Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 May 2020
Sec. Eating Behavior

Body Image and Sociocultural Predictors of Body Image Dissatisfaction in Croatian and Chinese Women

  • 1Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China

In the current paper, two different studies were designed to investigate body image dissatisfaction and perception of the attractive female body in Chinese and Croatian women and men using the correlational and experimental study research approach. Study 1 comprised 266 Chinese (160 women; 106 men) and 261 Croatian participants (161 women; 100 men). Women from both countries were asked to complete the measures of the Contour Rating Scale, SATAQ – 4, Body Area Scale and demographic data, while men were asked to complete the measures of the Contour Rating Scale and demographic data via online surveys. The obtained results indicated that thin internalization predicted body image dissatisfaction in both samples. Besides, while both samples scored relatively high on thin-ideal internalization, Chinese women, who were also in general less satisfied with their body image, had higher scores. Chinese women also scored higher on musculature internalization and felt more pressure from family, friends, and media to conform to standard beauty norms compared to Croatian women. The samples also differed in their perceptions of preferable body size, where Chinese women had a more negative perception of their actual body size. The obtained results furthermore revealed that Chinese men preferred thinner women compared to Croatian men. Moreover, both samples misjudged what their female counterparts found attractive, thinking that women wanted to have even thinner bodies than they actually reported. Similarly, women from both cultures revealed a preference for thinner figures than the ones selected as the most attractive by the opposite sex. Finally, in Study 2, experimental manipulation of thin-ideal was used to examine whether thin priming affected body image dissatisfaction. The obtained results revealed that when primed with thin-ideal women from both countries felt lower satisfaction with their body size. The observed effect was stronger for Chinese participants. Limitations of the current study are discussed in the conclusion.

Introduction

Previous studies have shown that body dissatisfaction and excessive investment in the body are precursors to the full range of unhealthy eating behaviors, including binge eating, restrictive dieting, and self-induced vomiting (Thompson and Stice, 2001; Wertheim et al., 2001; Levine and Piran, 2004). Extreme preoccupation with weight and body shape have been identified as vital risk factors for the development of eating disorders and have been included as diagnostic criteria for both anorexia and bulimia (Lydecker et al., 2017). Furthermore, body image dissatisfaction has been associated with depression, stress, low self-esteem (Johnson and Wardle, 2005), increased negative mood (Tiggemann and McGill, 2004), reduced appraisal of one’s own physical appeal (Bessenoff, 2006), global shame (Hayaki et al., 2002), increased social anxiety (Cash et al., 2004), emotional distress, appearance rumination, and a tendency toward unnecessary appearance management (Paxton et al., 2006; Etu and Gray, 2010).

Tripartite Influence Model

One of the most commonly used theories to explain the development of body image dissatisfaction is the Tripartite Influence Model (Thompson et al., 1999). This model proposes that three influences (peers, parents, and media) affect body image and eating problems through two mediational mechanisms: internalization of societal standards of appearance and excessive appearance comparison. According to this theory, social influences, such as peers, family, and media pressure, individuals to adhere to culturally defined standards of beauty (Stice and Agras, 1998; Grabe et al., 2008). After persistent exposure to these messages, the ideal becomes part of individuals’ personal beliefs of what constitutes attractiveness and beauty, and individuals start to internalize the appearance ideals, which they eventually interpret as originating from within the self. Those that internalize the dominant message within their socio-cultural environment to a greater extent, but at the same time don’t feel as they meet the standards are at higher risk of body image dissatisfaction (Thompson and Stice, 2001; Cafri et al., 2005). According to previous studies, thin-ideal internalization is an essential concept for a complete comprehension of body image dissatisfaction (Thompson and Stice, 2001; Stice, 2002). Previous studies have shown that the thin-ideal internalization is directly related to body dissatisfaction (Stice and Agras, 1998) and eating disorder symptomatology (Ballentine and Ogle, 2005; Suisman et al., 2012). Furthermore, thin internalization was also found to mediate the relationship between sociocultural influences and body dissatisfaction (Tiggemann, 2003; Keery et al., 2004). Musculature internalization is equivalent of thin internalization for male body image dissatisfaction (Schaefer et al., 2015). Although male body image has received considerably less research attention, existing studies have revealed that internalization of the muscular ideal is significantly associated with increased body dissatisfaction, negative affect, and unhealthy behaviors aimed at increasing muscle size (Karazsia and Crowther, 2008; Thompson et al., 2012). Nonetheless, over recent years a new body image trend of muscular and fit female bodies has become increasingly popular in Western societies (Bozsik et al., 2018; Rodgers et al., 2018), thus suggesting that musculature internalization might also be applicable in the female population. A substantial amount of previous research has provided support for Tripartite Influence Model, especially among young girls and women (e.g., Carlson Jones, 2004; Keery et al., 2004; Cafri et al., 2005; Bedford and Johnson, 2006; Shroff and Thompson, 2006). Besides body image disturbances and disordered eating, this model was also a good fit in explaining various body management practices among women like tanning and exercising, as well as attitudes toward cosmetic surgery (Sharp et al., 2014). The Tripartite Influence Model was also supported among boys and men (Karazsia and Crowther, 2008; Grammas and Schwartz, 2009; Jackson and Chen, 2010; Tylka, 2011; Hazzard et al., 2019); as well as among populations of homosexual and bisexual orientations (Tylka and Andorka, 2012; Huxley et al., 2015), and among various non-Western countries (Yamamiya et al., 2008; Mellor et al., 2009; Jackson and Chen, 2010; Papp et al., 2013; Shagar et al., 2019).

Not that long ago, it was generally believed that the body image dissatisfaction was predominantly Western World phenomenon (Nasser et al., 2003; Grabe and Hyde, 2006); nevertheless, the turn of the 21st century marked onset of the overwhelming spread of body image dissatisfaction in numerous countries outside the Western World (Rathner, 2001; Nasser et al., 2003). Tripartite influence model is especially relevant to non-White populations, since these women may feel under pressure to achieve standards of beauty that may significantly deviate from their ethnic features. Previous studies have found that when non-white women judge themselves through Western beauty standards, they are at higher risk of developing body image dissatisfaction and eating disorders (Hall, 1995; James et al., 2001; Oney et al., 2011). For example, a study conducted in Fiji revealed that body image dissatisfaction among young girls substantially increased following the introduction of western television to the island (Becker, 2004). Similar trends were observed in other non-Western countries such as Japan (Pike and Borovoy, 2004), Malaysia (Mellor et al., 2009), Korea (Lee, 2000), India (Shroff and Thompson, 2004), and many more (Swami et al., 2010). On the other hand, some studies have also reported higher resistance to thin-ideal internalization in women of African American heritage (Wildes et al., 2001). This same effect was also reported for Latina women, whose ethnic identity protected them to a certain extent from negative body image and negative comparisons with westernized beauty ideals (Schooler, 2008). Conversely, Asian cultures have been reported to be very susceptible to body image dissatisfaction and disturbances (Jung and Lee, 2006). Women in these countries have endorsed new canons of beauty that significantly differ from their traditional features and that include extremely thin bodies (Han, 2003; Rongmuang et al., 2011). Some Asian countries have developed a preference for large eyes and high noses, which notably deviate from their ethnic features, thus perpetuating the unprecedented spread of the cosmetic industry in these countries (Jung and Lee, 2006).

There is an interesting correspondence between increases in body image dissatisfactions in Asia and increases in body image dissatisfactions in the former socialist countries in Europe (Jung and Forbes, 2007). As it used to be the case with Asian countries, and the rest of the non–Western world, it is believed that body image dissatisfaction had a very low occurrence among the former socialistic countries in Europe (Catina and Joja, 2001; Rathner, 2001). Some researchers argue that socialist systems promoted gender equality, rejection of gender stereotypes, and preconceived notions of femininity and normative physical appearance (Catina and Joja, 2001). The demise of the socialist systems drastically redefined the women’s role within these societies possibly leading toward the alteration of beauty ideals and the rise of body image dissatisfaction. In fact, the rise of eating disorders in Western Europe and the United States, which occurred in the second half of the 20th century, coincided with a number of sweeping changes such as the rise of the consumer economy and associated impact on the status of women (Gordon, 2001). It has been well-documented that the prevalence of eating disorders was greater in industrialized or “post-industrialized” countries such as the United States, Western Europe, Japan, and similar compared to pre-industrialized societies such as those in sub-Sharan Africa (Levine and Smolak, 2010). Feminist theories argue that cultures with strong patriarchal tradition, rigid gender roles, and very fast social transformation, including fast-growing opportunities for women, are especially likely to internalize unrealistic appearance standards and to observe relatively high levels of body image dissatisfaction (Paludi, 2010).

Social Norms and Cultural Values

To fully apprehend the body image in two different cultures, it is necessary to understand the social and cultural norms, i.e., rules or expectations of behavior and thoughts based on shared beliefs on gender-roles within these specific cultural groups (Strahan et al., 2006). Attitudes toward gender-roles imply the general perception of gender roles such as gender-related tasks and power distribution. Since people are socialized into their community’s gender ideologies and rules about how men and women are supposed to think and behave, they accept these norms at face value. These values and norms can directly affect the way power are distributed in society, which in most of the patriarchal societies, is typical to the disadvantage of women (Sen and Östlin, 2008; Marcus and Harper, 2014). Consequently, the male view of femininity and beauty becomes not only a preference but a standard to strive toward. For example, traditional gender roles associate femininity with beauty and concern with appearance (Lennon et al., 1999). Indeed, previous studies have found that among young women, the notion of femininity is positively related to the importance of appearance. This finding suggests that an attractive appearance is likely to be more important for women endorsing the traditional attitudes toward gender roles than to women who hold non-traditional attitudes (Green et al., 2008). Thus, a woman’s traditional attitudes about male-femalee relations may implicitly require that she possesses and displays the physical and behavioral characteristics she believes men find attractive. Inglehart et al. (2002) reported that as countries undergo industrialization and modernization processes, their views on gender equality become more liberal compared to that of residents in less developed countries (Inglehart and Baker, 2000). Existing research has also identified a relationship between gender equality and cultural value dimensions of individualism/collectivism, power distance, and masculinity/femininity, which strongly determine the levels of gender equality in a specific society. E.g., individualistic cultures tend to have higher levels of gender equality compared to collectivistic cultures because the sanctity of the person in such individualistic cultures prevails over the ascribed status or social roles (Cohen and Kitayama, 2007). Previous studies have shown that gender roles were more pronounced in countries that scored higher on the cultural dimension of power distance, which describes the extent to which people in the society accept the uneven distribution of power (Basabe and Ros, 2005). Societies that score high on this dimension presume men to be competitive and to strive for material success and women to serve and care for non-material aspects of life and for children. In more masculine societies, gender inequality dominates, the social roles of sexes are different, and the mother has a weaker position in the family. Contrary, more feminine societies offer both sexes, especially women, more opportunities for the fulfillment of multiple social roles that are associated with more well-being and relationship satisfaction (Costa et al., 2001).

