Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 10 July 2018
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Making Sense of Adopted Children's Internal Reality Using Narrative Story Stem Techniques: A Mixed-Methods Synthesis

  • Department of Clinical Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Background: Extant research on adopted children has consistently shown that early adverse experiences confer vulnerability to myriad developmental problems, which may be mitigated by the “natural intervention” of adoption itself and/or by treatment efforts. Narrative Story Stems Techniques (NSSTs) have been used in research and clinical practice to assess adopted children's developmental profiles in middle childhood. However, no study to date has systematically reviewed this body of literature.

Objectives: This paper presents a systematic review of research using NSSTs to make sense of adopted children's internal reality (i.e., perceptions, experiences, and representations), in terms of exploring theoretical perspectives as well as critically synthesizing findings and discussing implications.

Methods: State-of-the-art PRISMA guidelines were followed throughout, resulting in the identification of 18 records, comprising six qualitative, 10 quantitative, and two mixed-methods primary papers, reporting on seven unique studies. All records were assessed with regard to methodological quality. Data were extracted and synthesized narratively using an integrated design for mixed-methods synthesis.

Results: The findings suggest that, although NSST research with adopted children is still in its infancy, there is relatively robust evidence supporting the use of these techniques to assess and track developmental change in adopted children's attachment representations. In this regard, the non-verbal (aside from the verbal) approach to children's complex internal reality, as well as a more fine-grained (aside from a categorical or dimensional) perspective on children's NSST responses, are highlighted as particularly valuable in tailoring treatment to a particular child's needs and vulnerabilities. Moreover, several promising avenues for future research and clinical application of NSSTs, including the extension to affect-regulatory and mentalizing perspectives, may further our knowledge and understanding of, and thus treatment efforts toward, these often vulnerable children. However, these findings should be interpreted with caution, due to the limited number of studies characterized by considerable methodological heterogeneity.

Conclusions: In light of the findings of the present review, we strongly advocate future studies using NSSTs in theoretically and empirically consistent ways, in order to gain a better understanding of adopted children's internal reality in terms of attachment representations, affect-regulatory strategies, and mentalizing processes, and to track changes therein.

Introduction

Adopted children have suffered at least one discontinuity in their caregiving environment. For whatever reason, they were separated from their primary caregivers to be adopted. For some of these children, this reason comprised the adverse events which they experienced at the hands of their primary caregivers. Moreover, for children adopted in an international context, most spent some time in residential care (Juffer and Tieman, 2009), some of which is of questionable quality. Such early adverse experiences have been shown to confer vulnerability to myriad developmental problems, ranging from insecure or disorganized attachment representations (e.g., Chisholm et al., 1995; van den Dries et al., 2009), deficits in emotion regulation, and difficulties maintaining peer relationships, to severe behavioral or educational problems (Perry, 2002, 2004; Anda et al., 2006). Once removed from the adverse environment, many of these children catch up to some degree (van Ijzendoorn and Juffer, 2006), due to the “self-righting” capacities of humans (Cicchetti and Rogosch, 1997) and the “good enough” environment of their new homes (Steele et al., 2010; Barone and Lionetti, 2012; Pace et al., 2012). However, alongside developmental progress and catch-up, developmental problems often persist, requiring help and treatment for the child and family.

To enable clinicians to tailor treatment to each adopted child's specific developmental profile, assessment methods that allow detailed exploration of a child's internal reality (i.e., perceptions, experiences, and representations) are needed. To this end, narrative story stem techniques (NSSTs) have been used in research and clinical practice. This generic term was first coined by Page (2001) to refer to narrative methods in which children are asked to complete story stems introduced by an interviewer. These methods have been developed to allow detailed examination of the child's representations of family life, which have been hypothesized to play a role in the mediation of the effects of early adversity on later development, and thus offer ports of entry for change and development (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2005).

NSSTs typically consist of a series of “stems,” the beginnings of stories describing everyday family scenarios, each of which contains an inherent dilemma (e.g., emotional upset, physical hurt, separation from parents, parental discipline and rejection/exclusion). For instance, in the “spilled juice” stem the family is seated around the table to drink juice, when the child protagonist accidentally knocks over the jar as s/he reaches for more juice. The stems are simultaneously spoken and played out with a standard set of family figures, animals, and play props. The child is then invited to “show me and tell me what happens next” (e.g., Hodges and Steele, 2000; Page et al., 2008). Interviews are videotaped and recordings are transcribed, to produce a script consisting of what the child says (the verbal narrative), what the interviewer says, and what the child does (the non-verbal narrative). Subsequently, children's narrative responses are coded for themes of interest. Currently, several approaches to coding NSSTs are used, most of which code elements of narrative content as well as overall structural characteristics of stories (for a review of scoring systems, see Page, 2001). Coding of children's story completions can thus reveal aspects of representations or scripts regarding social relationships, as well as affective and behavioral regulation processes (Robinson, 2007). Hence, NSSTs may prove useful in gaining important insights into adopted children's inner experiences from a theoretical, empirical, and clinical point of view. To date, extant research using NSSTs with adopted children has not been systematically reviewed. The present review aims to fill this gap.

