Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 18 February 2020
Sec. Psychopathology

Commentary: Screens, Teens, and Psychological Well-Being: Evidence From Three Time-Use-Diary Studies

  • 1Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States
  • 2Rawls College of Business, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, United States
  • 3Stern School of Business, New York University, New York, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, United States

A Commentary on
Screens, Teens, and Psychological Well-Being: Evidence From Three Time-Use-Diary Studies

by Orben, A., and Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Psychol. Sci. 30, 682–696. doi: 10.1177/0956797619830329

Orben and Przybylski (2019); hereafter, O&P make two primary claims: (a) There is no meaningful link between well-being and screen time measured with time diaries, and (b) the correlations between retrospective and time diary reports of screen time are so low that retrospective reports are not useful.

Regarding the first claim, a simple graph of the time diary data from the primary dataset used by O&P (Figure 1A) shows a clear and substantial relationship between depression and social media, especially for girls, with twice as many heavy (vs. light) users depressed, consistent with other studies (Haidt and Twenge, 2020). Yet O&P made six decisions that substantially reduced the size of the relationship. They chose to:

1. Rely solely on linear r. Linear r obscures the curvilinear associations typically found in this area (e.g., the Goldilocks Hypothesis, Przybylski and Weinstein, 2017). For example, the Growing Up in Ireland (GUI) weekend data shows a pronounced curvilinear effect (see Figure 1B). This dataset also shows a substantial association, with nearly twice as many heavy users (12.0%) vs. light users (6.4%) on weekdays having adjustment problems. These effects will appear to be small or null when presented only as linear correlations.

2. Include measures of mere participation. Eighty percent of the variables in O&P's analysis measure mere participation in an activity rather than amount of time spent. Research, including their own, has repeatedly shown that light users (not non-users) of digital media have the highest well-being (Przybylski and Weinstein, 2017). Thus, participation in the activity is not useful for predicting well-being. The relevant issue is heavy use.

3. Consider screen time monolithically. O&P's analysis combines activities more likely to be problematic (e.g., social media) with activities less likely to be problematic (talking on the phone, TV) into a monolithic measure of “screen time.” This mutes practically significant associations with well-being for (e.g.,) social media amid the noise of small or null associations for other activities. Given the wide variety of screen activities (McFarland and Ployhart, 2015), it seems clear that not all screen time is created equal.

4. Analyze boys and girls together. The mental health crisis during the smartphone era has been much more pronounced among girls (e.g., Mercado et al., 2017; Spiller et al., 2019). Thus, it is important to determine if associations between digital media use and well-being are stronger for girls than for boys. That is indeed the case in the time-diary data (see Figure 1A). Without separating the data by gender, the stronger associations for girls are obscured amid the weaker effects for boys.

5. Include control variables that are mediators or collider variables. O&P control for factors including negative attitudes toward school, time spent with parents, parent distress, and closeness to parents that are possible mediators or collider variables. For example, as a collider, parent distress could be caused by the child's heavy screen time or by the child's low well-being. As a mediator, the child's heavy screen time may cause parent distress, which may cause children's low well-being. Best practices guidelines in both psychology and epidemiology state that mediator or collider variables should not be used as controls in correlational analyses: “A solid rule of thumb is that researchers should not control for such posttreatment variables … If mediating variables are controlled for, the very processes of interest are controlled away” (Rohrer, 2018, p. 34, 37; see also Schisterman et al., 2009). In addition, negative attitudes toward school includes well-being items such as “How often do you feel unhappy at school?” In effect, this controls for well-being when well-being is the outcome variable.

6. Exclude relevant measures of well-being. O&P excluded the prosocial behavior subscale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) with no explanation. Low prosocial behavior was nearly twice as common among heavy gamers (28.7%) vs. light (14.5%). Also without explanation, O&P did not include the Piers-Harris self-esteem items in GUI even though they included self-esteem items from the two other datasets. In GUI, heavy gamers were 73% more likely to have low self-esteem than light users.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Percent low in well-being by levels of screen time. (A) Percent of girls and boys with clinically relevant levels of depressive symptoms by retrospectively reported or time diary hours spent on social media sites, Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). Controlled by age, ethnicity, family income, parent education, parent employment, number of siblings, father present, longstanding illness, and parent word score. Boys reporting 5+h in time diaries were only n = 8 and are thus excluded. (B) Percent with problematic adjustment on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire by hours per weekday of total screen time in time diaries, Growing Up in Ireland (GUI). Controlled by gender and age.