Body Image Dissatisfaction in Transition Economies

Both countries of our interest, China and Croatia, had a long history of communist and socialist social organizations, respectively, and have been witnessing fast socio-economic transition over the past couple of decades. Furthermore, both countries have strong patriarchal traditions rooted in Confucianism (Leung, 2003) and Catholic Church (Ramet and Matic, 2007), respectively; as well as heritage in a collectivist tradition that is gradually shifting toward more individualistic values (Podrug et al., 2006; Steele and Lynch, 2013; Hamamura and Xu, 2015; Su et al., 2016). Existing research proves that while religiosity does appear to promote patriarchal ideas (Hunnicutt, 2009; Ogle and Batton, 2009), it can also promote healthy body image and discourage dieting behaviors by supplying a source of worth other than the body (Brumberg, 2000; Kim, 2006), unlike Confucianism that has traditionally linked the worth of a woman with her corporality, i.e., the ability to produce domestic labor and to carry and give birth to sons thus perpetuating the male line. It has been suggested that even after the modernization, women in traditional Confucius cultures are encouraged to value themselves through their corporeal bodies, which are no longer respected for the ability to bear children but for the physical beauty that can positively affect one’s career or marital prospects (Kim, 2003). This has also been supported by recent research on the consumption of cosmetic surgery in China, which revealed that motivations to undertake plastic surgery operations came primarily from Chinese cultural hierarchies, i.e., family and society, as well as from the need to compete in a modernizing society (Hua, 2009). Confucianism, among others, endorses the idea of filial piety, which requires children to respect, support, and regard elderly in the family, as well as teachers, professional superiors, or anyone who is older in age. In such a culture, the family is the building block of society, while this hierarchical system of respect also applies to one’s country values as well (Chan, 2003). Previous studies have shown that perceived norms of more proximal or relevant reference groups may have a stronger impact on individual behaviors for members of such groups, including one’s gender (Lewis and Neighbors, 2004). Consequently, women’s identity is constructed in terms of social roles or identities where the physical appearance gets interpreted through group expectations and as something that can potentially bring face or dishonor on the family (Lindridge and Wang, 2008). As far as Croatia is concerned, according to existing research that has compared Croatian women with women from two other former Yugoslav countries, Slovenia and Serbia, on body image and beautifying practices, Croatian women have shown to be more likely to engage in physical beauty practices compared to their counterparts, and they have shown to be the most inclined toward cosmetic surgery. They were also the ones who have endorsed the post-traditional values to the highest level (Čevnik, 2016).

Culture has an impact on numerous factors that contribute to body image, including the media, peers, and family socialization (Grabe et al., 2008; Bell and Dittmar, 2011; Ferguson et al., 2011; Jones, 2011; Levine and Chapman, 2011; McCabe and McGreevy, 2011; Abraczinskas et al., 2012). Compared to the body of knowledge on body image dissatisfaction in Western World countries, there is still relatively little knowledge about these same concerns in non-Western countries. In words of Cash and Pruzinsky (2002) there is obvious paucity in “comparative cross-cultural studies of body image that are crucial to enhance our understanding of the diversity of body images and the influence of culture on body image development, dysfunction, and change” (p. 513). A large number of existing studies on body image dissatisfaction have focused on comparing non-Western populations with Western ones (e.g., Bush et al., 2001; Jung and Lee, 2006; Jung et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2013); yet, there are far fewer studies on body image dissatisfaction among two or more non-Western samples. The latter approach might be useful to move beyond established socio-cultural influences underpinning the body image dissatisfaction toward more specific and culturally driven interpretations of the same. The purpose of the present research was to overcome this limitation and examine body image dissatisfaction and perception of the attractive female body in Chinese and Croatian women and men using the correlational and experimental study research approaches. Two principal hypotheses were considered across the two studies, namely that thin internalization would predict body image dissatisfaction in both countries, and that both Chinese and Croatian women would display overall high levels of body image dissatisfaction; however, these levels would be higher for Chinese women. We designed two different studies to test our hypotheses. Study 1 investigated body image dissatisfaction in both samples, and influence of family, peers, and media on body image dissatisfaction, as well as male perspective on thin ideal in both countries. In Study 2, we experimentally manipulated the thin ideal in women from both countries to make causal inferences between thin internalization and body image dissatisfaction, thus overcoming the limitations related to the correlational nature of Study 1.

Study 1

Study 1 examined peer, family, and media influences on body image dissatisfaction in women from China and Croatia, as well as the importance of individual body items in each cultural context. In Study 1, we also examined men’s preferences for female body size in both cultures.

As already stated, both countries of our interest have witnessed a fast socio-economic transition in the past couple of decades (International Monetary Fund, Monetary, and Capital Markets Department, 2000). Both countries also have strong patriarchal traditions rooted in Confucianism (Leung, 2003) and Catholic Church (Ramet and Matic, 2007), where former emphasizes social and family hierarchy thus placing women in a subordinate position, and former advocates the ideas of equality, thus placing women in a more equal position to men. Furthermore, both countries are experiencing greater than ever interaction with Western cultural values (Leung, 2003). Separate researches from China and Croatia report increase in desire for thinness (Leung et al., 2001; Bralic and Kovacic, 2005; Rukavina and Pokrajac-Bulian, 2006; Chen et al., 2007) and increase in dieting and disordered eating among adolescent girls and young women (Pokrajac-Bulian, 1998; Pokrajac-Bulian et al., 2006; Jackson and Chen, 2008). Likewise, increased facial appearance dissatisfaction (Jackson and Chen, 2008), as well as height dissatisfaction, have been reported in the Chinese population (Watts, 2004; Chen et al., 2006). Also, recent research has proved that people in very wealthy and westernized countries of Asia had very high levels of body image dissatisfaction (BID; Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010), which is the effect we expected to observe in Chinese sample given the strong Chinese economic progression and a gradual shift toward individualistic values (Wu, 2004; Steele and Lynch, 2013). While the similar socio-economic transition is taking place in Croatia, Croatian women resemble more closely imposed Eurocentric beauty ideals widely represented by the Western media, which we believe could have a somewhat beneficial effect on their overall levels of BID in comparison to China.

Furthermore, previous studies have shown that one reason heterosexual women may internalize the thin-ideal is that thin women represented in the media tend to be rewarded by men (Fouts and Burggraf, 2000; Greenberg et al., 2003). Accordingly, a woman’s body image is strongly associated with her perception of what she thinks men prefer (Meltzer and McNulty, 2015). Numerous studies worldwide have reported that men have an overall preference for women with lower waist to hip ratio, which at the physiological level, has been associated with higher estrogen and progesterone levels (Jasienska et al., 2004), and thus higher possibility for conception (Jones et al., 2005). Consequently, the male perspective on desirable female size might greatly shape the general opinion on what is understood as physically attractive. This might be even more prominent in cultures with strong patriarchal traditions (Leung, 2003; Ramet and Matic, 2007), where men hold the primary power and have predominant roles in societies. Previous studies have identified an association between gender equality and Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions of individualism/collectivism, power distance, and masculinity-femininity, which strongly determine how gender-egalitarian a particular society is likely to be. China scores higher on power distance and masculinity and lowers on individualism compared to Croatia, which suggests that gender inequalities are higher in China compared to Croatia1. This is also consistent with the data reported by the United Nations and the gender inequality index, according to which China was on the 85th place for gender inequalities and Croatia on 46th2. Accordingly, our aim was to examine to which extent thin women were seen as more attractive in two countries of our interest. Specific hypotheses for Study 1 include:

(H1): In both countries, higher levels of thin internalization will predict higher body image dissatisfaction.

(H2): Chinese women will have higher levels of thin and musculature internalization, as well as higher levels of family, peer, and media pressure compared to Croatian women.

(H3): Chinese women will be more dissatisfied with their body image compared to Croatian women.

(H4): Chinese men will prefer thinner women than their Croatian counterparts.

Methods

Participants

The participants in this study were 321 women: 160 Chinese women aged between 18 and 30 years (M = 26.34, SD = 2.70); and 161 Croatian women aged between 18 and 30 years (M = 24.16, SD = 2.27), respectively, and 206 men: 106 Chinese men aged between 19 and 30 years (M = 26.48, SD = 2.68); and 100 Croatian men aged between 18 and 30 years (M = 22.17, SD = 3.18), respectively.

Procedure

Participants were recruited using a snowball sampling method, and they were invited to fill out online surveys posted at two online survey platforms: Survey Monkey3 for Croatian participants and Wenjuanxing4 for Chinese participants. Women were asked to fill out the following standardized instruments: Contour Rating Scale, Body Area Scale, and Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Questionnaire-4, as well as to provide their demographic data related to age, height, weight, and nationality, while men were asked to fill out Contour Rating Scale and to provide their demographic data. The Chinese and Croatian versions of the measures used in this study were translated as part of this study through formal procedures of translation and back-translation (Brislin, 1970; Song et al., 2019). The English version was used as a template from which the translation/back-translation was conducted for the Chinese and Croatian versions. Here we introduced the procedure using the Chinese version. The procedure of translation/back-translation in this study was conducted by the expert committee. Two translation teams were formed. Each translation team consisted of three native Chinese individuals who were proficient in both English and Chinese languages. The three translators in the first team made their translations independently, and then discussion ensued until a consensus was reached for the first drafts of the Chinese versions of the measures. The three translators in the second team made back-translation drafts independently and then made a final decision on the back-translation versions. Next, an expert committee was formed with a faculty member and a graduate student from the psychology department of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. All committee members were familiar with the questionnaires. Based on the comparison of the back-translation version and the original English version, and consideration of cultural adaptation, the expert committee made a decision on the final Chinese versions of the measures. The only difference for the Croatian version is that the committee members were made up of the first author and other two assistants proficient in both Croatian and English language.

The study was presented to participants as a study on the influence of the socio-cultural environment on body image. It took around 15 min for women to finish the survey and 5 min for men. By completing the survey, the participants have provided consent to participate in the research.

