, complements the prospective (forward feeding) and retrospective (backward feeding) perspectives, allowing us to observe how the relationship between focal behavior and conditional behaviors evolves over time. This analysis is therefore based on prospective and retrospective perspectives. Sackett (1980) applied Bakeman’s (1978) concept of prospectivity, but considered retrospective lags feeding forward from negative lags, going from a lag of −5 to a lag of −4, from −4 to −3, and so on successively, in an approach open to criticism. Anguera (1997) proposed a promising new concept, called genuine retrospectivity – included in the analysis algorithm on programming the HOISAN software – that allows backward feeding, from lag 0 to −1, from lag −1 to −2, and so on (Gorospe and Anguera, 2000; Gorospe et al., 2005).

Sackett (1980) ingeniously used the Zsum statistic proposed by Cochran (1954), providing a powerful means of data reduction provided the data are independent. He applied it to the obtained adjusted residual values (which are independent of each other because they each respond to a different calculation given that the lags are different) considering the criterion behavior of the sequential analysis as the focal behavior and the conditional behaviors in positive lags to obtain the prospective Zsum values. He applied the same method (but using conditional behaviors in negative rather than positive lags) to obtain the retrospective Zsum values. Note that the number of positive and negative lags must be the same (Sackett, 1980). Experience to date (Sackett, 1979; Anguera and Losada, 1999) indicates that at least five prospective (e.g., lags +1 to +5) and five retrospective lags (e.g., −1 to −5) to be analyzed (Anguera et al., 2018).

From the prospective and retrospective Zsum values, Sackett (1980) proposed a vectorialization of the relationships between focal behavior and conditional behaviors. Each vector has length or radius Length=(Zsumprospective)2+(Zsumretrospective)2 and an angle ϕ = ArcsenZsumretrospectiveLength.

As many vectors as conditional behaviors are obtained, all graphically with their origins in the focal behavior. Because the prospective and retrospective Zsum values have a positive or negative sign, the corresponding vectors can be plotted such that the prospective and retrospective values will be displayed along the horizontal (X)-axis and the vertical (Y)-axis, respectively.

The meaning of the vectors varies in function of the quadrant in which they are located, and the position of a vector in one quadrant or another is determined by the combination of positive or negative signs on the prospective and retrospective Zsum values:

Quadrant I (+ +): the focal and conditional behaviors activate each other.

Quadrant II (− +): the focal behavior inhibits and is activated by the conditional behavior.

Quadrant III (−−): the focal and conditional behaviors inhibit each other.

Quadrant IV (+ −): The focal behavior activates and is inhibited by the conditional behavior. Vector length indicates the strength (statistical significance) of the association between the focal and conditional behaviors.

Like T-pattern detection, polar coordinate analysis has been used in a wide range of fields, including clinical psychology (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017; Rodríguez-Medina et al., 2018; Alcover et al., 2019; Del Giacco et al., 2020).

Results

The way in which the therapist and the adolescents communicated with each other changed from session 5 to 29 and the qualitative changes detected were confirmed quantitatively within a rigorous analytical framework. Sample clinical vignettes for each session are reproduced below.

Tables 4A,B shows the records corresponding to the vignettes in Tables 2, 3 for multi-event sequence data and according to the syntax of the GSEQ5 program. These data make up an .SDS file, compiled for the program to check for formal errors and generating an .MDS file once verified as correct.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Fragment of record with multi-event sequence data using the syntaxis of the GSEQ5 program.

The results of the three techniques (T-pattern detection, lag sequential analysis, and polar coordinate analysis) are presented below.

T-Pattern Detection

For both sessions, the records obtained were transformed using the GSEQ5 program to adapt them to the syntax of the THEME program, which requires two files: the VVT.VVT file corresponding to the observation instrument, and the .RDT file corresponding to the recorded data. Tables 5A–C shows the VVT.VVT file and the respective records corresponding to the vignettes in Tables 2, 3, maintaining a conventional and constant distance according to the THEME syntax.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Fragment of record using the syntaxis of the THEME program.

