Skip to main content

FOCUSED REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 04 December 2018
Sec. Decision Neuroscience

Developmental Coordination Disorder: The Importance of Grounded Assessments and Interventions

  • 1Center for Research and Development, Evidens University College, Göteborg, Sweden
  • 2Center for Sensorimotor Research, Vestibularis Clinic, Kalmar, Sweden
  • 3Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, Institute of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Göteborg, Sweden

This focused review is based on earlier studies which have shown that both children and adults diagnosed as having developmental coordination disorder (DCD), benefited from sensorimotor therapy according to the method Retraining for Balance (RB). Different approaches and assessments for children and adults in regard to DCD are scrutinized and discussed in comparison to RB which mainly includes (a) vestibular assessment and stimulation (b) assessment and integration of aberrant primary reflexes and (c) assessment and stimulation of auditory and visual perception. Earlier results indicate that the process of Sensorimotor therapy using RB techniques could be described according to a conceptual Kinesthetic-Vestibular Developmental Model (KVDM) whereby the training elicited temporary physical and psychological regressions followed by transformations i.e., positive physical and psychological development. We have also seen that this recurring pattern is similar for children and adults. In our conceptual model vestibular stimulation (perceptual priming) stimulates the nervous system, which might enhance object-related priming. This perceptual priming will also assist the suppression of persistent aberrant primary reflexes. In order to develop effective methods for assessment and intervention of DCD over the life span the importance of primary reflex inhibition and vestibular stimulation as well as a combination of bottom-up and top-down approaches have to be considered.

Introduction

This Focused review aim at not only following-up our recent empirical study (Niklasson et al., 2015) but also at filling a gap in the literature concerning assessments and interventions of developmental coordination disorder (DCD). So far a grounded approach i.e., an approach, which has its starting point in fundamental neurological development including aberrant primary reflex assessment and inhibition as well as vestibular assessment and stimulation has been missing. We aim at filling that gap.

KEY CONCEPT 1. Developmental coordination disorder (DCD)
Characterized by a delayed and immature gross and fine motor development without obvious intellectual or medical causes, DCD is defined as a neurodevelopmental motor disorder.

KEY CONCEPT 2. Primary reflexes
Primary reflexes are movement patterns, which are complex, stereotyped and automatic. They are present at birth but are supposed to be integrated as the nervous system matures during the first year of life.

Our recent study (Niklasson et al., 2015) confirmed that motor problems don't disappear with age and showed that it was possible to use both the same diagnostic instrument and treatment method for children, adolescents and adults diagnosed with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). The study also concluded that sensorimotor problems in childhood should be taken seriously. The main purpose of this Focused review is to put our recent study in a wider perspective. We also intend to give a more thorough description of both the method Retraining for Balance (RB) and Sensorimotor therapy (SMT).

KEY CONCEPT 3. Sensorimotor therapy (SMT)
SMT is a process-oriented approach to sensorimotor training which emphasize the interdependence between primary reflex suppression and vestibular stimulation.

We start with a short presentation describing the present status of DCD followed by a description of three approaches to intervention. There after we discuss frequently used assessment instruments aimed for children and for adults. Finally a presentation of RB will follow and we close by discussing possible advantages for the use SMT for both younger and older children as well as for adults.

The Present Status of DCD

So far no “gold standards” for assessments and interventions of DCD have been established although several approaches have been used (Dewey et al., 2011; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2012). Neither are there any instruments for assessment of DCD to be used over the lifespan, from childhood to adulthood (Kirby and Sugden, 2007). DCD is still recognized as a “hidden problem” (Caçola, 2016) and “currently among the most neglected problems in the whole field of developmental medicine/child neuropsychiatry” (Gillberg, 2017).

During the last century children with clumsy movements and motor coordination difficulties have been described and often differently labeled in both medical and psychological literature (Ahonen et al., 2004; Tupper and Sondell, 2004), but since 1994 the construct of Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) (American Psychiatric Association., 1994, 2013) has been preferred. Although motor problems have been well known and described for long and despite an estimated prevalence of between 6 and 13% among all school-aged children (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2012) no consensus regarding symptoms, etiology (Gomez and Sirigu, 2015; Vaivre-Douret et al., 2016), terminology and concepts (Peters et al., 2001; Wilson, 2004; Gibbs et al., 2007) have been reached. There are only recommendations (Blank et al., 2012).

With extended knowledge about DCD and its persistence throughout life as well as knowledge about its different co-morbidities (Rasmussen and Gillberg, 2000), research regarding detection (Mahoney et al., 2004), assessment instruments (Schoemaker and Wilson, 2015), interventions in early childhood (Wilson, 2004), and in schools (Norlander et al., 2005) ought to be priority not the least due to a high risk of additional problems such as depression and anxiety (Caçola, 2016). Another problem tending to remain into adulthood (Tal-Saban et al., 2012; Purcell et al., 2015) is a sedentary or physical inactive behavior. In a qualitative study, one of us used a phenomenological perspective (Bergman and Norlander, 2005) in order to understand participant's physical inactivity. It was concluded that the resistance to physical activity might be due to an “unidentified psychological barrier.” Participant's had a desire to leave their inactive life but seemed to be stuck in an unbreakable vicious circle. This leaves us with the question of which approaches would be most suitable for intervention.

Approaches

Presently, approaches to DCD interventions belong mainly to either deficit-/process-oriented approaches (bottom-up) or to functional skills/task-oriented approaches (top-down, dynamic systems theory). Although these approaches have their weaknesses and strengths it would be useful to combine them when it comes to pediatric motor skills assessment (Kennedy et al., 2013).

Bottom-up

A deficit- or process-oriented approach to intervention (Wilson, 2004; Sugden, 2007; Blank et al., 2012; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2012) also known as the General Abilities Approach (Pless and Carlsson, 2000) is based on maturational, neurodevelopmental theory and on the assumption that underlying sensorimotor processes need to be remedied and improvements lead to a better performance of motor skills. An example of a bottom-up approach is Sensory Integration therapy (SIT) (Ayers, 1972). SIT uses sensory stimulation in order to enhance motor development and cognitive abilities but does not include integration of primary reflexes in order to develop motor milestones such as rolling, belly crawling and crawling on hands and knees (Niklasson, 2013a; Lin et al., 2014).