Measures

Contour rating scale – (CDRS; Thompson and Gray, 1995)

CDRS comprises nine drawings of a female figure. The figures gradually increase in size from extremely thin (1) to very obese (9). Participants are requested to rate the ideal figure, i.e., the one that reflects the best the way they ideally want to look (CDRSw2) and their current size, i.e., the one that closest resembles the way they perceive themselves (CDRSw1). The difference between the current size and ideal reproduces the body size dissatisfaction index (CDRSwscore) were requested to (a) choose the figure they perceived as the most attractive (CDRS1m); (b) choose the figure they thought most women find the most attractive and wish to have (CDRS2m) and (c) to choose the figure they thought was most widely represented in the media (CDRS3m). The previous study reported 14-week test–retest reliabilities ranging from 0.71 to 0.90, as well as satisfactory construct and discriminant validity (Wertheim et al., 2004). We slightly modified the figure drawings in a way that we removed lines picturing the ribs from drawings 1–4, and we obscured the women’s face and hair features with black squares (see “Supplementary Material”).

Body area scale – (BAS; Lerner et al., 1973)

BAS is an assessment system used for evaluating satisfaction with one’s body weight, body size, and satisfaction with different body parts. It contains 24 body parts or attributes rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from “very satisfied” to “very dissatisfied.” Body dissatisfaction is calculated by determining the number of body parts with which an individual expresses dissatisfaction. Dissatisfaction is determined by a response of “moderately dissatisfied” or “very dissatisfied.” Previous research has reported satisfactory reliability of this scale, as well as considerable “conceptual overlap” with other body image measures (Richards et al., 1990). In the present study, BAS has demonstrated excellent internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.88 in the Chinese sample and 0.89 in the Croatian sample.

Sociocultural attitudes toward appearance questionnaire-4 – (SATAQ-4; Schaefer et al., 2015)

SATAQ-4 has a total of 22 items that are divided across a five-factor structure containing the subscales of: (1) Thin Internalization: Thin/Low Body Fat subscale that contains five items and assesses to what degree a respondent internalizes a thin body with low body fat as an ideal. Example items include: I want my body to look very thin/I think a lot about having very little body fat. (2) Muscular Internalization: Muscular/Athletic subscale that contains five items and assesses to what degree a respondent internalizes an athletic body with muscles as an ideal. Example items include: I think a lot about looking muscular/I spend a lot of time doing things to look more muscular. (3) Family Pressures: Family subscale that contains four items and assesses to what degree a respondent experiences pressure from family to look a certain way. Example items include: I feel pressure from family members to look thinner/Family members encourage me to decrease my level of body fat. (4) Peer Pressures: Peers subscale that contains four items and assesses to what degree a respondent feels pressure from peers to look a certain way. Example items include: My peers encouraged me to get thinner./I get pressure from my peers to decrease my level of body fat. (5) Media Pressures: Media subscale that contains four items and assesses to what degree a respondent feels pressure from the media to look a certain way. Example items include: I feel pressure from the media to look thinner/I feel pressure from the media to improve my appearance. SATAQ-4 has already been used in Chinese (Jung and Forbes, 2007) and Croatian populations (Horak, 2008). In the present study, the SATAQ-4 subscales have demonstrated good to excellent internal consistency, with following Cronbach’s alphas: 0.60, 0.84, 0.88, 0.84, 0.95 for thin internalization, musculature internalization, family, peer and media pressure, respectively in Croatian sample; 0.81, 0.90, 0.79, 0.85, 0.86 for thin internalization, musculature internalization, family, peer and media pressure, respectively in a Chinese sample.

Body mass index – (BMI)

The self-reported height and self-reported weight were used to calculate the BMI in participants using the following formula: weight/height2.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 20 was used for data analysis (IBM; United States). First, we performed correlation analyses, which were followed by regression analyses and independent-sample t-tests for female samples. Afterward, group comparisons for men were conducted using independent sample t-tests, which were followed by 2 × 2 ANOVA. The detailed results are reported in Tables 16.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlation matrix for Chinese sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation matrix for Croatian sample.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of sociocultural predictors of body image dissatisfaction between two cultural groups.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple hierarchical regressions with body image dissatisfaction as assessed by CDRS scale as the dependent variable.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Independent Samples t -tests between BAS items in two cultural groups.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Comparison of Chinese and Croatian men’ view on female body size.

Results

Chinese women (M = 26.34, SD = 2.70) were older compared to Croatian women (M = 24.16, SD = 2.27), F(1,319) = 61.63, p < 0.001. Furthermore, BMI revealed no statistical difference in BMI among Croatian women (M = 21.99, SD = 3.46) and Chinese women (M = 21.12, SD = 4.75); F(1,318) = 3.57, p = 0.06. Descriptive statistics for all variables and bivariate correlations were also calculated for each sample independently and are reported in Tables 1, 2. Chinese men (M = 26.48, SD = 2.68) were older than Croatian men (M = 22.17, SD = 3.18), F(1,215) = 111.19, p < 0.001, while there was no difference in BMI between Croatian (M = 23.96, SD = 2.83) and Chinese men (M = 22.99, SD = 5.11); F(1,205) = 48.02, p = 0.09.

Next, independent samples t-tests were run to examine the potential differences between the internal and external pressures in both countries. Culture was used as the independent variable and thin internalization, musculature internalization, family pressure, media pressure and peer pressure as dependent variables. The independent-samples t-test revealed that Chinese women scored higher on thin internalization (4.26 ± 0.57) compared to Croatian women (3.17 ± 0.76), t(319) = −14.43, p < 0.001. They also scored higher on musculature internalization [(Chinese vs. Croatian = 2.74 ± 0.93 vs. 2.50 ± 0.85), t(319) = −2.38, p < 0.05], family pressure [(Chinese vs. Croatian = 2.87 ± 0.90 vs. M = 1.88 ± 1.03), t(319) = −17.09, p < 0.001], media pressure [(Chinese vs. Croatian = 3.32 ± 0.93 vs. M = 2.86 ± 1.38), t(319) = −3.49, p < 0.001], and peer pressure [(Chinese vs. Croatian = 3.29 ± 0.97 vs. M = 1.60 ± 0.79), t(319) = −5.72, p < 0.001]. Corresponding effect size is presented in Table 3.

To test which of the variables predicted body dissatisfaction assessed by the CDRS scale, we conducted a multiple hierarchical regression where age and BMI were included in the first block as predictor variables given that previous studies have identified these variables as relevant biological predictors in body image (Ålgars et al., 2009; Slevec and Tiggemann, 2011), while SATAQ-4 subscale scores were entered as predictor variables in the second block.

The regression results for the Croatian sample showed that age and BMI explained 48% of the variance, while SATAQ-4 variables accounted for 18% of the variance. In the final model, we found that BMI (β = 0.48, p < 0.001), Thin Internalization (β = 0.40, p < 0.001) and Media pressure (β = 0.15, p < 0.01) significantly predicted body image dissatisfaction assessed by CDRS scale. The regression results for the Chinese sample showed that age and BMI explained 5% of the variance, while SATAQ-4 variables accounted for 23% of the variance. In the final model, we found that BMI (β = 0.18, p < 0.01), and Thin Internalization (β = 0.38, p < 0.001) significantly predicted body image dissatisfaction assessed by CDRS scale (see Table 4).

Next, an independent-samples t-test was conducted to compare average body image dissatisfaction assessed by the BAS scale in two groups of women. The obtained results revealed a significant difference between Chinese (M = 12.33, SD = 5.74) and Croatian women (M = 4.70, SD = 4.15); t(319) = −13.65, p < 0.001. These results suggested an obvious difference in body image dissatisfaction assessed by the BAS scale between the two groups of women, where Croatian women were, on average, dissatisfied with four to five aspects of their physical appearance, while Chinese women were dissatisfied with 12 to 13 body areas. The independent samples t-tests were also run to examine the differences between the body image dissatisfaction assessed by the BAS scale with individual body parts in both countries. Statistically significant differences observed between two cultural groups on individual BAS items and their corresponding t-values and effect sizes are shown in Table 5.

When analyzing individual BAS scale items, it was found that Chinese women expressed high dissatisfaction with almost all body parts and were especially dissatisfied with their ears, eyes, and hair color. They also reported relatively high levels of dissatisfaction with their mouth, neck, ankles, and general appearance. Contrary, Croatian women revealed to be the most dissatisfied with their legs, weight, and the looks of their thighs. Interestingly, both Chinese (M = 2.79, SD = 1.16) and Croatian women (M = 2.78, SD = 1.31) were almost equally dissatisfied with their weight.

This discontent with body weight observed in both samples was furthermore supported by the body image dissatisfaction index (CDRS scale). The independent-samples t-test that was used to compare the CDRSw scores between the two groups revealed no statistically significant differences, thus suggesting that both Croatian (M = 1.07, SD = 1.45) and Chinese women (M = 1.33, SD = 1.59) were equally dissatisfied with their body size; t(319) = −1.50, p = 0.132. However, the perception of ideal body size was significantly different among the two groups, with Chinese women (M = 3.32, SD = 1.17) preferring significantly thinner bodies compared to Croatian women (M = 3.81, SD = 0.96); t(319) = 4.16, p < 0.001. Next, we examined the figures that the participants from both cultures selected from the CDRS scale as their current and their ideal figures. Among the examined samples, only 10% of Chinese women and 20.5% of Croatian women were completely satisfied with their body size, whereas 75% of Chinese women and 66.5% of Croatian women felt their body was bigger than their perceived ideal size. Figure 1 illustrates the trends in dissatisfaction with body size among the two examined groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Contour Rating Scale score percentages in both populations. A positive score indicates a preference for a slimmer body.

The independent samples t-test was also conducted in order to examine to which extent Chinese and Croatian men preferred thin to heavier women, which body size they thought was the most appealing to women, and which body size they thought was most represented in the media. The observed results suggested an obvious difference in body size preferences among Chinese and Croatian men, where Chinese men preferred thinner women (M = 3.67, SD = 1.21) compared to Croatian men (M = 4.22, SD = 1.03). Chinese men also selected significantly smaller body size as the one that women from their country would prefer (M = 2.73, SD = 1.43) compared to their Croatian counterparts (M = 3.17, SD = 1.01). The results are shown in Table 6.