For the initial sesión (see Figure 1), we detected four T-patterns for the therapist as focal subject. These were related to two communication modalities: questioning (code QA) and repetition or paraphrasing (code RP).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. T-patterns in the initial session: minimum occurrence 30 and p < 0.005. Four T-patterns were identified linking turn-taking by the therapist to the questioning (QA, depicted as PA in the graph) and repetition/paraphrasing (RP) categories (both from the “facilitating conversation” dimension). The vertical lines correspond to each co-occurrence of t and pa behaviors (patterns 137 and 138), followed by co-occurrences of t and pa (pattern 137) and t and rp (pattern 138). The vertical lines also show co-occurrences of t and rp (patterns 139 and 140) followed t and pa (pattern 139), and t and rp (pattern 140). The length of the horizontal line fragments is proportional to their duration.

For the final sesión (see Figure 2), we detected two T-patterns, again related to two communication modalities: questioning (QA) and mentalization (MNT).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. T-patterns in the final session: minimum occurrence 30 and p < 0.005. Two T-patterns were identified. One was linked to the turn-taking of the therapist in the mentalization (MNT) category (from the “mentalization” dimension), and the other was linked to the questioning (QA) category (from the “facilitating conversation” dimension). Vertical lines reflect co-occurrences of t and mnt (pattern 151) and t and qa (pattern 152) behaviors, followed by co-occurrences of t and mnt (pattern 151), and t and qa (pattern 152) behaviors. The length of the horizontal line fragments is proportional to their duration.

Lag Sequential Analysis

For both sessions, sequential lag analysis was performed considering T as the criterion behavior and all other observation instrument codes as conditional behaviors. Table 6 shows the adjusted residual values obtained using the GSEQ5 program.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Lag sequential analysis for the initial and final sessions, 5 and 29, respectively.

Sequential lag analysis of the data from session 5 revealed a behavioral pattern in which paraphrasing and use of questioning alternated between lags −3 and +2. A sequential pattern with mentalization located in the center (lag 0) was detected for session 29.

Polar Coordinate Analysis

Obtained, considering T as the focal behavior and all other observation instrument codes as conditional behaviors, were parameters corresponding to the prospective and retrospective Zsum values, from which vector length and angle values were calculated along with the quadrant in which the values were located. All values were obtained using HOISAN.

Tables 7, 8 show the parameters corresponding to sessions 5 and 29, respectively.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Parameters corresponding to the prospective and retrospective Zsum values obtained in session 5, considering T (therapist) as the focal behavior, from which vector length, vector angle, and quadrant were calculated.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Parameters corresponding to the prospective and retrospective Zsum values obtained in session 29, considering T (therapist) as the focal behavior, from which vector length, vector angle, and quadrant were calculated.

Graphs of the vectors, created using R, are depicted in Figures 3, 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Polar coordinates for the initial session. The vectors correspond to interventions by the therapist (T) as focal behaviors and the communication strategies questioning (QA), paraphrasing (RP), and mentalization (MNT) as conditional behaviors.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Polar coordinates for the final session. The vectors correspond to interventions by the therapist (T) as focal behaviors and the communication modalities questioning (QA), paraphrasing (RP), and mentalization (MNT) as conditional behaviors.

The polar coordinate analysis showed that the therapist activated questioning and paraphrasing (quadrant I) in the initial session, and questioning and mentalization in the final session (quadrant I).

Discussion

The results obtained in the T-pattern, lag sequential, and polar coordinate, analyses all show changes in the conversation-facilitation techniques used by the lead therapist from the start to the end of therapy. Although the combination of these three techniques has been used in different fields (e.g., Santoyo et al., 2017; Tarragó et al., 2017), this is the first time they have been applied in combination to clinical psychology. Our findings show that this is a remarkably productive approach to identifying relationships between communication modalities and changes that occur during the therapeutic process.

From the observation instrument composed of 15 dimensions and 28 categories (Table 1), three communication modalities in particular were identified – questions (QA), paraphrasing (RP), and mentalization (MNT) – suggesting that these are all powerful communication strategies for encouraging patient interaction in group therapy (Oetzel and Scherer, 2003).

Questioning by the therapist was observed in both the earlier and later sessions and its use shows that the therapist expressed interest in what the participants had to say, strengthening the therapist-patient bond. Questioning stimulates dialogue and encourages more inhibited group members to take the floor in a conversation and to express their experiences and feelings.