Top-down

A functional skills/task-oriented approach to intervention (Pless and Carlsson, 2000; Wilson, 2004; Thelen and Smith, 2006; Sugden, 2007; Blank et al., 2012; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2012) is a top-down approach, which implies that the child or adult is taught how to handle a task, a special skill or a strategy in its environmental context. The most promising and also the most recommended method is Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance (CO-OP). Coming from the field of occupational therapy, CO-OP is mainly focused on cognitive strategies for the acquisition of age-specific functional skills.

Dynamical Systems Theory

Among skills/task-oriented approaches a perspective on motor development provided by the framework of dynamical systems theory (DST) has gained much attention and has been influential for developmental psychology research for more than 25 years, mainly due to its connection to physical science (Hollenstein, 2011). Originated in mathematics DST emphasizes i.e., “changes over time,” “emergence,” “non-linear,” and “self-organization” (Thelen and Smith, 2006). Using the framework of DST a dynamic systems perspective on motor development (Goldfield and Wolff, 2004) holds that motor behavior is an emerging result of the interaction between different subsystems. Without any special entity directing it, the system is self-organizing with changes appearing to be non-linear. An example could be the transition from crawling to walking. According to the dynamic systems perspective this transition is due to a dynamic interaction between the child's ability, the context of the environment and the task in question. An important force for a developmental change is the child's motivation to explore (Thelen, 2000; Thelen and Smith, 2006). According to the dynamic perspective the nervous system is a part of an embodied system with the brain acting as a medium “informationally coupled” to the environment yielding patterns for control and coordination mainly through visual information (Goldfield and Wolff, 2004). Shumway-Cook and Woollacott (1995) meant that one limitation of this perspective could be the anticipation that the nervous system is supposed to play a rather unimportant role.

From our point of view this remark is well founded since our studies (Niklasson et al., 2009, 2015) have indicated that the persistence of aberrant primary reflexes together with a vestibular dysfunction are hindrances for the development of motor milestones. Therefore, we tentatively suggest that motivation is not enough. The child must primarily be able to handle gravity (Adolph and Franchak, 2017) in order to develop a dynamic interaction between perception and movement.

Assessments

Approaches to DCD interventions such as those described above are strongly dependent on high quality instruments in order to monitor complex training processes.

According to recent recommendations from the European Academy of Childhood Disability (Blank et al., 2012) a DCD diagnosis not only requires standardized, valid and reliable motor tests and questionnaires but also a clinical examination and history taking. There are several instruments available for the assessment of DCD in children but they are scarce regarding the diagnosis of adults and so far absent (Kirby and Sugden, 2007) concerning instruments to be used over the lifespan, from childhood to adulthood. However, a recent study by Sigmundsson et al. (2016), which evaluated a test battery aimed at determine fine- and gross motor competence across the life span showed promising results.

Instruments Aimed for Children

Sensory and Integration Praxis Tests (SIPT)

Sensory and Integration Praxis Tests (SIPT) Ayers (1989); Mailloux (1990). SIPT is standardized for children aged between 4 years and 11 years 8 months and has 17 subtests divided into four parts: (1) tactile processing and discrimination, (2) vestibular and proprioceptive processing, (3) bilateral integration, sequencing, and praxis and finally (4) perception of shape, space and visuo-motor coordination. The test score reliability is high when it comes to determining sensory motor integration disorder. However, additional information such as case history and clinical observations needs to be added for reliable interpretations of scores (Jorquera-Cabrera et al., 2017). According to Wilson (2004) the test provides limited information about how the perceptual-motor system works and the validity is low. SIPT has not been revised since 1989, which might be one limitation and another could be the 2 h estimated for administration (Jorquera-Cabrera et al., 2017).

Movement ABC-2

Movement Assessment Battery for Children-second edition (MABC-2) (Henderson et al., 2007) is standardized for children aged 3 to 16 years and probably one of the most commonly used and recommended tests for assessment of motor impairment. MABC-2 is designed to provide a general index and includes a performance part and a checklist. The performance part, which takes 20–40 min to administer, is descriptive and product oriented and mainly aimed at evaluating movements at the functional level (Wilson, 2004). This part covers tests for ball skills, manual dexterity and static and dynamic balance. However, as stressed by Barnett (2014) it is not a “test of DCD,” rather a general motor test used to detect motor difficulties. The checklist is a validated questionnaire (Wilson et al., 2009; Blank et al., 2012) to be completed by a parent or by another adult, familiar with the child's general motor function. For a proper evaluation it is recommended that both parts of the test are used.

BOT−2

Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Profiency-2 (BOT-2) (Bruininks and Bruininks, 2005) is another commonly used and recommended test. BOT-2 is standardized for children and adolescents aged 4 to 21 years 11 months and the complete test takes 45–60 min to perform. Like Movement ABC-2 the test is not aimed for diagnostic purposes. It is rather a motor test designed to detect motor difficulties (Barnett, 2014). The test, which generates a general motor ability factor is divided into 8 sections: Fine motor precision, Fine motor integration, Manual dexterity, Upper-limb coordination, Bilateral coordination, Balance, Running speed/agility, and Strength.

It was also mentioned (Kaplan et al., 1998) that while MABC allows instructions only ahead of performance and BOT allows for coaching during the test, MABC might penalize children with difficulties to remember. Furthermore, neither of the tests measures the quality of movement completely.

DCDQ′07

The Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire 2007 (DCDQ′07) (Wilson et al., 1998, 2009; Wilson and Crawford, 2012) is a questionnaire for parents aiming at identifying subtle motor problems in children aged 8 to 14 years 6 months. The questionnaire consists of 15 items divided into three factors: Control during movement, Fine motor and handwriting, and General coordination. Although the DCDQ'07 is a reliable and valid instrument (Montoro et al., 2016) it cannot by itself be used to identify DCD (Pannekoek et al., 2012). A recent study by Montoro et al. (2016) indicated concurrent validity between DCDQ and MABC-2 suggesting the use of MABC-2 as an indicator for DCD.

Retraining for Balance

Retraining for Balance (RB) (Niklasson and Niklasson, 2007a,b; Niklasson et al., 2007) is an umbrella term for both assessments and interventions used in sensorimotor therapy (SMT). The method mainly includes (a) vestibular assessment and stimulation (b) assessment and integration of aberrant primary reflexes and (c) assessment and stimulation of auditory perception. A more thorough description will follow below.