Next, an additional analysis was performed by combing the men’s preference and women’s preference so as to explore the effect of gender and culture on desirable body size. A 2 (culture: China, Croatia) x 2 (gender: men, women) ANOVA was performed on the influence of culture and gender on the perception of the idealized body. First, we wanted to test the relationship between the body size most attractive to men (CDRS1m), and the one women rated as the most attractive (CDRSw2). Besides the significant main effect of culture on the perception of idealized body F(1,522) = 28.87, p < 0.001, the main effect of gender F(1,522) = 18.03, p < 0.001 was also significant, while the interaction between gender and culture was not. These results suggested that both Chinese (M = 3.32, SD = 1.17) and Croatian women (M = 3.81, SD = 0.96) wanted body size that was much thinner than the one chosen by men as desirable one (M = 3.67, SD = 1.20; M = 4.22, SD = 1.02, respectively).

Next, we wanted to examine the relationship between the body size men thought women want (CDRS2m) and the one women rated as the most attractive (CDRSw2). Again, besides the significant main effect of culture on the perception of idealized body F(1,521) = 27.64, p < 0.001, the main effect of gender was also significant F(1,521) = 36.46, p < 0.001, while the interaction between gender and culture was not. These results suggested that when comparing the body size men thought women from their countries might desire to actual preferences reported by Chinese (M = 3.32, SD = 1.17) and Croatian women (M = 3.81, SD = 0.96), men from both countries substantially misjudged the body size that women found attractive, opting for very small body size (M = 2.73, SD = 1.43; M = 3.17, SD = 1.01, respectively).

Discussion

In Study 1, we tested a series of hypotheses revealing that thin internalization was related to body image dissatisfaction assessed by the CDRS scale in both Chinese and Croatian samples. When accounting for other variables identified as relevant to body image dissatisfaction by previous studies (Bearman et al., 2006; Laus et al., 2011; Santana et al., 2013), thin internalization was still the strongest predictor variable. Furthermore, while women from both countries reported having a stronger preference for slimmer bodies compared to their current body size, this discrepancy, as well as more obvious preference for slimmer body size, was more prominent in a Chinese sample. Chinese women scored significantly higher on thin internalization compared to their Croatian counterparts, and they had significantly lower levels of satisfaction with almost all individual body parts assessed by the BAS scale compared to Croatian women, which was entirely consistent with our hypotheses.

Additionally, the obtained results revealed that women from both cultures wanted thinner bodies than the ones selected as the most attractive by men, which was consistent with previous studies (Swami et al., 2010). This was in line with evolutionary theory, according to which men view of the attractive body is an important determinant of body image satisfaction among heterosexual women (Bergstrom et al., 2004). Interestingly enough, men from both countries agreed on the female body size most commonly represented in media, which suggests that both Chinese and Croatian men started from a similar baseline, i.e., both cultural contexts were exposed to similar media images endorsing a similar desirable female size. Even so, Chinese men preferred thinner women compared to Croatian men, thus confirming our hypothesis. As already stated, gender inequalities are greater in Chinese culture compared to Croatian culture that suggests that Chinese, in general, hold a more traditional view of gender roles (Cohen and Kitayama, 2007), which also encompass the ideas of desirable female body image. If we consider that Asian women, in general, have thinner bodies compared to European women, it is possible that exposure to thin-ideal in such culture results in even more extreme pursuit of thinness, which is further reinforced by male-dominant hierarchical social structures.

Study 2

Study 1 showed that thin internalization was the strongest predictor of body image dissatisfaction among all the examined sociocultural influences and that male and female perception of desirable body size were significantly inconsistent, with women opting for significantly thinner body size compared to the one chosen by men in both countries. However, Study 1 was correlational, which means that causal relationships between thin internalization and body image dissatisfaction could not be inferred. In order to overcome this limitation, we experimentally manipulated thin exposure in women from both cultural groups. The specific hypothesis for Study 2 was that women in the experimental group from both countries would experience higher body image dissatisfaction following thin ideal priming, but the observed effect would be stronger among Chinese women.

Methods

Participants

The participants in this study were 200 women: 100 Chinese women aged between 18 and 30 years (M = 22.85, SD = 3.69), and 100 Croatian women aged between 18 and 30 years (M = 22.19, SD = 2.56), respectively. Participants were recruited through printed and electronic advertisements posted on different notice boards across countries of interest. They were randomly allocated to one of the groups (50 allocated to experimental; and 50 allocated to control group).

Procedure

The stimulus materials containing images of professional fashion models were drawn from two popular fashion outlets (Vogue and Yoka). A pilot test was conducted with a small group of female volunteers (N = 10; five from each cultural group) to ensure that the fashion models were considered attractive and thin by the desired populations (18 to 30 years). Photos of 30 models who had the highest attractiveness ratings were included in the study. All the photos were full-page (A4-size) pictures, and they featured a full view of a woman. Control advertisements featured various products, and they did not contain any persons or body parts (see “Supplementary Material”). Participants were informed that they were participating in an experiment on consumer behavior. After receiving the instructions, participants were asked to complete a demographic questionnaire and then to rate each of the 30 advertisements using the following statements:” In my opinion, this ad was effective./In my opinion this ad was ineffective.” The purpose of this measure was to make the cover story more believable and to encourage participants to pay closer attention to presented advertisements. To furthermore distract the participants from the real purpose of the study, additional marketing questions were created by the investigator. After viewing all of the images, participants completed all the questions, including the CDRS scale. The whole procedure took approximately 15 min, after which the participants were debriefed about the actual purpose of the study.

Measures

Contour rating scale – (CDRS; Thompson and Gray, 1995)

We used a somewhat modified version of the CDRS scale, where nine drawings of a female figure were substituted with images of virtual models created by modelmydiet website. The figures were modified to look more closely virtual models that can be found in online stores with virtual dressing rooms or shopping applications (e.g., Looklet, H&M), thus making the cover story more believable (see “Supplementary Materials”). The figures gradually increased in size from extremely thin (1) to very obese (9). Participants were requested to rate the ideal figure, i.e., the one that reflects the best the way they ideally want to look (CDRS2) and their current size, i.e., the one that closely resembles the way they perceive themselves (CDRS1). The difference between the ideal and current size reproduces the body size dissatisfaction index.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 20 was used for all statistical analyses. We conducted a series of 2 × 2 ANOVA analyses to compare groups across cultures and conditions. Detailed results are reported in Tables 7, 8.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Descriptive Statistics for evaluation of body image size by condition and culture.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Group condition x Culture Factorial Analysis of Variance for the perception of current body size, ideal body size, and body image dissatisfaction.

Results

A 2 (culture: China, Croatia) x 2 (condition: experimental, control) analysis of variance was conducted on the influence of culture and thin exposure on the perception of current body, idealized body and body image dissatisfaction calculated as the discrepancy between the former two (see Tables 7, 8).

As shown in Table 8, the main effect of culture F(1,195) = 0.41, p = 0.52, or thin exposure F(1,195) = 0.16, p = 0.69 on perception of current body size, i.e., CDRS1, were not significant, nor was their interaction; F(1,195) = 0.01, p = 0.91. This was in line with our expectations, given that priming of the thin ideal was less likely to change the general idea one has of their actual size compared to body size one desires to have.

Regarding the perception of ideal body size, i.e., CDRS2, statistical analyses yielded completely opposite results to CDRS1, such that there was a significant interaction effect between culture and thin exposure F(1,195) = 7.93, p < 0.01. Also, the main effects of culture F(1,195) = 25.89, p < 0.001, and condition were both statistically significant F(1,195) = 12.90, p < 0.001; thus suggesting that experimental manipulation had a significant effect on the perception of the ideal body size among women assigned to experimental groups in both countries, where they expressed stronger preference toward thinner body size compared to control groups.

Finally, regarding the CDRS score, i.e., the discrepancy between the ideal and preferred body size, the interaction effect was not significant F(1,195) = 3.54, p < 0.06; while both main effects had significant effect on CDRS score, whereas women from China (M = 1.44, SD = 0.13) reported significantly higher levels of CDRS score compared to Croatian women (M = 0.92, SD = 0.13) F(1,195) = 8.35, p < 0.001; while women exposed to experimental manipulation (M = 1.37, SD = 0.13) F(1,195) = 4.43, p < 0.04 reported significantly higher levels of CDRS score compared to those from control groups (M = 0.99, SD = 0.13).

Discussion

The obtained results confirmed that exposure to thin-ideal leads to higher levels of body image dissatisfaction in women regardless of their cultural identity, which is in line with existing research on thin internalization and body image dissatisfaction (e.g., Thompson and Stice, 2001; Stice, 2002; Tiggemann, 2003; Keery et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2004; Keel and Forney, 2013). The reported results also provided support for our hypothesis, according to which Chinese women would experience higher levels of body image dissatisfaction, which was also in line with existing research arguing that Asian women are very susceptible to body image dissatisfactions (e.g., Han, 2003; Jung and Lee, 2006; Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010; Rongmuang et al., 2011), and that Asian countries are more prone to body image dissatisfactions compared to European countries (Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010).

General Discussion

The current study aimed to examine how Croatian and Chinese women and men felt about female attractiveness, as well as to understand the influence of cultural background on body image satisfaction in women. Overall, the results of the current study supported our hypotheses, showing that Chinese women had higher levels of musculature and thin internalization, where the latter one predicted body image dissatisfaction in both samples. Chinese women also felt more pressure from family, peers, and media to conform to standard beauty norms compared to Croatian women. The samples also differed in their perceptions of preferable body size, where Chinese women had a more negative perception of their actual body size. Furthermore, women from both cultures revealed a preference for thinner figures than the ones selected as the most attractive by the opposite sex. Similarly, Chinese men reported preferring women of smaller size compared to Croatian men, even though men from both countries were in agreement on the female body size most widely represented in the media, which further suggests that both cultural contexts were exposed to similar thin-ideal media messages. Finally, the experimental manipulation revealed that thin exposure led to higher body image dissatisfaction in women from both cultures; however, this effect was more obvious in Chinese women. These findings are reviewed in more detail below.