Paraphrasing was a particularly common strategy used in the initial session. Repeating what someone has said is a common technique for facilitating communication; it shows active listening and interest on the part of the therapist and facilitates an atmosphere of empathy and acceptance. This result corroborates the importance attached to reciprocity by adolescents as reported by Lavik et al. (2018). By repeating what the adolescent has just said, the therapist gives them the chance to continue talking ad treats them as an equal. In a sense, it constitutes a verbal reflection or “mirroring” of the speaker’s expressiveness that serves to hold attention and stimulate. Our findings, however, show that the therapist did not use this technique frequently, as it accounted for just 10% of her interventions, compared with 25% for questioning. The remaining 65% of interventions comprised a highly variable presence of the other 14 categories. Questioning and paraphrasing, both common strategies in psychotherapy, form part of the “facilitating conversation” dimension of the study observation instrument (Table 1).

Mentalization appeared in the final session, reflecting the communicative maturity of the group. In a previous study by our group, we found that paraphrasing was used to activate conversation from the early stages of therapy and also that it encouraged the emergence of mentalization (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017). The results of the present study support this regulatory effect of paraphrasing as a prior requirement for the mentalization process (Fonagy et al., 2002); demonstrated is its relationship with the reflection or mirroring effect, while affect regulation and mentalization are linked to the development of self (Fonagy et al., 2018). In terms of the distinction between empathy with patients and challenging of their judgments by therapists, as described by Voutilainen et al. (2018), we saw that interventions designed to stimulate mentalization posed a true challenge to the adolescents in our group, who were found to “defend themselves” from this process, resorting to jokes and noises, touching, playful or more forceful hitting, laughter, and changes of subject. The therapist attempted to contain these emotions by non-judgmental interventions and by encouraging the adolescents to express themselves. The results suggest that a certain level of empathy and acceptance is necessary in psychotherapy to create an environment in which the therapist’s challenges are heeded and contribute to personal growth (Karver et al., 2006; Binder et al., 2011; Sagen et al., 2013).

From the clinical perspective, our results provide objective evidence, supported by three different analytical methods, of the important use that a therapist makes of three of the 28 communication strategies in the observation instrument, namely, questioning, repetition/paraphrasing, and interventions to improve mentalization. Unlike our previous study, which focused on differences in turn-taking in group sessions, this study focuses on differences in early and late communication strategies of the therapist. Our findings show how use of the different communication strategies varies from early to late therapy stages. In terms of novel findings, all three analytical methods detected a statistically significant increase in the use of questioning and repetition/paraphrasing as “conversation facilitators” in the early stages of therapy. In previous study (Arias-Pujol and Anguera, 2017), these strategies were grouped into a single block — conversation-facilitating DYN categories — formed by seven codes (FF, FO, RP, RT, QA, QC, and QV). A second novel finding, detected again by all three methods, was that the therapist made significantly greater use of questioning and mentalization in the later session compared to the earlier session to achieve the goals of the intervention.

This mixed methods study employed systematic observation and a succession of QUAL-QUAN-QUAL stages. We have shown that the combined use of T-pattern detection, polar coordinate analysis, and lag sequential analysis can offer meaningful and objective insights into what occurs in group psychotherapy from the angle of the therapist.

This study has three novel methodological aspects. First, it is the first to apply T-pattern detection to group psychotherapy with adolescents; second, it is first to combine T-pattern detection, polar coordinate analysis, and lag sequential analysis to analyze what occurs during the course of group psychotherapy from the perspective of the therapist; and finally, within the framework of a mixed methods study, our research incorporates one of the most powerful methods for linking qualitative and quantitative data, namely, the connecting method, which involves the systematized transformation of qualitative data into robust quantitative data for objective analysis.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Head of the Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation (FETB) Research Department. Approval by an ethics committee was not required as per applicable institutional and national guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

EA-P developed the project. MA conducted the method section and T-patterns, polar coordinate, and sequential analysis. Both authors participated in writing the article.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding

This study was supported by the Catalan Government under Grant number 2017 SGR 876 for the project Grup de Recerca de Parella i Família (GRPF). We also gratefully acknowledge the support of the Spanish Government subproject Integration ways between qualitative and quantitative data, multiple case development, and synthesis review as main axis for an innovative future in physical activity and sports research (PGC2018-098742-B-C31) (2019–2021) (Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades/Agencia Estatal de Investigación/European Regional Development Fund), part of the coordinated project New approach of research in physical activity and sport from mixed methods perspective (NARPAS_MM) (SPGC201800X098742CV0).