Instruments Aimed for Adults

In accordance with substantial research (e.g., Rasmussen and Gillberg, 2000), which has shown that individuals do not grow out of motor problems, DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association., 2013; Purcell et al., 2015) recognizes DCD as persistent throughout the life span. However, standardized tests for adults are lacking. Sometimes MABC-2 and BOT-2 are used but their psychometric properties when used to assess adults are still unclear (Barnett, 2014). So far, a self-rating questionnaire such as the Adult Developmental Co-ordination Disorders/Dyspraxia Checklist (ADC) (Kirby et al., 2010) is most commonly used. ADC is a comprehensive instrument, which was developed and tested on individuals in the age range of 17 to 42 years. It consists of 40 items, 10 rates childhood performance while 30 rates current adult performance. The items cover both motor behavior, for example movement skills and handwriting, and behavior beyond the motor capacity such as social skills and general organization. Despite the growing awareness and knowledge about DCD in adults during recent years, evidence based research is still scarce (Missiuna et al., 2012; Tal-Saban et al., 2012) and the need to develop appropriate assessment tools for adults is urgent (Barnett, 2014).

Our recent study (Niklasson et al., 2015), which used the method Retraining for Balance in a group of adults, indicated promising results. The method follows hierarchical principles (Wiest, 2012) and includes both assessment tools and guidelines for intervention regarding persistent primary reflexes and vestibular dysfunction. The main logic behind RB is that un-integrated primary reflexes together with vestibular dysfunction cause an arrested sensorimotor development, which will be released only through a proper and methodical suppression of the reflexes. This is why the method can be used and also apparently works in all age groups. There is a strong connection between certain primary reflexes and the vestibular system. One example of special interest is the Moro reflex, which is expected to be gradually suppressed during the first 6 months of life. Nonetheless it was frequently found among participants in both the child- and the adult-group in our study (Niklasson et al., 2015). Another study by Konicarova and Bob (2012) showed that a persisting Moro reflex was closely linked to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in school children aged 8 to 11 years.

KEY CONCEPT 4. The vestibular system
The vestibular system is the organ for detection of gravity and for movement. A balance system, which regulates i.e., eye-movements and postural reflexes.

In the newborn the Moro reflex is unconditioned and released through a sharp alteration of the infant's position in space. The first form of anxiety is then the fear of falling, closely connected to the excitement of the vestibular part of the eighth cranial nerve. The other part of this cranial nerve, which is connected to the auditory system, is stimulated by sudden loud noises. Since the newborn is rather insensitive to noise the reaction and fear for loud and/or sudden noises, a Startle reflex develops somewhat later and is conditioned. The common denominator for both anxiety and fear is an irritation of at least one of the branches of the eighth cranial nerve (Feldenkrais, 1988). Although it has been argued that The Moro reflex and the Startle reflex are different entities (e.g., Rousseau et al., 2017) they are thought to be part of the same developmental chain (Goddard Blythe, 2014) and therefore we find it likely that they are two sides of the same coin. In a recent study, Poli and Angrilli (2015) demonstrated that a strong Startle reflex was associated with higher anxiety levels. We speculate that retention of the Moro reflex and a too easily elicited Startle reflex among both adolescents and adults with sensorimotor problems and/or anxiety is more common than expected. Therefore, we suggest that the hitherto neglected roles of persistent primary reflexes and of vestibular dysfunction ought to be parts of future DCD diagnosing. Retraining for Balance constitute such a diagnosing instrument.

The Framework of Retraining for Balance

Theory

Through a combination of theoretical knowledge (e.g., Ayers, 1972; Thelen, 2000) and clinical practice (e.g., Blythe, 2009) we came to understand the importance of mixing the inhibition of aberrant primary reflexes with vestibular stimulation and thereby creating assessments and interventions grounded in fundamental neurological development. In a previous naturalistic study (Niklasson et al., 2009), comprised of 232 children and adolescents diagnosed with sensorimotor disorder, results showed that the integration of persisting primary reflexes together with vestibular stimulation according to the method Retraining for Balance (RB) enhanced sensorimotor development. At the time we could not find any reports on equal interventions with adults. Therefore, we decided to study a group of adults who had voluntary signed up to be assessed and treated for SMD and compare their results with a group of older children with an average age of 12 years (Niklasson et al., 2015).

KEY CONCEPT 5. Sensorimotor disorder (SMD)
SMD is a tentatively suggested complementary definition of the existing Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) diagnose. The difference is primarily the stressing of sensory causes such as vestibular impairment but also the importance of assessment and integration of remaining primary reflexes.

Training according to RB (Niklasson et al., 2007) is a process, which takes on average 3 years. During that time participants are doing exercises for about 15 min/day at home together with either their parents or their spouse. The method is a blend of five different approaches and the perspective includes the following notions:

(a) The deficit- or process-oriented approach (Wilson, 2004; Sugden, 2007; Blank et al., 2012; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2012) or the General Abilities Approach (GA) (Pless and Carlsson, 2000; Niklasson et al., 2009), which implies that deficits in underlying neurological structures needs to be remediated. A logic of RB as well as of GA is that persistent primary reflexes will become a hindrance for development of postural reactions and of gross motor milestones such as rolling, belly-crawling and creeping on hands and knees which in turn will affect the development of more complex motor skills.

(b) RB is also partly a Sensory Integration Approach (SI) (Pless and Carlsson, 2000; Niklasson et al., 2009) since the assumption that the development of motor skills is dependent on the individual's sensory integration ability. Another logic of RB is that vestibular stimulation plays a key role when it comes to the suppression of primary reflexes. During infancy the vestibular system is very responsive, reaching a peak between 6 and 12 months (e.g., Piontelli, 2015) a period which coincide with the development of belly-crawling, creeping on hands and knees and the child learning to walk (Capute and Accardo, 1991).