Turning to the second claim, O&P find low correlations between retrospective report measures and time diary measures of screen time. However, these measures do not ask about the same activities and in one case do not use the same time scale. The GUI retrospective items examine computer use, gaming, and TV/videos, but the time diaries examine internet use, talking on the phone or texting, gaming (with different wording), and TV/videos. Thus, half of the time diary items in the GUI have no direct equivalent in the retrospective items. Even more striking, the retrospective reports in the U.S. data use a scale of “never” to “every day” while the time diaries use minutes per day, also with two different lists of activities.

When similar items are compared, correlations are much higher. In GUI, the retrospective and time diary accounts of gaming on weekdays correlate r = 0.33, nearly twice as large as the r = 0.18 reported in O&P for all measures combined. This is larger than correlations between self-reports of usual behavior (personality) and multiple-day time diary accounts (largest r = 0.25: Rohrer and Lucas, 2018). In fact, the GUI correlation is remarkably strong given that the retrospective accounts ask about a typical weekday while the time diary includes just one weekday, which lowers its predictive value via greater error variance (Iida et al., 2012).

In addition, if retrospective and time diary measures are as different as O&P claim they are, they should show very different associations with well-being. However, that is not the case—the effects are instead very similar (see Figure 1A). Thus, O&P's conclusion that retrospective reports are not useful is not supported by these data.

In conclusion, there is in fact a relationship between adverse mental health outcomes and heavy use (not light use) of some forms of screen-based activity (more than “screen time” in general) for girls (more than for boys), and these relationships appear in both retrospective and time diary reports.

Author Contributions

JT wrote the first draft of the manuscript and performed analyses. AB obtained and organized the dataset and provided crucial revisions. JH and WC wrote sections of the manuscript and provided crucial revisions.

Conflict of Interest

JT, JH, and WC have received speaking honoraria from non-profit and for-profit entities for presenting their research, but have not done paid speaking or consulting for any company that would have a particular interest in the links between social media and mental health.

The remaining author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Haidt, J., and Twenge, J. M. (2020). Social Media Use and Mental Health: A Review. Unpublished ongoing literature review, in a public Google document. Available online at: https://www.thecoddling.com/better-mental-health

Iida, M., Shrout, P. E., Laurenceau, J.-P., and Bolger, N. (2012). “Using diary methods in psychological research,” in APA Handbook of Research Methods in Psychology, Vol 1, eds H. Cooper (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 277–305. doi: 10.1037/13619-016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McFarland, L. A., and Ployhart, R. E. (2015). Social media: a contextual framework to guide research and practice. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 1653–1677. doi: 10.1037/a0039244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercado, M. C., Holland, K., and Leemis, R. W. (2017). Trends in emergency department visits for nonfatal self-inflicted injuries among youth aged 10 to 24 years in the United States, 2001-2015. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 318, 1931–1933. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.13317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orben, A., and Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Screens, teens, and psychological well-being: evidence from three time-use-diary studies. Psychol. Sci. 30, 682–696. doi: 10.1177/0956797619830329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A. K., and Weinstein, N. (2017). A large-scale test of the Goldilocks hypothesis: quantifying the relations between digital-screen use and the mental well-being of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 28, 204–215. doi: 10.1177/0956797616678438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohrer, J. M. (2018). Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: graphical causal models for observational data. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 1, 27–42. doi: 10.1177/2515245917745629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohrer, J. M., and Lucas, R. E. (2018). Only so many hours: correlations between personality and daily time use in a representative German panel. Collabra Psychol. 4, 1. doi: 10.1525/collabra.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schisterman, E. F., Cole, S. R., and Platt, R. W. (2009). Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 20, 488–495. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181a819a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiller, H. A., Ackerman, J. P., Spiller, N. E., and Casavant, M. J. (2019). Sex- and age-specific increases in suicide attempts by self-poisoning in the United States among youth and young adults from 2000 to 2018. J. Pediatr. 210, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.02.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: digital media, psychological well-being, time use, depressive symptoms, time-diary data

Citation: Twenge JM, Blake AB, Haidt J and Campbell WK (2020) Commentary: Screens, Teens, and Psychological Well-Being: Evidence From Three Time-Use-Diary Studies. Front. Psychol. 11:181. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00181

Received: 25 October 2019; Accepted: 27 January 2020;
Published: 18 February 2020.

Edited by:

Gabriella Martino, University of Messina, Italy

Reviewed by:

Serge Brand, University Psychiatric Clinic Basel, Switzerland

Copyright © 2020 Twenge, Blake, Haidt and Campbell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jean M. Twenge, jtwenge@mail.sdsu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.