Acknowledgments

We thank all those at the Center for Child and Adolescent Mental Health of Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation in Barcelona, Spain, who so willingly helped to make this study possible, as well as all the adolescents and families who participated. Finally, we also acknowledge support from Ramon Llull University (PGRiD of FPCEE Blanquerna).

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.mangold-international.com/en/products/software/gseq
  2. ^ www.patternvision.com
  3. ^ www.menpas.com

References

Alcover, C., Mairena, M. A., Mezzatesta, M., Elías, N., Díez, M., Balañá, G., et al. (2019). Mixed methods approach to describe social interaction during a group intervention for adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Front. Psychol. 10:1158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, P. D., and Liker, J. K. (1982). Analyzing sequential categorical data on dyadic interaction: a comment on gottman. Psychol. Bull. 91, 393–403.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (in press). “Desarrollando la observación indirecta: alcance, proceso y habilidades metodológicas en el análisis de textos” [Development of indirect observation: Scope, process and methodological abilities in textual analysis], in Patrones de Habilidades Metodológicas y Conceptuales de Análisis, Evaluación e Intervención en Ciencias del Comportamiento, ed. Santoyo (Mexico: UNAM/PAPIIT).

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1997). “From prospective patterns in behavior to joint analysis with a retrospective perspective,” in Proceedings of the Colloque sur Invitation “Méthodologie d’Analyse des Interactions Sociales, (Paris: Université de la Sorbonne).

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2005). “Microanalysis of T-patterns. Analysis of symmetry/asymmetry in social interaction,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 51–70.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2020). “Is it possible to perform “liquefying” actions in conversational analysis? The detection of structures in indirect observation,” in The Temporal Structure of Multimodal Communication. Intelligent Systems Reference Library, Vol. 164, eds L. Hunyadi and I. Szekrényes (Cham: Springer), 45–67. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-22895-8_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Losada, J. L. (1999). “Reducción de datos en marcos de conducta mediante la técnica de coordenadas polares” [Data reduction in behavioral frameworks through polar coordinate analysis],” in Observación de la Conducta Interactiva en Situaciones Naturales: Aplicaciones, ed. M. T. Anguera (Barcelona: E.U.B), 163–188.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., and Losada, J. L. (2001). Diseños observacionales, cuestión clave en el proceso de la metodología observacional [Observational designs, a key question in the observational methodology process]. Metodol. Cien. Comp. 3, 135–161.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Camerino, O., Castañer, M., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2017a). The specificity of observational studies in physical activity and sports sciences: moving forward in mixed methods research and proposals for achieving quantitative and qualitative symmetry. Front. Psychol. 8:2196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Jonsson, G. K., and Sánchez-Algarra, P. (2017b). Liquefying text from human communication processes: a methodological proposal based on T-pattern detection. J. Multi. Commun. Stud. 4, 10–15.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Portell, M., Chacón-Moscoso, S., and Sanduvete-Chaves, S. (2018). Indirect observation in everyday contexts: concepts and methodological guidelines within a mixed methods framework. Front. Psychol. 9:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Portell, P., Hernández-Mendo, A., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Jonsson, G. K. (in press). “Diachronic analysis of qualitative data,” in Reviewer’s Guide for Mixed Methods Research Analysis, eds A. J. Onwuegbuzie and R. B. Johnson (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., and Anguera, M. T. (in press). “Investigación en psicoterapia psicoanalítica: metodologia observacional como mixed methods [Research in psychoanalytic psychotherapy: observational methodology as mixed methods],” in Investigación en Psicoteràpia Psicoanalítica, eds J. Castillo and J. Mercadal (Barcelona: Herder).

Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2004). Detección de patrones de conducta comunicativa en un grupo terapéutico de adolescentes [Detection of communicative behavior patterns in an adolescent therapy group]. Acción Psicol. 3, 199–206.

Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2005). Análisis de la comunicación en un grupo terapéutico de adolescentes: estudio diacrónico [Analysis of communication in a therapeutic group of adolescents: a diachronic study]. Rev. Psicopatol. Salud Ment. Niño Adolesc. 1, 25–36.

Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2017). Observation of interactions in adolescent group therapy: a mixed methods study. Front. Psychol. 8:1188. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., Fieschi, E., Miralbell, J., Castellò, C., Soldevila, A., Anguera, M. T., et al. (2015). Efectos de la imitación en la interacción social recíproca en un niño con Trastorno del Espectro Autista Grave [Effects of imitation on mutual social interaction in children with severe autism spectrum disorder]. Rev. Psicopatol. Salud Ment. Niño Adolesc. 25, 9–20.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R. (1978). “Untangling streams of behavior: sequential analysis of observational data,” in Observing Behavior, Vol. 2 Data Collection and Analysis Methods, ed. G. P. Sackett (Baltimore, MD: University Park Press), 63–78.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R. (1991). From lags to logs: advances in sequential analysis. Rev. Mex. Anal. Conduct. 17, 65–83.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Gottman, J. M. (1987). “Applying observational methods: a systematic view,” in Handbook of Infant Development, 2nd Edn, ed. J. D. Osofsky (New York, NY: Wiley), 818–854.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (1996). Análisis de la Interacción. Análisis Secuencial con SDIS y GSEQ [Analysis of Interaction. Sequential Analysis with SDIS and GSEQ]. Madrid: Ra-Ma.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2011). Sequential Analysis and Observational Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bartholomew, T. T., and Lockard, A. J. (2018). Mixed methods in psychotherapy research: a review of method(ology) integration in psychotherapy science. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 1687–1709. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, P. E., Moltu, C., Hummelsund, D., Sagen, S. H., and Holgersen, H. (2011). Meeting an adult ally on the way out into the world: adolescent patients’ experiences of useful psychotherapeutic ways of working at an age when independence really matters. Psychother. Res. 21, 554–566. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2011.587471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchet, A., Batt, M., Trognon, A., and Masse, L. (2005). “Language and behaviour patterns in a therapeutic interaction sequence,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 124–140.

Google Scholar

Chacón-Moscoso, A., Anguera, M. T., Sanduvete-Chaves, S., Losada, J. L., Lozano-Lozano, J. A., and Portell, M. (2019). Methodological quality checklist for studies based on observational methodology (MQCOM). Psicothema 31, 458–464. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochran, W. G. (1954). Some methods for strengthening the common χ2 tests. Biometrics 10, 417–451. doi: 10.2307/3001616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement of partial credit. Psychol. Bull. 70, 213–220. doi: 10.1037/h0026256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creswell, J. W., and Plano Clark, V. L. (2017). Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research (1st ed., 2007). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Del Giacco, L., Anguera, M. T., and Salcuni, S. (2020). The action of verbal and non-verbal communication in the therapeutic alliance construction: a mixed methods approach to assess the initial interactions with depressed patients. Front. Psychol. 10:234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giacco, L., Salcuni, S., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). The communicative modes analysis system in psychotherapy from mixed methods framework: introducing a new observation system for classifying verbal and non-verbal communication. Front. Psychol. 10:782. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Down, R., Willner, P., Watts, L., and Griffiths, J. (2011). Anger management groups for adolescents: a mixed-methods study of efficacy and treatment preferences. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 16, 33–52. doi: 10.1177/1359104509341448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., Herrero-Nivela, M. L., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Preschool metacognitive skill assessment in order to promote educational sensitive response from mixed-methods approach: complementarity of data analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P. (1991). Thinking about thinking: some clinical and theoretical considerations in the treatment of a borderline patient. Int. J. Psychoanal. 72, 639–656.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Cottrell, D., Phillips, J., Bevington, D., Glaser, D., and Allison, E. (2015). What Works for Whom? A Critical Review of Treatments for Children and Adolescents. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., and Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., and Target, M. (2018). Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Gorospe, G., and Anguera, M. T. (2000). Modificación de la técnica clásica de coordenadas polares mediante un desarrollo distinto de la retrospectividad: aplicación al tenis [Retrospectivity in polar coordinates analysis: application to tennis]. Psicothema 12, 279–282.