(c) A dynamic systems perspective on motor development stipulates that novel motor behavior emerge as a result of interactions between different subsystems. The nervous system is viewed as self-organizing and the process is arranged by the components themselves without any outside influence (e.g., Goldfield and Wolff, 2004). The terms emergence and self-organizing are in line with how we conceptualize the process of RB when primary reflexes are suppressed and novel gross motor patterns such as crawling and creeping on hands and knees emerge. However, we suggest that there is an outside agent, the ever-present gravitational force, which as the driving force, acts on the nervous system via the vestibular system (Hydén, 1961; Niklasson, 2012, 2013a; Adolph and Franchak, 2017). The process of RB is a “changing over time” and according to (van Geert, 2011), “a dynamic system is a way to explain how the ‘next’ state of the system comes about as a result of its ‘preceding’ state.”

(d) Bjorklund and Ellis (2014) argued that an evolutionary approach is of importance within the field of developmental psychology. Motor development organization obviously follows hierarchical principles where lower and automatic levels are inhibited by higher motor systems (Wiest, 2012) and the integrated layers of the brain are in accordance with hierarchical theory (e.g., MacLean, 1990) i.e., our evolutionary history (Wiest, 2012; Kirmayer and Crafa, 2014). MacLean's model on the evolution of the brain is also of importance for the understanding of emotions (LeDoux, 2004). In this way a hierarchical and neurodevelopmental concept has become a natural approach to sensorimotor training according to the method Retraining for Balance.

(e) In a previous qualitative study (Niklasson et al., 2010) the analysis indicated a strong connection between physiological exercises and psychological development through regressions and transformations. This was described through the conceptual Kinesthetic Vestibular Developmental Model (KVDM) (see below) which equals Piaget (1953) who wrote, “The physiology of the organism furnishes a heredity mechanism which is already completely organized and virtually adapted but has never functioned. Psychology begins with the use of this mechanism.” By “heredity mechanism” Piaget meant the infant's innate motor reflexes i.e., primary reflexes and further that in parallel with these reflexes fulfilling their aims, different psychological phenomena emerge (Cramer, 2006). A fundamental rationale of RB is that the developmental process could have been arrested but can be released through treatment (Gedo and Goldberg, 1973).

Anamneses

In our recent study (Niklasson et al., 2015), prior to their first visit, the participants were interviewed on telephone by one of two therapists. After the conversation a decision was made as whether or not the individual should come for assessment. This verbal history taking is similar to the Adult Developmental Co-ordination Disorders/Dyspraxia Checklist (ADC) (Kirby et al., 2010) and to the Movement ABC-2 checklist or the revised version of the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCD-Q-R) (Wilson et al., 2009) for children. Regarding the children, Conners' Teacher Rating Scale (Conners, 1990; Janols and von Knorring, 1991; Niklasson et al., 2009) and Conners' Parent Symptom Questionnaire (Conners, 1973; Niklasson et al., 2009) were used before and after intervention. Adult participants, as well as the children's parents, were also asked to complete the questionnaire Reasons for training (Bergström et al., 1999) in order to indicate additional problems. Besides sensorimotor difficulties both groups reported attention problems, generalized anxiety, reading problems and sensitivity to stress. The similarity between the groups might be an indication that comorbidities do not wane with age and hints toward the importance of an early identification of sensorimotor problems in order to minimize additional problems later in life (Missiuna et al., 2015).

Assessments

In contrast to the instruments Movement ABC-2 (Henderson et al., 2007) and Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Profiency-2 (Bruininks and Bruininks, 2005), which mainly assess the child's abilities on the functional level, the method Retraining for Balance uses instruments, which primarily evaluate motor patterns below the functional level (Niklasson and Niklasson, 2007a,b). The RB perspective is partly an answer to Wann (2007) who rhetorically wrote that although DCD is a heterogeneous diagnosis “there must be common perceptuomotor subsystems that are poorly developed and refined.” Previously, Sigmundsson (2003) stressed the importance of finding links or fundamental mechanisms between behavior and basic neural processing and argued for a process-oriented approach. We are well aware of the complexity of diagnosing developmental disorders (e.g., Dewey and Bottos, 2004; Flouris et al., 2005; Pennington, 2009; Zwicker et al., 2009) but when it comes to the visibility of sensory and motor difficulties and DCD one logic of RB has been that the lowest common denominator is the interaction between the vestibular system and primary reflexes (Niklasson et al., 2009, 2015). Both vestibular function and primary reflexes belong under the concept neurological soft signs (Ayd, 2000; American Psychiatric Association., 2013).

In our previous study (Niklasson et al., 2015) the concept sensorimotor disorder was tentatively suggested to label a diagnosis of DCD plus vestibular problems. Vestibular dysfunction has so far been overlooked within the framework of DCD but our studies (Niklasson et al., 2009, 2010, 2015) indicate the need for reconsideration. Recently the importance of a well-functioning vestibular system has been stressed in quite a few articles, connecting it to mental health (e.g., Gurvich et al., 2013), cognitive development (e.g., Weiner-Vacher et al., 2013) and a recent editorial (Besnard et al., 2015) declared that “The sky's the limit for new ideas and developments in vestibular therapy (both pharmacological and physical devises).”

The instruments used by the method Retraining for Balance are as follows:

RB-P. Retraining for Balance–Physiological Test (RB-P) (Niklasson and Niklasson, 2007a; Niklasson et al., 2015) is a battery of 41 different subtests aiming at detecting aberrant primary reflexes (e.g., Capute et al., 1978; Blythe, 2009; Goddard Blythe, 2009) and determine the relation between primary reflexes, postural reactions, gross motor milestones, and sports-related gross motor skills (Capute and Accardo, 1991). Of special interest are primary reflexes (McPhillips et al., 2000; Zafeiriou, 2004) connected to the vestibular system i.e., the asymmetrical tonic neck reflex, the tonic labyrinthine reflex, the symmetrical tonic neck reflex and the Moro reflex. The primary reflexes, except the symmetrical tonic neck and the Moro reflexes are assessed in prone, supine, table position and standing position. There are different approaches to how to examine and evaluate neurological soft signs including primary reflexes in children (e.g., Touwen and Prechtl, 1970; Prechtl, 1977; Touwen, 1979; Vaivre-Douret et al., 2016) and adults (e.g., Rodnitzky, 1988). However, to our knowledge only RB uses vestibular related primary reflexes, postural reactions and gross motor skills in combination. A prerequisite for locomotion is the development of posture, which in turn is dependent on the integration of primary reflexes (Zafeiriou, 2004). During the first year of life, starting with the lifting of the head while in prone position the child is normally expected to defy the gravitational force via rolling, belly crawling, creeping on hands and knees and then further to the ability to walk. This developmental succession is of importance for RB. As shown in our previous study (Niklasson et al., 2015) both the younger group and the group of adults diverged from expected ability levels at first assessment. Of special interest is the ability to belly crawl, which we hold as an important transition between activities in prone position and the four feet position. Our experience is in line with both Adolph (2008) who wrote that only about 50% of the infants who later will creep on hands and knees also belly crawl and with Holt (1991) who showed that a skipping of belly crawl might lead to bottom shuffling. Not only will bottom shufflers walk later, they will also miss out valuable time in 4 feet position which should enable them to develop increasing strength in fingers, hands, neck and trunk and the ability to develop a cross pattern. A well-developed cross pattern is a prerequisite for the acquisition of sports-related gross motor skills. Our recent study (Niklasson et al., 2015) showed that even these skills were below expected levels in both groups before intervention.