Our results showed that the thin internalization strongly predicted body image dissatisfaction in both countries, being the strongest predictor of body image dissatisfaction in Chinese women, and the second strongest predictor in Croatian women. These findings are in line with a number of previous studies that have shown how thin internalization is the risk factor for body image dissatisfaction (Keel and Forney, 2013). They are also consistent with the premise that the thin-ideal internalization is the essential concept for complete comprehension of body image dissatisfaction (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., 2012), and compared to the awareness of socio-cultural standards or mere exposure to idealized media images, it has the stronger impact on body image dissatisfaction (Cafri et al., 2005). Previous studies have shown that not all women respond to idealized media images and imposed sociocultural standards in the same way (Joshi et al., 2004), and those women who internalize the prevalent socio-cultural attitudes about the desirable appearance are at the higher risk of developing body image dissatisfaction (Thompson and Stice, 2001). Additionally, our results revealed significant differences between Chinese and Croatian women on thin internalization, musculature internalization, family pressure, media pressure, and peer pressure, whereas Chinese women reported being more susceptible to all of the above. Cultural context and ethnicity have been shown to have a protective effect against body image dissatisfaction (Warren et al., 2005); nonetheless, Asian cultures appear to be very susceptible to body image dissatisfaction (Jung and Lee, 2006), which was consistent with our results. This could be explained by the construct of collectivism, which includes psychological concepts such as attitudes, values, self-representations, and social contexts (Triandis, 2001). Brewer and Chen (2007) have introduced an aspect of collectivism that differentiates collectivism into relational collectivism and group collectivism. Relational collectivism refers to the relationship and role that individual has with regard to others, where individuals achieve a balance between the expression of social conformity and individuality that culturally differs in relation to specific relational or more symbolic collective contexts. According to this approach, Westerners achieve collectivism through group collectivism, whereas Easterners tend to rely on relational collectivism (Kreuzbauer et al., 2009). As was previously mentioned, China has its collectivist cultural traditions rooted in Confucianism (Leung, 2003), while in Croatia, these traditions stem from the long-lasting influence of the Catholic Church (Ramet and Matic, 2007). Accordingly, religious affiliation falls under group collectivism, whereas Confucianism is more deeply ingrained in Chinese social life and thus is more likely to affect a person’s identity (Huang and Gove, 2015). In cultures with strong relational collectivism, relationships with other members of the group and the interconnectedness between people have a central role in a person’s identity. Existing studies have shown that perceived norms of more proximal or relevant reference groups may have a stronger impact on individual behaviors for members of such groups, including one’s gender (Lewis and Neighbors, 2004), which in turn may affect perceptions of group-specific body image norms. These observations were especially interesting in relation to musculature internalization. Originally, musculature internalization SATAQ subscale was used for the male population as body image concerns in male populations mainly involve a desire for greater muscularity (Cafri et al., 2002; Thompson and Cafri, 2007). Given that muscular and fit female bodies have become increasingly popular in Western societies’ over recent years (Bozsik et al., 2018; Rodgers et al., 2018), we assumed this same trend might be slowly spreading outside the Western countries, which was supported by our study results. Since Chinese women appeared to be very susceptible to thin internalization, it was an educated guess that they will also have a stronger tendency toward endorsing new body ideals such as musculature or fit ideal. In fact, one study showed that both Chinese men and women rated male and female Caucasian models as more attractive than Chinese models (Jankowiak et al., 2008), thus highlighting the role of the media in transmitting Western appearance ideals to non-Western societies, as well as the extent of media’s influence in such contexts (e.g., Becker, 2004; Frith and Gleeson, 2004). Western cultural values instruct that appearance is vital to women’s value and role in society, and that thinness guarantees success and life satisfaction (Warren et al., 2005). In non-Western contexts, women would have to strive even harder to meet the preset ideals, given that modification of certain physical features goes beyond simple dieting, thus suggesting that body image would be more complex concept related to other physical features besides body fat, which was supported by our results but also by previous studies (Jackson and Chen, 2008; Tewari and Alvarez, 2009). Accordingly, in the present study, Chinese women appeared to be less satisfied with all of the physical items addressed in the study. These results were also in line with previous studies that have shown how people from affluent and westernized Asian countries are more dissatisfied with their bodies than people from the United States, while Americans are less happy with their bodies than Europeans (Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010). Furthermore, Chinese women were especially dissatisfied with their ears, eyes, and hair color. Contrary, Croatian women revealed to be most dissatisfied with their legs, weight, and the looks of their thighs. The dissatisfaction with facial features and hair has been previously reported as very relevant factors in overall body image satisfaction in Asian women (Tewari and Alvarez, 2009). In addition, separate researches from both countries have reported an increase in desire for thinness (Leung et al., 2001; Bralic and Kovacic, 2005; Rukavina and Pokrajac-Bulian, 2006; Chen et al., 2007) and increase in dieting and disordered eating among adolescent girls and young women (Rukavina and Pokrajac-Bulian, 2006). Likewise, increased facial appearance dissatisfaction (Jackson and Chen, 2008), as well as height dissatisfaction in China, have also been reported (Chen et al., 2006). A somewhat surprising finding was the overall satisfaction with individual body sites observed in the Croatian sample. While the observed differences did provide full support for our hypothesis that was based on the fact that Croatian women resemble more closely imposed Eurocentric beauty ideals widely represented by the Western media, the discontent in the Croatian sample was unexpectedly small. In fact, it appears that the Croatian sample perfectly fits the thin ideal paradigm based on the normative discontent with body size (Littleton and Ollendick, 2003) whereas body image dissatisfaction appears to be a more complex concept in a Chinese sample. It should also be noted that Chinese people tend to be more negative in self-evaluations (Sedikides et al., 2003, 2005), as well as more self-critical (Heine et al., 2000), which could also potentially explain the observed differences, and thus needs to be examined by future studies.

Interestingly, both Chinese and Croatian women were almost equally dissatisfied with their body size; however, the perception of ideal body size was significantly different between the two groups, with Chinese women reporting higher preferences for thinner bodies compared to Croatian women. Further analysis revealed that among the examined samples, only 10% of Chinese women and 20.5% of Croatian women were completely satisfied with their body size, whereas 75% of Chinese women and 66.5% of Croatian women felt their body was bigger than their perceived ideal size. These results provided further support for the overwhelming endorsement of thin-ideal through industrial and developing countries (Leavy, 2004). In the present study, we did not use the Western sample, where most of the thin ideals appear to originate from; nevertheless, according to a study that has compared body image dissatisfaction in the United States, Ukraine, and Ghana using the same measurement instrument as used in the present study (Frederick et al., 2008), United States women perceived themselves as the smallest compared to Chinese and Croatian women; whereas their perception of the ideal figure was bigger compared to Chinese preference and smaller compared to Croatian preference. Also, they were more satisfied with their body than Chinese women but less satisfied than Croatian women, which is in line with existing studies showing that people from affluent and westernized Asian countries are more dissatisfied with their bodies than people from the United States; while Americans are less happy with their bodies than Europeans or Australians (Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010). This could be explained by the fact that most of the imposed ideals are Eurocentric, which means that women from these countries are more likely to closely fit the ideals compared to Asian populations or countries with greater ethnic diversity, such as the United States. The observed results could also be partially explained from the evolutionary perspective suggesting that men’s view of the attractive body is an important determinant of body image satisfaction among heterosexual women (Bergstrom et al., 2004). Interestingly, women from both countries revealed a preference for thinner figures than the ones selected as the most attractive by the opposite sex, which was consistent with previous studies suggesting that media alter the ideal female body, which women in turn erroneously interpret as the most desirable to men (Sanderson et al., 2002). Similarly, men from both countries selected significantly smaller body size as the one that women from their countries would prefer to have. It is possible that men from both countries thought women wanted such thin bodies, given that women are universally known to diet more (Morry and Staska, 2001) and are more encouraged to diet. Also, women’s magazines tend to display much more ads and articles encouraging weight loss compared to men’s magazines, while, over 75% of the magazine covers for female audience comprise at least one message about how to change the physical appearance (Malkin et al., 1999). Furthermore, Chinese men reported to prefer women of smaller size compared to Croatian men, even though men from both countries were in agreement on the female body size most widely represented in the media, which further suggested that both cultural contexts were exposed to similar thin-ideal media messages, but that thin ideal was endorsed to the higher context in China. Not that long ago, cultures with enough resources had a higher preference for slender bodies and lower body mass index, whereas cultures with scarcer resources had a strong preference for heavier body types (Marlowe and Wetsman, 2001; Swami and Tovée, 2005). Recent research has challenged this type of categorization, proposing that distinctions made based on this cultural basis and distinctions are no longer true and that it is more appropriate to focus on sites’ social, economic status discrimination. That is to say that socioeconomically developed sites have fully adopted thin ideal as a direct result of global westernization (Swami, 2015). Westernized images and products have become especially powerful in non-Western contexts precisely because of their perceived “exclusivity” (Mazzarella, 2003), and those who found themselves between the rapidly changing traditional and modernized world might experience internal uncertainty and identity crises, which in turn might have adverse consequences on their perception and experience of their own body (Nasser et al., 2003). This was further supported with our Study 2, which confirmed how exposure to thin images negatively affected body image satisfaction in women from both cultures, which was also consistent with numerous previous studies (Wegner et al., 2000; Brown and Dittmar, 2005; Harper and Tiggemann, 2008). The observed differences were more prominent in Chinese sample which was in line with the results from Study 1 and Study 2, where levels of thin internalization did significantly vary between two examined cultures; however, they were still universally present in both of the examined samples leading to increased levels of body image dissatisfaction.

Our results provide the basis for a more comprehensive understanding of body image dissatisfaction in China and Croatia. While Croatian women appeared to be relatively satisfied with different aspects of their body, their discontent was relatively consistently expressed across both measures used to evaluate body image size. However, in Chinese women, the discontent with body weight appeared to be less prominent when participants were asked about other physical features, and more evident when participants were asked to focus exclusively on body size. This might be related to variations in cognitive processing styles considering that Chinese people tend to have holistic thinking style (Varnum et al., 2010), and it is possible that when asked to focus on different body parts and facial features, the later ones came to prominence as more important for the overall perception of body image. Contrary, it is possible that thin ideal has already become a normative source of discontent among Chinese women, where discontent with other physical features is emerging as a potentially more problematic matter. The results from the present study could be used to design different counseling programs that could be delivered to women of different age groups. The reported results could also be used by counselors to develop different scales and evaluation instruments, keeping in mind the specific cultural differences of each population, such as the overall tendency of Chinese participants to be more self-critical, as well as differences in thinking styles that could be relevant when selecting the scales to evaluate the body image. Furthermore, women should be explicitly educated about the potential impact of consistent media exposure on body image. They should also be provided education on media literacy, which would allow them to critically analyze the content they are exposed to on a daily basis. In fact, critical screening of the media, and especially online media, appears to be of great importance, especially if we know that an average person spends approximately 6 h per day on the Internet5. Finally, parents should be educated on the effect their behavior may have on their child’s perception of their own body. They should limit the “fat talk,” avoid dieting discourses in front of their children, and in general, they should teach their children to focus on the functionality and health of their bodies, as opposed to the looks. The protective role of the family might be especially important for Chinese women since when activating the need for belonging, which previous studies have shown to be related with self-esteem, and approval or support from others (Strudwicke, 2000), Chinese women are more likely to focus and count on relational collectivism. Individuals who internally identify with a culture see their cultural group membership as an integral part of who they are. Accordingly, they are more likely to engage in events and behaviors that are related to their culture (Yeh and Huang, 1996). These results encourage future researchers to expand upon this current study to give voice to unrepresented samples in the body image literature and to develop research of clinical utility for practitioners committed to providing culturally-sensitive services to those susceptible or affected by negative body image.