Google Scholar

Gorospe, G., Hernández Mendo, A., Anguera, M. T., and Martínez de Santos, R. (2005). Desarrollo y optimización de una herramienta observacional en el tenis de individuales [Development and optimization ofan observational tool for singles tennis]. Psicothema 17, 123–127.

Google Scholar

Halfon, S., Cavdar, A., Orsucci, F., Schiepek, G. K., Andreassi, S., Giuliani, A., et al. (2016). The non-linear trajectory of change in play profiles of three children on psychodynamic play therapy. Front. Psychol. 7:1494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halfon, S., Cavdar, A., Paoloni, G., Andreassi, S., Giulani, A., Orsucci, F. F., et al. (2019). Monitoring nonlinear dynamics of change in a single case of psychodynamic play therapy. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol. Life Sci. 23, 113–115.

Google Scholar

Haynal-Reymond, V., Jonsson, G. K., and Magnusson, M. S. (2005). “Nonverbal communication in doctor-suicidal patient interview,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 142–149.

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J. A., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana, J. L. (2012). Hoisan 1.2: programa informático para uso en metodología observacional [Hoisan 1.2: software for observational methodology]. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/s1578-84232012000100006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, C. C., and Magnusson, M. S. (2016). “Using hidden behavioral patterns to study nausea in a preclinical model,” in Discovering Hidden Temporal Patterns in Behavior and Interaction. T-Pattern Detection and Analysis with THEME, eds M. S. Magnusson, J. K. Burgoon, and M. Casarrubea (New York, NY: Springer), 237–254.

Google Scholar

Johnson, R. B., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., and Turner, L. A. (2007). Toward a definition of mixed methods research. J. Mixed Methods Res. 1, 112–133. doi: 10.1177/1558689806298224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karver, M. S., Handelsman, J. B., Fields, S., and Bickman, L. (2006). Meta-analysis of therapeutic relationship variables in youth and family therapy: the evidence for diferent relationship variables in the child and adolescent treatment outcome literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 50–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinbub, J. R. (2017). State of the art of interpersonal physiology in psychotherapy: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 8:2053. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krippendorff, K. (2013). Content Analysis. An Introduction to Its Methodology, 3rd Edn. California, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavik, K. O., Frøysa, H., Brattebø, K. F., McLeod, J., and Moltu, C. (2017). The first sessions of psychotherapy: a qualitative meta−analysis of alliance formation processes. J. Psychother. 28, 348–366. doi: 10.1037/int0000101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavik, K. O., Veseth, M., Frøysa, H., Binder, P. E., and Moltu, C. (2018). ‘Nobody else can lead your life’: what adolescents need from psychotherapists in change processes. Counsell. Psychother. Res. 18, 262–273. doi: 10.1002/capr.12166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (1996). Hidden real-time patterns in intra- and inter-individual behavior. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 12, 112–123. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.12.2.112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 32, 93–110. doi: 10.3758/BF03200792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2005). “Understanding social interaction: discovering hidden structure with model and algorithms,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 4–22.

Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2016). “Time and self-similar structure in behavior and interactions: From sequences to symmetry and fractals,” in Discovering Hidden Temporal Patterns in Behavior and Interactions: T-Pattern Detection and Analysis with THEME, eds M. S. Magnusson, J. K. Burgoon, M. Casarrubea, and D. McNeill (New York, NY: Springer), 3–35. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-3249-8_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2018). “Temporal patterns in interactions,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Group Interaction Analysis, eds E. Brauner, M. Boos, and M. Kolbe (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 323–353. doi: 10.1017/9781316286302.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2020). T-pattern detection and analysis (TPA) with THEMETM: a mixed methods approach. Front. Psychol. 10:2663. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzano, J., Palacio Espasa, F., and Abella, A. (2018). Compendio de Técnica Psicoanalítica: con su Aplicación a la Psicoterapia [Summary of Psychoanalytic Technique With its Application to Psychotherapy]. Barcelona: Herder.