RB-OB. Retraining for Balance-Orientation and Balance test (RB-OB) (Niklasson and Niklasson, 2007b; Niklasson et al., 2009, 2015) is a group of tests aimed at detecting vestibular dysfunction and balance problems. Of special interest is the participants' reaction while being slowly rotated in a chair (e.g., Guyton, 1991; Barnett-Cowan, 2013) especially since it is common that children with sensorimotor problems prefer to move quickly.

RB-A. Retraining for Balance-Audiometric test (RB-A) (Niklasson et al., 2009, 2015) is an auditory perceptual test aimed at detecting discrepancies between right- and left-ear dominance. A clinical diagnostic audiometer was used to measure ear preference according to a scale spanning from 0 to 200. Values below 100 indicated left ear dominance and values above 100 indicated right ear dominance. Speech sounds are supposed to be more rapidly processed through the right ear (Sininger and Cone-Wesson, 2004), which stresses the importance of a well-developed right-ear advantage (REA) early in life.

KVST. Keystone Visual Skills test (Burman, 1977) is a visual skills test relating to vestibular function. The test has 14 subtests and uses 15 cards, which measures eye coordination, simultaneous perception, and effective acuity during resting accommodation at different distances as well as stereovision.

Discussion

Our results have shown (Niklasson et al., 2010, 2015) that sensorimotor therapy (SMT) using primary reflex suppression and vestibular stimulation according to the method Retraining for Balance can be described in terms of a classical developmental curve containing plateaus, regressions (negative development), and transformations (positive development). It was through a qualitative analysis (Niklasson et al., 2010) with 8 children that the process, a change over time, became possible to describe through a conceptual Kinesthetic-Vestibular Developmental Model (KVDM) (Figure 1) thereby showing how the training (I) elicited temporary physical and psychological regressions (R) followed by transformations (T). The KVDM was validated in the same study through a comparison between the 8 children and 224 other children who had undergone the same treatment. In our recent study (Niklasson et al., 2015) the analysis showed no significant difference between children (mean age about 12 years) and adults (mean age about 35 years) regarding degree of alignment to the conceptual model. Neither were there any significant differences between the 2 age groups and a reference group compiled of 398 treated children (aged 4–17). Although children and adults showed the same patterns concerning periods of regressions and transformations, the regressions were often stronger among the younger participants.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The participants needed close to 16 visits on average in order to complete the treatment in accordance with the sensorimotor therapy program Retraining of Balance. The visits (the introductions) are distributed along the curve above (I1–I16) and indicated as dots, During the treatment period three regression periods, here illustrated as circles (R1–R3), and four periods of transformation (T1–T4) were identified. (Reprinted from Social Behavior personality by kind permission. Originally Published 2010, 38(3), 335).

The concept regression connotes a return to a hierarchically and developmentally lower level of psychological and/or physiological function (Wiest, 2012). We tentatively propose that a re-activation and integration of arrested primary reflexes together with vestibular stimulation causes regressions, which in turn are prerequisites for the release and emergence of both physiological and psychological transformations. We also speculate that the “unidentified psychological barrier” (Bergman and Norlander, 2005) mentioned above might be a result of vestibular dysfunction known to affect both emotions and body balance (Rajagopalan et al., 2017). Our results have indicated that sensorimotor problems in early life do affect not only physiological- but also psychological development. Thereby is the need for an early detection highlighted. One of us (Gillberg and Rasmussen, 2003) has previously argued for the urgent need of physical, neurological and neurodevelopmental examination of all children with learning and behavioral problems. As for now we find it appropriate to include children who show a sedentary behavior. Our results also indicated for the first time, as far as we know, that sensorimotor problems can be treated within all age groups.

The distinction between bottom-up and top-down approaches is too general and in need of revision (Berthoz and Petit, 2006). One of us has previously proposed (Niklasson, 2013b) an extension of the dynamical systems perspective on motor development (Goldfield and Wolff, 2004) through the inclusion of the vestibular system as a priming factor. Priming in the sense that repeated stimulus improves processing (Grill-Spector et al., 2006). Since priming can be viewed both as a basic perceptual process (bottom-up) and as an object-related process (top-down) it operates along the ascending as well as the descending loops in the nervous system (Rauss and Pourtois, 2013). We suggest, tentatively, that this is how sensorimotor therapy (SMT) (Niklasson et al., 2009, 2015) appears to work. In our conceptual model vestibular stimulation (perceptual priming) “ignites”/“nourish” the nervous system, assist the suppression of persistent primary reflexes thereby enhancing feedforward-feedback loops, making the nervous system more prone to object-related priming. In order to develop effective methods for assessment and intervention of DCD over the life span the importance of primary reflex suppression and vestibular stimulation as well as a combination of bottom-up and top-down approaches have to be considered.

Ethics Statement

This Focused Review followed the ethical standards of the World Medical Association's Declaration of Helsinki concerning Ethical Principles of Medical Research involving Human Subjects and in accordance with the Swedish of Ethics.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Author Biography

yes Mats Niklasson received his Licentiate degree in Psychology from Karlstad University in Sweden and is currently preparing his Ph.D. thesis. He has earned a B.A. from Uppsala University with a major in literature and is a former Physical education teacher by training. His interest in sensorimotor development has grown out of his experience from the classroom. Presently he is working as a sensorimotor therapist at the Vestibularis Clinic in Kalmar and is affiliated research fellow at Evidens University College in Göteborg

References

Adolph, K. E. (2008). “Motor and physical development: locomotion” in Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development, eds. M. M. Haith and J. B. Benson (Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press), 359–373.