Limitations

This study has some limitations that need to be pointed out. First, the present work did not include the Western sample. While the comparison of European and Asian populations provided valuable information, the inclusion of Western samples would provide useful information as well as a valuable reference point for levels of body image dissatisfaction, given that thin-ideal initially originated in industrialized countries. Another limitation refers to the self-report format of the questionnaires, where observed findings may be influenced by a variety of factors such as participant’s feelings or social desirability. For example, body image is often a sensitive topic, and participants may feel too ashamed to report their actual opinions or private details. Thus, there is no way of knowing if reported accounts truthfully represent participants’ attitudes and feelings relating to appearance and body image. Existing studies have shown that Eastern cultures tend to be more negative in self-evaluations (Sedikides et al., 2003), and more self-critical (Heine et al., 2000), which might have a certain effect in this line of research, and thus should be considered by future studies. Finally, Study 1 was a cross-sectional study, and causal relationships could not be inferred, which was partially addressed by Study 2; nonetheless, future experimental studies are necessary to further verify our findings.

Conclusion

The present research study explored variables related to body image dissatisfaction among Croatian and Chinese women. Our findings indicated that thin internalization could predict body image dissatisfaction in two transition economies, China and Croatia. Besides, Chinese women were, in general, more dissatisfied with their bodies.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, XR, upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences.

Author Contributions

IS conceived the presented idea and developed the theory and wrote the manuscript. XD and IS collected and analyzed the data for Studies 1 and 2. XR and IS collected and analyzed the data for Study 3. All authors conceived and planned the experiments, while XR and IS carried out the experiments. All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript. XR supervised the project.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00731/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.hofstede-insights.com/product/compare-countries/
  2. ^ http://hdr.undp.org/en/content/gender-inequality-index-gii
  3. ^ www.surveymonkey.com
  4. ^ www.wjx.cn
  5. ^ www.statista.com

References

Abraczinskas, M., Fisak, B. Jr., and Barnes, R. D. (2012). The relation between parental influence, body image, and eating behaviors in a nonclinical female sample. Body Image 9, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ålgars, M., Santtila, P., Varjonen, M., Witting, K., Johansson, A., Jern, P., et al. (2009). The adult body: how age, gender, and body mass index are related to body image. J. Aging Health 21, 1112–1132. doi: 10.1177/0898264309348023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballentine, L. W., and Ogle, J. P. (2005). The making and unmaking of body problems in Seventeen magazine, 1992–2003. Fam. Consum. Sci. Res. J. 33, 281–307. doi: 10.1177/1077727x04274114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basabe, N., and Ros, M. (2005). Cultural dimensions and social behavior correlates: individualism-collectivism and power distance. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 18, 189–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bearman, S. K., Presnell, K., Martinez, E., and Stice, E. (2006). The skinny on body image dissatisfaction: a longitudinal study of adolescent girls and boys. J. Youth Adolesc. 35, 217–229. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9010-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, A. E. (2004). Television, disordered eating, and young women in Fiji: negotiating body image and identity during rapid social change. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 28, 533–559. doi: 10.1007/s11013-004-1067-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedford, J. L., and Johnson, C. S. (2006). Societal influences on body image dissatisfaction in younger and older women. J. Women Aging 18, 41–55. doi: 10.1300/j074v18n01_04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, B. T., and Dittmar, H. (2011). Does media type matter? The role of identification in adolescent girls’ media consumption and the impact of different thin-ideal media on body image. Sex Roles 65:478. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9964-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergstrom, R. L., Neighbors, C., and Lewis, M. A. (2004). Do men find “bony” women attractive? Consequences of misperceiving opposite sex perceptions of attractive body image. Body Image 1, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/s1740-1445(03)00025-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bessenoff, G. R. (2006). Can the media affect us? Social comparison, self-discrepancy, and the thin ideal. Psychol. Women Q. 30, 239–251. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00292.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozsik, F., Whisenhunt, B. L., Hudson, D. L., Bennett, B., and Lundgren, J. D. (2018). Thin is in? Think again: the rising importance of muscularity in the thin ideal female body. Sex Roles 79, 609–615. doi: 10.1007/s11199-017-0886-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bralic, I., and Kovacic, V. (2005). Social and behavioural determinants of body mass index among adolescent females in Croatia. Public Health 119, 189–191. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2004.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, M. B., and Chen, Y. R. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychol. Rev. 114, 133. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.114.1.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A., and Dittmar, H. (2005). Think “thin” and feel bad: the role of appearance schema activation, attention level, and thin–ideal internalization for young women’s responses to ultra–thin media ideals. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24, 1088–1113. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2005.24.8.1088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brumberg, J. J. (2000). Fasting Girls: The History of Anorexia Nervosa. New York, NY: Vintage.

Google Scholar

Bush, H. M., Williams, R. G. A., Lean, M. E. J., and Anderson, A. S. (2001). Body image and weight consciousness among South Asian, Italian and general population women in Britain. Appetite 37, 207–215. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cafri, G., Strauss, J., and Thompson, J. K. (2002). Male body image: satisfaction and its relationship to well-being using the somatomorphic matrix. Int. J. Men’s Health 1:215. doi: 10.3149/jmh.0102.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cafri, G., Yamamiya, Y., Brannick, M., and Thompson, J. K. (2005). The influence of sociocultural factors on body image: a meta−analysis. Clin. Psychol. 12, 421–433. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpi053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson Jones, D. (2004). Body image among adolescent girls and boys: a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 40:823. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, T. F., and Pruzinsky, T. (eds.) (2002). “Future challenges for body image theory, research, and clinical practice,” in Body Image: A Handbook of Theory, Research, and Clinical Practice (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 509–516.

Google Scholar

Cash, T. F., Theriault, J., and Annis, N. M. (2004). Body image in an interpersonal context: adult attachment, fear of intimacy and social anxiety. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 89–103. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.1.89.26987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catina, A., and Joja, O. (2001). “Emerging markets: submerging women,” in Eating Disorders and Cultures in Transition, eds M. Nasser, M. Katzman, and L. Gordon (Hove: Brunner-Routledge), 111–120.

Google Scholar

Chan, J. (2003). “Giving priority of the worst off: a confucian perspective on social welfare,” in Confucianism for the Modern World, eds D. A. Bell and H. Chaibong (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 161–177.

Google Scholar

Chen, H., Gao, X., and Jackson, T. (2007). Predictive models for understanding body image dissatisfaction among young males and females in China. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1345–1356. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., Jackson, T., and Huang, X. (2006). The negative physical self scale: initial development and validation in samples of Chinese adolescents and young adults. Body Image 3, 401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, D., and Kitayama, S. (2007). “Cultural psychology: this stanza and the next,” in Handbook of Cultural Psychology, eds S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 847–851.

Google Scholar

Costa, P. T. Jr., Terracciano, A., and McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 81:322. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Čevnik, L. (2016). The impact of value orientations on body image and the use of physical beauty practices by young women in slovenia, croatia and serbia. Sociologija 58, 53–71. doi: 10.2298/soc1601053c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etu, S. F., and Gray, J. J. (2010). A preliminary investigation of the relationship between induced rumination and state body image dissatisfaction and anxiety. Body Image 7, 82–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, C. J., Munoz, M. E., Contreras, S., and Velasquez, K. (2011). Mirror, mirror on the wall: peer competition, television influences, and body image dissatisfaction. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 30, 458–483. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.5.458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzsimmons-Craft, E. E., Harney, M. B., Koehler, L. G., Danzi, L. E., Riddell, M. K., and Bardone-Cone, A. M. (2012). Explaining the relation between thin ideal internalization and body image dissatisfaction among college women: the roles of social comparison and body surveillance. Body Image 9, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fouts, G., and Burggraf, K. (2000). Television situation comedies: Female weight, male negative comments, and audience reactions. Sex Roles 42, 925–932.

Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Forbes, G. B., and Anna, B. (2008). Female body image dissatisfaction and perceptions of the attractive female body in Ghana, the Ukraine, and the United States. Psihologijske Teme 17, 203–219.

Google Scholar

Frith, H., and Gleeson, K. (2004). Clothing and embodiment: men managing body image and appearance. Psychol. Men Masc. 5:40. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.5.1.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, R. A. (2001). Eating disorders east and west: a culture-bound syndrome unbound. Eat. Disord. Cult. Trans. 1:16.

Google Scholar

Grabe, S., and Hyde, J. S. (2006). Ethnicity and body image dissatisfaction among women in the United States: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132:622. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., and Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body image dissatisfactions among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychol. Bull. 134:460. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grammas, D. L., and Schwartz, J. P. (2009). Internalization of messages from society and perfectionism as predictors of male body image. Body Image 6, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. A., Davids, C. M., Skaggs, A. K., Riopel, C. M., and Hallengren, J. J. (2008). Femininity and eating disorders. Eat. Disord. 16, 283–293. doi: 10.1080/10640260802115829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, B. S., Eastin, M., Hofschire, L., Lachlan, K., and Brownell, K. D. (2003). Portrayals of overweight and obese individuals on commercial television. Am. J. Public Health 93, 1342–1348. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.8.1342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamamura, T., and Xu, Y. (2015). Changes in Chinese culture as examined through changes in personal pronoun usage. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 46, 930–941. doi: 10.1177/0022022115592968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, M. (2003). Body image dissatisfaction and eating disturbance among Korean college female students: relationships to media exposure, upward comparison, and perceived reality. Commun. Stud. 54, 65–78. doi: 10.1080/10510970309363266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. C. I. (1995). Asian eyes: body image and eating disorders of Asian and Asian American women. Eat. Disord. 3, 8–19. doi: 10.1080/10640269508249141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayaki, J., Friedman, M. A., and Brownell, K. D. (2002). Shame and severity of bulimic symptoms. Eat. Behav. 3, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(01)00046-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, B., and Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal media images on women’s self-objectification, mood, and body image. Sex Roles 58, 649–657. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9379-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazzard, V. M., Schaefer, L. M., Schaumberg, K., Bardone-Cone, A. M., Frederick, D. A., Klump, K. L., et al. (2019). Testing the tripartite influence model among heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women. Body Image 30, 145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heine, S. J., Takata, T., and Lehman, D. R. (2000). Beyond self-presentation: evidence for selfcriticism among Japanese. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 71–78. doi: 10.1177/0146167200261007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmqvist, K., and Frisén, A. (2010). Body image dissatisfaction across cultures: findings and research problems. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 18, 133–146. doi: 10.1002/erv.965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, D. (2008). Body Satisfaction in Relation with Sex and Sexual Orientation. Doctoral dissertation, University in Rijeka, Croatia.