Google Scholar

Martínez del Pozo, M. (1993). Análisis del “Proceso de Elaboración del duelo” en el Hipertenso Esencial: Estudio Empírico de sus Fases Mediante Observación Sistemática [Analysis of the “Grief Process” in Individuals with Essential Hypertension: An Empirical Study of Grief Phases through Systematic Observation]. Ph.D. thesis, University of Barcelona, Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Merten, J., and Schwab, F. (2005). “Facial expression patterns in common and psychotherapeutic situations,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 150–159.

Google Scholar

Midgley, N., Reynolds, S., Kelvin, R., Loades, M., Calderon, A., Martin, P., et al. (2018). Therapists’ techniques in the treatment of adolescent depression. J. Psychother. Integr. 28, 413–428. doi: 10.1037/int0000119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Keefe, S., Martin, P., Goodyer, I. M., Wilkinson, P., Consortium, I., and Midgley, N. (2018). Predicting dropout in adolescents receiving therapy for depression. Psychother. Res. 28, 708–721. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2017.1393576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oetzel, K., and Scherer, D. G. (2003). Therapeutic engagement with adolescents in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy 40:215. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.40.3.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onwuegbuzie, A. J., Hitchcock, J., Natesan, P., and Newman, I. (2018). Using fully integrated thinking to address the 1+1=1 integration challenge. Int. J. Multi. Res. Approaches 10, 666–678. doi: 10.29034/ijmra.v10n1a43

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plano Clark, V. L., and Sanders, K. (2015). “The use of visual displays in mixed methods research,” in Use of Visual Displays in Research and Testing, eds M. T. McCrudden, G. Schraw, and C. W. Buckendahl (Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, Inc), 177–206.

Google Scholar

Plumet, M.-H., and Tardif, C. (2005). “Understanding the functioning of social interaction with autistic children,” in The Hidden Structure of Social Interaction. From Genomics to Culture Patterns, eds L. Anolli, S. Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, and G. Riva (Amsterdam: IOS Press), 182–193.

Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Chacón-Moscoso, S., and Sanduvete-Chaves, S. (2015). Guidelines for reporting evaluations based on observational methodology (GREOM). Psicothema 27, 283–289.

Google Scholar

Portell, M., Sene-Mir, A. M., Anguera, M. T., Jonsson, G. K., and Losada, J. L. (2019). Support system for the assessment and intervention during the manual material handling training at the workplace: contributions from the systematic observation. Front. Psychol. 10:1247. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quera, V. (2018). “Analysis of interaction sequences,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Group Interaction Analysis, eds E. Brauner, M. Boos, and M. Kolbe (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 295–322. doi: 10.1017/9781316286302.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, L. D., and Allen, P. J. (2019). A call for the inclusion of mixed methods research in the undergraduate psychology curriculum. Front. Psychol. 9:2709. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Medina, J., Arias, V., Arias, B., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Polar Coordinate Analysis, from HOISAN to R: A Tutorial Paper. Available online at: https://jairodmed.shinyapps.io/HOISAN_to_R/ (accessed June 30, 2019).

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Medina, J., Rodríguez-Navarro, H., Arias, V., Arias, B., and Anguera, M. T. (2018). Non-reciprocal friendships in a school-age boy with autism: the ties that build? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 2980–2994. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3575-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roustan, M., Izquierdo, C., and Anguera, M. T. (2013). Sequential analysis of an interactive peer support group. Psicothema 25, 396–401. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2012.93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (1979). “The lag sequential analysis of contingency and cyclicity on behavioral interaction research,” in Handbook of Infant Development, ed. J. D. Osofsky (New York, NY: Wiley), 623–649.

Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (1980). “Lag sequential analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research,” in Exceptional Infant. Psychosocial Risks in Infant-Environment Transactions, eds D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticuff (New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel), 300–340.