Google Scholar

Adolph, K. E., and Franchak, J. M. (2017). The development of motor behavior. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2017:e1430. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahonen, T., Kooistra, L., Viholainen, H., and Cantell, M. (2004). “Developmental motor learning disability. A neuropsychological approach,” in Developmental Motor Disorders. A Neuropsychological Perspective, eds. D. Dewey and D. E. Tupper (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 265–290.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Ayd, F. J. Jr. (2000). Lexicon of Psychiatry, Neurology, and the Neurosciences. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 686.

Google Scholar

Ayers, A. J. (1972). Sensory Integration and Learning. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Ayers, A. J. (1989). Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Barnett, A. L. (2014). Is there a ‘Movement Thermometer’ for developmental coordination disorder? Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 1, 132–139. doi: 10.1007/s40474-014-0011-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnett-Cowan, M. (2013). Vestibular perception is slow: a review. Multisens. Res. 26, 387–403. doi: 10.1163/22134808-00002421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergman, A., and Norlander, T. (2005). “Hay sacks anonymous”: living in the shadow of the unidentified. Psychological aspects of physical inactivity from a phenomenological perspective. Qual. Rep. 10, 795–816.

Google Scholar

Bergström, M., Niklasson, M., and Niklasson, I. (1999). Reasons for Training. Mönsterås: Vestibularis.

Berthoz, A., and Petit, J. L. (2006). The Physiology and Phenomenology of Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Besnard, S., Lopez, C., Brandt, T., Denise, P., and Smith, P. F. (2015). Editorial: the vestibular system in cognitive and memory process in mammalians. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9:55. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F., and Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Dev. Rev. 34, 225–264. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, R., Smits-Engelsman, B., Polatajko, H., and Wilson, P. (2012). European Academy of Childhood Disability (EACD): recommendations on the definition, diagnosis and intervention of developmental coordination disorder (long version). Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 54, 54–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04171.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blythe, P. (2009). “Development of the INPP method - From theory to practice” in Attention, Balance and Coordination. The ABC of Learning Success, ed S. Goddard Blythe (Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell), 311–323.

Bruininks, R. H., and Bruininks, B. D. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd Edn. Winsor: NFER-Nelson.

Burman, B. (1977). Keystone Visual Skills Test. Malmö: All-Optik and American Optical Co.

Caçola, P. (2016). Physical and mental health of children with developmental coordination disorder. Front. Public Health 4:224. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capute, A. J., and Accardo, P. J. (1991). Developmental Disabilities in Infancy and Childhood. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brooks.

Capute, A. J., Accardo, P. J., Vining, E. P. G., Rubenstein, J. E., and Harryman, S. (1978). Primitive Reflex Profile. Baltimore, MA: University Park Press.

Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (1973). Rating scales for use in drug studies. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 9, 24–29.

Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (1990). Conners Rating Scales: Manual, Conners Teacher Rating Scales, Conners Parent Rating Scales: Instruments for use With Children and adolescents. North Tonawanda, NY: Multihealth Systems.

Cramer, P. (2006). Protecting the Self. Defense Mechanisms in Action. New York, NY: Springer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Dewey, D., and Bottos, S. (2004). ”Neuroimaging of developmental motor disorders” in Developmental Motor Disorders. A Neuropsychological Perspective, eds. D. Dewey, and D. E. Tupper (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 26–43.

Google Scholar

Dewey, D., Creighton, D. E., Heath, J. A., Wilson, B. N., Anseeuw-Deeks, D., Crawford, S. G., et al. (2011). Assessment of developmental coordination disorder in children born with extremly low birth weights. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 42–56. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldenkrais, M. (1988). Body and Mature Behaviour. Tel-Aviv: ALEF Ltd., 83–94.

Google Scholar

Flouris, A. D., Faught, B. E., Hay, J., and Cairney, J. (2005). Exploring the origins of developmental disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47:436. doi: 10.1017/S0012162205000848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gedo, J. E., and Goldberg, A. (1973). Models of the Mind. A Psychoanalytic Theory. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Gibbs, J., Appleton, J., and Appleton, R. (2007). Dyspraxia or developmental coordination disorder? Unravelling the enigma. Arch. Dis. Child 92, 534–539. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.088054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillberg, C., and Rasmussen, P. (2003). To what extent are learning and behavioral problems brain related? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 108, 81–82. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00128.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goddard Blythe, S. (2009). Attention, Balance and Coordination. The abc of Learning Success. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Goddard Blythe, S. (2014). Neuromotor Immaturity in Children and Adults. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Goldfield, E. C., and Wolff, P. H. (2004). “A dynamical systems perspective on infant action and its development,” in Theories of Infant Development, eds G. Bremner and A. Slater (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.), 3–29.

Google Scholar

Gomez, A., and Sirigu, A. (2015). Developmental coordination disorder:core sensori-motor deficits, neurobiology and etiology. Neuropsychologia 79(Pt B), 272–287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R., and Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurvich, C., Maller, J. J., Lithgow, B., Haghgooie, S., and Kulkarni, J. (2013). Vestibular insights into cognition and psychiatry. Brain Res. 1537, 244–259. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyton, A. C. (1991). Basic Neuroscience: Anatomy and Physiology. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.

Google Scholar

Henderson, S., Sugden, D., and Barnett, A. (2007). Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edn. Oxford: Pearson.

Hollenstein, T. (2011). Twenty years of dynamic systems approaches to development: significant contributions, challenges, and future directions. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 256–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00210.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt, K. S. (1991). Child Development: Diagnoses and Assessment. London: Butterworth-Heinemann.

Hydén, H. (1961). “Biochemical approaches of brain activity,” in Man and Civilization: Control of the Mind, eds. S. Farger and R. Wilson (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill), 18–41.

Janols, L. O., and von Knorring, A. L. (1991). Är med medikamentell behandling motiverad vid hyperaktivitet hos barn? [Is stimulant drug action motivated when the child is hyperactive?]. Läkartidningen 88, 3057–3058.