Google Scholar

Hua, W. E. N. (2009). “Being good-looking is capital”: cosmetic surgery in China today. Asian Anthropol. 8, 89–107. doi: 10.1080/1683478x.2009.10552588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, G. H. C., and Gove, M. (2015). “Confucianism, Chinese families, and academic achievement: exploring how Confucianism and Asian descendant parenting practices influence children’s academic achievement,” in Science Education in East Asia, ed. M. S. Khine (Cham: Springer), 41–66. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16390-1_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunnicutt, G. (2009). Varieties of patriarchy and violence against women: resurrecting “patriarchy” as a theoretical tool. Violence Against Women 15, 553–573. doi: 10.1177/1077801208331246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huxley, C. J., Halliwell, E., and Clarke, V. (2015). An examination of the tripartite influence model of body image: does women’s sexual identity make a difference? Psychol. Women Q. 39, 337–348. doi: 10.1177/0361684314554917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglehart, R., and Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. Am. Sociol. Rev. 65, 19–51.

Google Scholar

Inglehart, R. F., Norris, P., and Welzel, C. (2002). Gender equality and democracy. Comp. Sociol. 1, 235–264.

Google Scholar

International Monetary Fund, Monetary, and Capital Markets Department (2000). Annual Report on Exchange Arrangements and Exchange Restrictions, 2000. Washington DC: International Monetary Fund.

Google Scholar

Jackson, T., and Chen, H. (2008). Sociocultural influences on body image dissatisfactions of young Chinese males. J. Adolesc. Res. 23, 154–171.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jackson, T., and Chen, H. (2010). Factor structure of the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-3) among adolescent boys in China. Body Image 7, 349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankowiak, W., Gray, P. B., and Hattman, K. (2008). Globalizing evolution: female choice, nationality, and perception of sexual beauty in China. Cross Cult. Res. 42, 248–269. doi: 10.1177/1069397108317672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, K. A., Phelps, L., and Bross, A. L. (2001). Body image dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and self−esteem in African American college females. Psychol. Sch. 38, 491–496. doi: 10.1002/pits.1037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasienska, G., Ziomkiewicz, A., Ellison, P. T., Lipson, S. F., and Thune, I. (2004). Large breasts and narrow waists indicate high reproductive potential in women. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, 1213–1217. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, F., and Wardle, J. (2005). Dietary restraint, body image dissatisfaction, and psychological distress: a prospective analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114:119. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.114.1.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Smith, M. L., et al. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Horm. Behav. 48, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. C. (2011). Interpersonal and familial influences on the development of body image. Body Image 2, 110–118.

Google Scholar

Joshi, R., Herman, C. P., and Polivy, J. (2004). Self−enhancing effects of exposure to thin−body images. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 333–341. doi: 10.1002/eat.10253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, J., and Lee, S. H. (2006). Cross−cultural comparisons of appearance self-schema, body image, self−esteem, and dieting behavior between Korean and US women. Family Consum. Sci. Res. J. 34, 350–365. doi: 10.1177/1077727x06286419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, J., and Forbes, G. B. (2007). Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating among college women in China, South Korea, and the United States: contrasting predictions from sociocultural and feminist theories. Psychol. Women Q. 31, 381–393. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2007.00387.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, J., Forbes, G. B., and Chan, P. (2010). Global body and muscle satisfaction among college men in the United States and Hong Kong-China. Sex Roles 63, 104–117. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9760-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karazsia, B. T., and Crowther, J. H. (2008). Psychological and behavioral correlates of the SATAQ-3 with males. Body Image 5, 109–115. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keel, P. K., and Forney, K. J. (2013). Psychosocial risk factors for eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 433–439. doi: 10.1002/eat.22094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keery, H., Van den Berg, P., and Thompson, J. K. (2004). An evaluation of the tripartite influence model of body image dissatisfaction and eating disturbance with adolescent girls. Body Image 1, 237–251. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H. C. (2003). Religion, Body Weight, Body Image, Dieting, Fat Intake, and Physical Activity. Ph.D. Thesis, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY.

Google Scholar

Kim, K. H. C. (2006). Religion, body satisfaction and dieting. Appetite 46, 285–296. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreuzbauer, R., Chiu, C. Y., and Lin, S. (2009). Relational versus group collectivism and optimal distinctiveness in a consumption context. ACR North Am. Adv. 36:742.

Google Scholar

Laus, M. F., Costa, T. M. B., and Almeida, S. S. (2011). Body image dissatisfaction and its relationship with physical activity and body mass index in Brazilian adolescents. J. Bras. Psiquiatr. 60, 315–320. doi: 10.1590/s0047-20852011000400013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavy, P. (2004). “The global marketing of body image disturbance: mass media as a mediating structure between global capital and women’s bodies,” in XXIII CSSP (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science), 1–25.

Google Scholar

Lee, S. (2000). Eating disorders are becoming more common in the East too. BMJ 321:1023.

Google Scholar

Lennon, S. J., Rudd, N. A., Sloan, B., and Kim, J. S. (1999). Attitudes toward gender roles, self-esteem, and body image: application of a model. Clothing Textiles Res. J. 17, 191–202. doi: 10.1177/0887302x9901700403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, R. M., Karabenick, S. A., and Stuart, J. L. (1973). Relations among physical attractiveness, body attitudes, and self-concept in male and female college students. J. Psychol. 85, 119–129. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1973.9923870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, F., Lam, S., and Sze, S. (2001). Cultural expectations of thinness in Chinese women. Eat. Disord. 9, 339–350. doi: 10.1080/106402601753454903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A. S. (2003). Feminism in transition: Chinese culture, ideology and the development of the women’s movement in China. Asia Pacific J. Manag. 20, 359–374.

Google Scholar

Levine, M. P., and Chapman, K. (2011). “Media influences on body image,” in Body Image: A Handbook of Science, Practice, and Prevention, 2nd Edn, eds T. F. Cash, and L. Smolak (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 101–109.

Google Scholar

Levine, M. P., and Piran, N. (2004). The role of body image in the prevention of eating disorders. Body Image 1, 57–70. doi: 10.1016/s1740-1445(03)00006-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, M. P., and Smolak, L. (2010). “Cultural influences on body image and the eating disorders,” in The Oxford Handbook of Eating Disorders, eds W. S. Agras and A. Robinson (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 223–246.

Google Scholar

Lewis, M. A., and Neighbors, C. (2004). Gender-specific misperceptions of college student drinking norms. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 18:334. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.18.4.334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindridge, A. M., and Wang, C. (2008). Saving “face” in China: modernization, parental pressure, and plastic surgery. J. Consum. Behav. 7, 496–508. doi: 10.1002/cb.267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littleton, H. L., and Ollendick, T. (2003). Negative body image and disordered eating behavior in children and adolescents: what places youth at risk and how can these problems be prevented? Clin. Child Family Psychol. Rev. 6, 51–66.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lydecker, J. A., White, M. A., and Grilo, C. M. (2017). Form and formulation: examining the distinctiveness of body image constructs in treatment-seeking patients with binge-eating disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 85, 1095. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malkin, A. R., Wornian, K., and Chrisler, J. C. (1999). Women and weight: gendered messages on magazine covers. Sex Roles 40, 647–655.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marcus, R., and Harper, C. (2014). Gender Justice and Social Norms—Processes of Change for Adolescent Girls. London: Overseas Development Institute.

Google Scholar

Marlowe, F., and Wetsman, A. (2001). Preferred waist-to-hip ratio and ecology. Personal. Ind. Diff. 30, 481–489. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00039-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzarella, W. (2003). Shoveling Smoke: Advertising and Globalization in Contemporary India. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Google Scholar

McCabe, M. P., and McGreevy, S. J. (2011). Role of media and peers on body change strategies among adult men: is body size important? Eur. Eat. Dis. Rev. 19, 438–446. doi: 10.1002/erv.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellor, D., McCabe, M., Ricciardelli, L., Yeow, J., and Daliza, N. (2009). Sociocultural influences on body image dissatisfaction and body change behaviors among Malaysian adolescents. Body Image 6, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzer, A. L., and McNulty, J. K. (2015). Telling women that men desire women with bodies larger than the thin-ideal improves women’s body satisfaction. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 6, 391–398. doi: 10.1177/1948550614561126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morry, M. M., and Staska, S. L. (2001). Magazine exposure: internalization, self-objectification, eating attitudes, and body satisfaction in male and female university students. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 33:269. doi: 10.1037/h0087148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasser, M., Katzman, M., and Gordon, R. (eds.) (2003). Eating Disorders and Cultures in Transition. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Ogle, R. S., and Batton, C. (2009). Revisiting patriarchy: its conceptualization and operationalization in criminology. Crit. Criminol. 17, 159–182. doi: 10.1007/s10612-009-9081-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oney, C. N., Cole, E. R., and Sellers, R. M. (2011). Racial identity and gender as moderators of the relationship between body image and self-esteem for African Americans. Sex Roles 65:619. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9962-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paludi, M. A. (ed.) (2010). Feminism and Women’s Rights Worldwide, Vol. 1. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Google Scholar

Papp, I., Urbán, R., Czeglédi, E., Babusa, B., and Túry, F. (2013). Testing the tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance among Hungarian adolescents. Body Image 10, 232–242. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxton, S. J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Hannan, P. J., and Eisenberg, M. E. (2006). Body image dissatisfaction prospectively predicts depressive mood and low self-esteem in adolescent girls and boys. J. Clinical Child Adolesc. Psychol. 35, 539–549. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pike, K. M., and Borovoy, A. (2004). The rise of eating disorders in Japan: issues of culture and limitations of the model of “Westernization”. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 28, 493–531. doi: 10.1007/s11013-004-1066-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podrug, N., Pavièiæ, J., and Bratiæ, V. (2006). “Cross-cultural comparison of hofstede’s dimensions and decision-making style within CEE context,” in From Transition to Sustainable Development: The Path to European Integration (Sarajevo: Croatian Scientific Bibliography).