Google Scholar

Sagen, S. H., Hummelsund, D., and Binder, P. E. (2013). Feeling accepted: A phenomenological exploration of adolescent patients’ experiences of the relational qualities that enable them to express themselves freely. Eur. J. Psychother. Counsel. 15, 53–75. doi: 10.1080/13642537.2013.763467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Anguera, M. T. (2013). Qualitative/quantitative integration in the inductive observational study of interactive behaviour: impact of recording and coding among predominating perspectives. Qual. Quant. 47, 1237–1257. doi: 10.1007/s11135-012-9764-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santoyo, C., Jonsson, G. K., Anguera, M. T., and López-López, J. A. (2017). Observational analysis of the organization of on-task behavior in the classroom using complementary data analyses. Anal. Psicol. 33, 497–514. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.271061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santoyo, C., Jonsson, G. K., Anguera, M. T., Portell, M., Allegro, A., Colmenares, L., et al. (2020). T-patterns integration strategy in a longitudinal study: a multiple case analysis. Physiol. Behav. 222:112904. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffen, P. R., Fidalgo, L., Schmuck, D., Tsui, Y., and Brown, T. (2014). Psychotherapy participants show increased physiological responsiveness to a lab stressor relative to matched controls. Front. Psychol. 5:795. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suárez, N., Sánchez, C. R., Jiménez, J. E., and Anguera, M. T. (2018). Is reading instruction evidence-based? Analyzing teaching practices using T-Patterns. Front. Psychol. 9:7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swank, J. M., and Shin, S. M. (2015). Garden counseling groups and self-esteem: a mixed methods study with children with emotional and behavioral problems. J. Special. Group Work 40, 315–331. doi: 10.1080/01933922.2015.1056570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanzilli, A., Majorana, M., Fonzi, L., Pallagrosi, M., Picardi, A., Coccanari de’ Fornari, M. A., et al. (2018). Relational variables in short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy: an effectiveness study. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome 21, 190–200.

Google Scholar

Target, M. (2018). 20/20 Hindsight: A 25-year programme at the Anna Freud Centre of efficacy and effectiveness research on child psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Psychother. Res. 28, 30–46. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2017.1349351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarragó, R., Iglesias, X., Lapresa, D., Anguera, M. T., Ruiz-Sanchís, L., and Arana, J. (2017). Analysis of diachronic relationships in successful and unsuccessful behaviors by world fencing champions using three complementary techniques. Anal. Psicol. 33, 471–485. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.271041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torras de Beà, E. (2013). Psicoterapia de Grupo [Group Psychotherapy]. Barcelona: Octaedro.

Google Scholar

Vaimberg, R. (2010). Psicoterapias Tecnológicamente Mediadas [Technologically Mediated Psychotherapies]. Doctoral thesis, Universidad de Barcelona, Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Venturella, M., Carbonell, X., Cabré, V., and Arias-Pujol, E. (2019). A psychoanalytical perspective on the co-therapeutic relationship with a group of siblings of children with autism: an observational study of communicative behavior patterns. Front. Psychol. 10:1832. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voutilainen, L., Henttonen, P., Kahri, M., Ravaja, N., Sams, M., and Peräkylä, A. (2018). Empathy, challenge, and psychophysiological activation in therapist–client interaction. Front. Psychol. 9:530. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, D. L., Yefimova, M., Kim, H., and Phillips, L. R. (2016). “Detecting and characterizing patterns of behavioral symptoms of dementia,” in Discovering Hidden Temporal Patterns in Behavior and Interaction. T-Pattern Detection and Analysis with THEME, eds M. S. Magnusson, J. K. Burgoon, and M. Casarrubea (New York, NY: Springer), 125–142. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-3249-8_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mixed methods, QUAL-QUAN-QUAL integration, group therapy, adolescents, psychotherapist interactions

Citation: Arias-Pujol E and Anguera MT (2020) A Mixed Methods Framework for Psychoanalytic Group Therapy: From Qualitative Records to a Quantitative Approach Using T-Pattern, Lag Sequential, and Polar Coordinate Analyses. Front. Psychol. 11:1922. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01922

Received: 03 November 2019; Accepted: 13 July 2020;
Published: 11 August 2020.

Edited by:

Reitske Meganck, Ghent University, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Giulio de Felice, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Antonio Hernández-Mendo, University of Malaga, Spain
Rafael E. Reigal, University of Malaga, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Arias-Pujol and Anguera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eulàlia Arias-Pujol, eulaliaap@blanquerna.url.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.