Jorquera-Cabrera, S., Romero-Ayuso, D., Rodriguez-Gil, G., and Triviño-Juárez, J. M. (2017). Assessment of sensory processing characteristics in children between 3 and 11 years old: a systematic review. Front. Pediatr. 5:57. doi: 10.3389/fped.2017.00057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, B. N., Dewey, D., and Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Hum. Move. Sci. 17, 471–490. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00010-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, J., Brown, T., and Stagnitti, K. (2013). Top-down and bottom-up approaches to motor skill assessment of children: are child-report and parent-report perceptions predictive of children's performance-based assessment results? Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 20, 45–53. doi: 10.3109/11038128.2012.693944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, A., Edwards, L., Sugden, D., and Rosenblum, S. (2010). The development and standardization of the adult developmental co-ordination checklist (ADC). Res. Dev. Disabil. 31, 131–139. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, A., and Sugden, D. A. (2007). Children with developmental coordination disorder. J. R. Soc. Med. 100, 182–186. doi: 10.1177/014107680710011414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirmayer, L. J., and Crafa, D. (2014). What kind of psychiatry? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:435. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Konicarova, J., and Bob, P. (2012). Retained primitive reflexes and ADHD in children. Act. Nerv. Super. 54, 135–138. doi: 10.1007/BF03379591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (2004). The Emotional Brain. London: Phoenix.

Google Scholar

Lin, C. K., Kuo, B. C., and Wu, H. M. (2014). Response to Niklasson's comment on Lin, et al. (2012): “The relation between postural movement and bilateral motor integration.” Percept. Mot. Skills 119, 650–654. doi: 10.2466/15.10.PMS.119c24z9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, P. D. (1990). The Triune Brain in Evolution. Role in Paleocerebral Function. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Mahoney, G., Robinson, C., and Perales, F. (2004). Early motor intervention: the need for new treatment paradigms. Infants Young Child. 17, 291–300. doi: 10.1097/00001163-200410000-00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mailloux, Z. (1990). An overview of sensory integration and praxis tests. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 44, 589–594. doi: 10.5014/ajot.44.7.589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPhillips, M., Hepper, P. G., and Mulhem, G. (2000). Effects of replicating primary-reflex movements on specific reading difficulties in children: a randomized, double blind, controlled trial. Lancet 355, 537–541. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)02179-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missiuna, C., Moll, S., King, G., Stewart, D., and Macdonald, K. (2012). Life experiences of young adults who have coordination difficulties. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 75, 157–166. doi: 10.1177/000841740807500307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missiuna, C., Polatajkajo, H. J., and Pollok, N. (2015). “Strategic management of children with developmental coordination disorder,” in Developmental Coordination Disorder and its Consequences, ed J.Cairney (Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press), 215–252.

Montoro, A. P. P. N., Capistrano, R., Ferrari, E. P., da Silva Reis, M., Luiz Cardoso, F., and Beltrame, T. S. (2016). Concurrent validation of the MABC-2 and developmental coordination disorder questionnaire-BR. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 26, 74–80. doi: 10.7322/jhgd.110421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklasson, M. (2012). “Could motor development be an emergent property of vestibular stimulation and primary reflex inhibition? A tentative approach to sensorimotor therapy,” in Learning Disabilities, ed. W. Sittiprapaporn (Rijeka: InTech), 241–274.

Google Scholar

Niklasson, M. (2013a). The relation between postural movement and bilateral motor integration: comment on Lin, et al. (2012). Percept. Mot. Skills 117, 647–650. doi: 10.2466/15.10.PMS.117x23z9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklasson, M. (2013b). Sensorimotor Therapy: Assessing Quantitative and Qualitative Expressions of Physiological and Psychological Development in Children. Licentiate thesis, Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences, Karlstad University, Sweden.

Niklasson, M., and Niklasson, I. (2007a). Retraining for Balance-Physiological Test Revised. Mönsterås: Vestibularis.

Niklasson, M., and Niklasson, I. (2007b). Retraining for Balance-Orientation and Balance Test Revised. Mönsterås: Vestibularis.

Google Scholar

Niklasson, M., Niklasson, I., and Bergstöm, M. (2007). Retraining for Balance Methods Revised. Mönsterås: Vestibularis.

Niklasson, M., Niklasson, I., and Norlander, T. (2009). Sensorimotor therapy: using stereotypic movements and vestibular stimulation to increase sensorimotor proficiency of children with attentional and motor difficulties. Percept. Mot. Skills 108, 643–669. doi: 10.2466/pms.108.3.643-669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklasson, M., Niklasson, I., and Norlander, T. (2010). Sensorimotor therapy: physical and psychological regressions contribute to an improved kinesthetic and vestibular capacity in children and adolescents with motor difficulties and concentration problems. Soc. Behav. Pers. 38, 327–346. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2010.38.3.327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklasson, M., Rasmussen, P., Niklasson, I., and Norlander, T. (2015). Adults with sensorimotor disorders: enhanced physiological and psychological development following specific sensorimotor training. Front. Psychol. 6:480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norlander, T., Moås, L., and Archer, T. (2005). Noise and stress in primary and secondary school children: noise reduction and increased concentration ability through a short but regularly exercise and relaxation program. School Effectiv. School Improve. 16, 91–99. doi: 10.1080/092434505000114173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pannekoek, L., Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Barrett, N. C., and Schoemaker, M. (2012). The revised DCDQ: is it a suitable screening measure for motor difficulties in adolecents? Adapt. Phys. Activ. Q. 29, 81–97. doi: 10.1123/apaq.29.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F. (2009). Diagnosing Learning Disorders. A Neuropsychological Framework. New York, NY: The Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Peters, J. M., Barnett, A. L., and Henderson, S. E. (2001). Clumsiness, dyspraxia and developmental coordination disorder: how do health and educational professionals in the UK define the terms? Child Care Health Dev. 27, 399–412. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2001.00217.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1953). The Origin of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routhledge and Kegan Paul LTD.

Google Scholar

Piontelli, A. (2015). Development of Normal Fetal Movements. The Last 15 Weeks of Gestation. Milan: Springer.

Pless, M., and Carlsson, M. (2000). Effects of motor skill intervention on developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis. Adapt Phys. Act. Q. 17, 381–401. doi: 10.1123/apaq.17.4.381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poli, E., and Angrilli, A. (2015). Great general startle reflex is associated with greater anxiety levels: a correlational study on 111 young women. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prechtl, H. (1977). The Neurological Examination of the Full-Term Newborn Infant, 2nd Edn. Clinics in developmental medicine no. 63. London: William Heinemann Medical Books LTD.