Google Scholar

Pokrajac-Bulian, A. (1998). Nezadovoljstvo tijelom i bulimični simptomi u studentskoj populaciji. Društ. Istraž. Časop. Opća Društ. Pita. 7, 581–601.

Google Scholar

Pokrajac-Bulian, A., Živčić-Bećirević, I., Calugi, S., and Dalle Grave, R. (2006). School prevention program for eating disorders in Croatia: a controlled study with six months of follow-up. Eat. Weight Dis. Stud. Anor. Bulim. Obesity 11, 171–178. doi: 10.1007/bf03327568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramet, S. P., and Matic, D. (2007). Democratic Transition in Croatia: Value Transformation, Education, and Media. College Station, TX: Texas A&M University Press.

Google Scholar

Rathner, G. (2001). “Post-communism and the marketing of the thin ideal,” in Eating Disorders and Cultures in Transitions, eds M. Nasser, M. Katzman, and R. Gordon (New York, NY: Routledge), 93–104.

Google Scholar

Richards, M. H., Boxer, A. W., Petersen, A. C., and Albrecht, R. (1990). Relation of weight to body image in pubertal girls and boys from two communities. Dev. Psychol. 26:313. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R. F., Franko, D. L., Lovering, M. E., Luk, S., Pernal, W., and Matsumoto, A. (2018). Development and validation of the female Muscularity scale. Sex Roles 78, 18–26. doi: 10.1007/s11199-017-0775-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rongmuang, D., Corte, C., McCreary, L. L., Park, C. G., Miller, A., and Gallo, A. (2011). Salience of physical appearance characteristics among young women in Thailand. Body Image 8, 396–403. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rukavina, T., and Pokrajac-Bulian, A. (2006). Thin-ideal internalization, body image dissatisfaction and symptoms of eating disorders in Croatian adolescent girls. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 11, 31–37. doi: 10.1007/bf03327741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanderson, C. A., Darley, J. M., and Messinger, C. S. (2002). “I’m not as thin as you think I am”: the development and consequences of feeling discrepant from the thinness norm. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 172–183. doi: 10.1177/0146167202282004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santana, M. L., Rita de Cássia, R. S., Assis, A. M., Raich, R. M., Machado, M. E. P., Pinto, E. D. J., et al. (2013). Factors associated with body image dissatisfaction among adolescents in public schools students in Salvador, Brazil. Nutr. Hosp. 28, 747–755.

Google Scholar

Schaefer, L. M., Burke, N. L., Thompson, J. K., Dedrick, R. F., Heinberg, L. J., Calogero, R. M., et al. (2015). Development and validation of the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire-4 (SATAQ-4). Psychol. Assess. 27:54.

Google Scholar

Schooler, D. (2008). Real women have curves a longitudinal investigation of TV and the body image development of Latina adolescents. J. Adolesc. Res. 23, 132–153. doi: 10.1177/0743558407310712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. J. Personal. Soc.Psychol. 84:60. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., and Vevea, J. L. (2005). Pancultural self-enhancement reloaded: a meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 539–551. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sen, G., and Östlin, P. (2008). Gender inequity in health: why it exists and how we can change it. Glob. Public Health 3(Suppl. 1), 1–12. doi: 10.1080/17441690801900795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shagar, P. S., Donovan, C. L., Loxton, N., Boddy, J., and Harris, N. (2019). Is thin in everywhere : A cross-cultural comparison of a subsection of tripartite influence model in Australia and Malaysia. Appetite 134, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, G., Tiggemann, M., and Mattiske, J. (2014). The role of media and peer influences in Australian women’s attitudes towards cosmetic surgery. Body Image 11, 482–487. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shroff, H., and Thompson, J. K. (2004). Body image and eating disturbance in India: media and interpersonal influences. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 198–203. doi: 10.1002/eat.10229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shroff, H., and Thompson, J. K. (2006). The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance: a replication with adolescent girls. Body Image 3, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slevec, J. H., and Tiggemann, M. (2011). Predictors of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in middle-aged women. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 515–524. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, X., Anderson, T., Himawan, L., McClintock, A., Jiang, Y., and McCarrick, S. (2019). An investigation of a cultural help-seeking model for professional psychological services with US and Chinese samples. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 50, 1027–1049. doi: 10.1177/0022022119878506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, L. G., and Lynch, S. M. (2013). The pursuit of happiness in China: individualism, collectivism, and subjective well-being during China’s economic and social transformation. Soc. Indic. Res. 114, 441–451. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0154-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., and Agras, W. S. (1998). Predicting onset and cessation of bulimic behaviors during adolescence: a longitudinal grouping analysis. Behav. Ther. 29, 257–276. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7894(98)80006-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 128:825. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strahan, E. J., Wilson, A. E., Cressman, K. E., and Buote, V. M. (2006). Comparing to perfection: how cultural norms for appearance affect social comparisons and self-image. Body Image 3, 211–227. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strudwicke, L. (2000). Sense of Belonging and Self-Esteem: What are the Implications for Educational Outcomes of Secondary School Students : A Literature Review. Ph.D. Thesis, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA.

Google Scholar

Su, H., Ren, X. P., Lu, K. W., and Zhang, H. (2016). The evolution of given names and times change. Youth Stud. 3, 31–38.

Google Scholar

Suisman, J. L., O’Connor, S. M., Sperry, S., Thompson, J. K., Keel, P. K., Burt, S. A., et al. (2012). Genetic and environmental influences on thin−ideal internalization. Int. J. Eat. Dis. 45, 942–948. doi: 10.1002/eat.22056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Tovée, M. J. (2005). Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: a cross-cultural study. Body Image 2, 115–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., Frederick, D. A., Aavik, T., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Anderson, D., et al. (2010). The attractive female body weight and female body image dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: results of the international body project I. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 309–325. doi: 10.1177/0146167209359702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V. (2015). Cultural influences on body size ideals. Eur. Psychol. 1, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Taylor, D., Szpakowska, I., and Swami, V. (2013). Weight discrepancy and body appreciation among women in Poland and Britain. Body Image 10, 628–631. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tewari, N., and Alvarez, A. (2009). Asian American psychology: Current Perspectives. Milton Park: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Thompson, J., and Cafri, G. E. (2007). The Muscular Ideal: Psychological, Social, and Medical Perspectives (pp. x-265). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., Coovert, M. D., and Stormer, S. M. (1999). Body image, social comparison, and eating disturbance: a covariance structure modeling investigation. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 26, 43–51. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199907)26:1<43::aid-eat6>3.0.co;2-r

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, M. A., and Gray, J. J. (1995). Development and validation of a new body-image assessment scale. J. Personal. Assess. 64, 258–269. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6402_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., Schaefer, L., and Menzel, J. (2012). Internalization of Thin-Ideal and Muscular-Ideal. Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance. London: Elsevier, 499.

Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., and Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization: mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 181–183. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, G. L., Warren, S., and Carter, L. (2004). It’s not my fault: predicting high school teachers who blame parents and students for students’ low achievement. High Sch. J. 87, 5–14. doi: 10.1353/hsj.2004.0005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M. (2003). Media exposure, body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating: television and magazines are not the same! Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 11, 418–430. doi: 10.1002/erv.502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., and McGill, B. (2004). The role of social comparison in the effect of magazine advertisements on women’s mood and body image dissatisfaction. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23:23. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.1.23.26991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (2001). Individualism−collectivism and personality. J. Personal. 69, 907–924. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.696169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L. (2011). Refinement of the tripartite influence model for men: dual body image pathways to body change behaviors. Body Image 8, 199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L., and Andorka, M. J. (2012). Support for an expanded tripartite influence model with gay men. Body Image 9, 57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varnum, M. E., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., and Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: the social orientation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 9–13. doi: 10.1177/0963721409359301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, C. S., Gleaves, D. H., Cepeda−Benito, A., Fernandez, M. D. C., and Rodriguez−Ruiz, S. (2005). Ethnicity as a protective factor against internalization of a thin ideal and body dissatisfaction. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 37, 241–249. doi: 10.1002/eat.20102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, J. (2004). China’s cosmetic surgery craze. Lancet 363:958. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(04)15832-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wegner, B. S., Hartmann, A. M., and Geist, C. R. (2000). Effect of exposure to photographs of thin models on self-consciousness in female college students. Psychol. Rep. 86(Pt 2), 1149–1154. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3c.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wertheim, E. H., Koerner, J., and Paxton, S. J. (2001). Longitudinal predictors of restrictive eating and bulimic tendencies in three different age groups of adolescent girls. J. Youth Adolesc. 30, 69–81. doi: 10.1023/a:1005224921891

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wertheim, E. H., Paxton, S. J., and Tilgner, L. (2004). Test–retest reliability and construct validity of contour drawing rating scale scores in a sample of early adolescent girls. Body Image 1, 199–205. doi: 10.1016/s1740-1445(03)00024-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wildes, J. E., Emery, R. E., and Simons, A. D. (2001). The roles of ethnicity and culture in the development of eating disturbance and body image dissatisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 521–551. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00071-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, P. C. (2004). HRM in Taiwan. Managing Human Resources in Asia-Pacific. Hove: Psychology Press, 93–112.

Google Scholar

Yamamiya, Y., Shroff, H., and Thompson, J. K. (2008). The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance: a replication with a Japanese sample. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 88–91. doi: 10.1002/eat.20444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, C. J., and Huang, K. (1996). The collectivistic nature of ethnic identity development among Asian-American college students. Adolescence 31:645.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: culture, body image dissatisfaction, thin-ideal internalization, China, Croatia

Citation: Stojcic I, Dong X and Ren X (2020) Body Image and Sociocultural Predictors of Body Image Dissatisfaction in Croatian and Chinese Women. Front. Psychol. 11:731. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00731

Received: 22 June 2019; Accepted: 25 March 2020;
Published: 06 May 2020.

Edited by:

Antonios Dakanalis, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Amanda Nerini, University of Florence, Italy
Camilla Matera, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Stojcic, Dong and Ren. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaopeng Ren, renxp@psych.ac.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.