Purcell, C., Scott-Roberts, S., and Kirby, A. (2015). Implications of DSM-5 for recognising adults with developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Br. J. Occup. Ther. 78, 295–302. doi: 10.1177/0308022614565113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajagopalan, A., Jinu, K. V., Sailesh, K. S., Mishra, S., Reddy, U. K., and Mukkadan, J. K. (2017). Understanding the links between vestibular and limbic systems regulating emotions. J. Nat. Sci. Biol. Med. 8, 11–15. doi: 10.4103/0976-9668.198350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, P., and Gillberg, C. (2000). Natural outcome of ADHD with developmental coordination disorder at age 22 years: a controlled, longitudinal, community based study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 1424–1431. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200011000-00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauss, K., and Pourtois, G. (2013). What is bottom-up and what is top-down in predictive coding? Front. Psychol. 4:276. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodnitzky, R. L. (1988). Van Allen's Pictorial Manual of Neurological Tests. Chicago, IL: Year Book Medical Publishers, Inc.

Rousseau, P. V., Matton, F., Lecuyer, R., and Lahaye, W. (2017). The moro reaction: more than a reflex, a ritualized behavior of nonverbal communication. Infant Behav. Dev. 46, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoemaker, M. M., and Wilson, B. N. (2015). “Screening for developmental coordination disorder in school-age children,” in Developmental Coordination Disorder and its Consequences, ed J. Cairney (Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press), 169–191.

Shumway-Cook, A., and Woollacott, M. (1995). Motor Control. Theory and Practical Applications. Baltimore, MA: Williams and Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Sigmundsson, H. (2003). Perceptual deficits in clumsy children: inter- and intra- modal matching approach- a window into clumsy behavior. Neural Plast. 10, 27–38. doi: 10.1155/NP.2003.27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigmundsson, H., Lorås, H., and Haga, M. (2016). Assessment of motor competence across the life span: aspects of reliability and validity of a new test. SAGE Open 6, 1–10. doi: 10.1177/2158244016633273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sininger, Y. S., and Cone-Wesson, B. (2004). Asymmetric cochlear processing mimics hemispheric specialization. Science 305:1581. doi: 10.1126/science.1100646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smits-Engelsman, B. C., Blank, R., van der Kaay, A. C., Mosterd-van der Meijs, R., Vlugt-van den Brand, E., Polatajko, H. J., et al. (2012). Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 55, 229–237. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugden, D. (2007). Current approaches to intervention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 467–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00467.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tal-Saban, M., Zarka, S., Grotto, I., Ornoy, A., and Parush, S. (2012). The functional profile of young adults with suspected developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Res. Dev. Disabil. 33, 2193–2202. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelen, E. (2000). Motor development as foundation and future of developmental psychology. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 24, 385–397. doi: 10.1080/016502500750037937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelen, E., and Smith, L. B. (2006). “Dynamic systems theory,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, ed R. M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons Inc.), 358–312.

Google Scholar

Touwen, B. C. L. (1979). Examination of the Child With Minor Neurological Dysfunction, 2nd Edn. Clinics in Developmental Medicine no. 71. London: William Heinemann Medical Books Ltd.

Google Scholar

Touwen, B. C. L., and Prechtl, H. F. R. (1970). The Neurological Examination of the Child With Minor Nervous Dysfunction. Clinics in developmental medicine no. 38. London: William Heinemann Medical Books Ltd.

Google Scholar

Tupper, D. E., and Sondell, S. K. (2004). “Motor disorders and neuropsychological development. A historical appreciation,” in Developmental Motor Disorders. A Neuropsychological Perspective, eds D. Dewey and D. E. Tupper (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 3–25.

Vaivre-Douret, L., Lalanne, C., and Golse, B. (2016). Developmental coordination disorder, an umbrella term for motor impairments in children: nature and co-morbid disorders. Front. Psychol. 7:502. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Geert, P. (2011). The contribution of complex dynamic systems to development. Child Dev. Perspect. 5, 273–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00197.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wann, J. (2007). Current approaches to intervention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49:405. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00405.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner-Vacher, S. R., Hamilton, D. A., and Weiner, S. I. (2013). Vestibular activity and cognitive development in children: perspectives. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:92. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiest, G. (2012). Neural and mental hierarchies. Front. Psychol. 3:516. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, B. N., and Crawford, S. G. (2012). The Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire 2007. Available online at: http://www.dcd.ca (Accessed March 23, 2017).

PubMed Abstract

Wilson, B. N., Crawford, S. G., Green, D., Roberts, G., Aylott, A., and Kaplan, B. J. (2009). Psychometric properties of the revised developmental coordination disorder questionnaire. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 29, 182–202. doi: 10.1080/01942630902784761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, B. N., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G., and Roberts, G. (1998). The Development Coordination Disorder Questionnaire 2007. Calgary, AB: Alberta Children's Hospital Decision Support Research Team.

Wilson, P. H. (2004). Practitioner review: approches to assessment and treatment of children with DCD: an evaluative review. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 806–823. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01409.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Zafeiriou, D. I. (2004). Primitive reflexes and postural reactions in neurodevelopmental examination. Pediatr. Neurol. 31, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2004.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwicker, J. G., Missiuna, C., and Boyd, L. A. (2009). Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: a review of hypotheses. J. Child Neurol. 24, 1273–1281. doi: 10.1177/0883073809333537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: DCD, primary reflexes, sensorimotor disorders, sensorimotor therapy, vestibular dysfunction, kinesthetic-vestibular developmental model, arrested development, perceptual priming

Citation: Niklasson M, Rasmussen P, Niklasson I and Norlander T (2018) Developmental Coordination Disorder: The Importance of Grounded Assessments and Interventions. Front. Psychol. 9:2409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02409

Received: 01 September 2017; Accepted: 15 November 2018;
Published: 04 December 2018.

Edited by:

Natasha Kirkham, Birkbeck University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Klaus Libertus, University of Pittsburgh, United States
Jessica S. Horst, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2018 Niklasson, Rasmussen, Niklasson and Norlander. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: mats.irene@vestibularis.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.