Here, we set the stage by elaborating on why an attachment perspective is central to understanding adoption and developmental processes in adopted children. After describing the methodology used to conduct this systematic review, we provide a systematic, critical, and integrated overview of the quantitative and qualitative findings of primary studies that have attempted to make sense of adopted children's internal reality using NSSTs. We conclude with a discussion of the current evidence base for the use of NSSTs with adopted children, and highlight promising avenues for research and clinical application.

Adoption in An Attachment Perspective

Attachment experiences (i.e., day-to-day interactive experiences between child and caregiver) become organized in memory in internal working models (IWMs; Bowlby, 1973, 1982) from infancy onwards. Through accumulated experience with specific caregivers the developing child internalizes experiences of being responded to by the caregiver and learns about the predictability of available care and protection. IWMs tend to become automatic, increasingly operating outside conscious awareness (Hodges et al., 2003), allowing the child to interpret and predict the caregiver's behavior, and to plan immediate and future responses. IWMs, as basic scripts for human relationships, thus exert a powerful influence on the child's understanding of current experiences, as well as his/her expectations of and reactions to new interactions and experiences (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2003, 2005). When children's needs for care have been adequately met, they develop secure attachment representations or IWMs (Page et al., 2008). Conversely, when children experience significantly inadequate care, as is the case for adopted children with a history of early adverse experiences, they are likely to develop insecure or disorganized IWMs (Carlson et al., 1989; Chisholm et al., 1995; Jaffari-Bimmel et al., 2006; Page et al., 2008). In this context, the meta-analysis of van den Dries et al. (2009) found that adopted children with a history of institutionalization showed less attachment security and more disorganized attachments, as assessed with observational but not self-report measures, than their non-adopted peers.

Although extreme insecure attachments in childhood have been associated with a wide range of developmental problems later in life (Howe, 2001; Hushion et al., 2006; Rutter et al., 2007; Page et al., 2008), research also suggests there is potential to catch up at cognitive and emotional levels (Dozier, 2005; Juffer and van Ijzendoorn, 2007; Bick and Dozier, 2008). These seemingly contradictory findings originate in the inherent nature of IWMs, which are characterized by both relative endurance and relative mutability (Page et al., 2008). IWMs can be revised over time in relation to significant changes in the caregiving environment (Steele et al., 2008, 2010; Pace et al., 2012). In this regard, several authors have described adoption as a “natural intervention” (e.g., van Ijzendoorn and Juffer, 2006). Adoption and the accompanying new attachment relationships are the most dramatic of any interventions that can help the child revise their early insecure or disorganized IWMs and alter the developmental course of children who have suffered traumatic experiences in early life (Schofield and Beek, 2005; Jaffari-Bimmel et al., 2006; Steele et al., 2007a; Pace et al., 2012). The new attachment relationships within the adoptive family develop in the interplay between what the child brings in terms of internalized early relationship experiences (i.e., IWMs) and what the adoptive caregivers bring in terms of their own expectations and attachment histories (Steele et al., 2003).

Researchers and clinicians are seeking ways to accurately assess change in aspects of adopted children's internal reality, including representations of attachment relationships. Although observational-behavioral measures (e.g., the Strange Situation; Ainsworth et al., 1978) or parent-report questionnaires (e.g., Attachment Security Questionnaire; Chisholm et al., 1995) also provide information about children's IWMs, these measures are limited in two crucial ways. First, there is a surprising lack of measures developed to specifically assess IWMs in middle childhood, although from a developmental perspective IWMs can undergo meaningful changes during this period (e.g., Bosmans and Kerns, 2015). Second, although these measures provide more general information about IWMs in terms of attachment classifications and/or dimensions, the question arises whether such general classifications/dimensions can provide practitioners with sufficiently detailed information about the adopted child's development to guide treatment. Several interview measures (e.g., Friends and Family Interview; Steele and Steele, 2005) are used in research and clinical practice to assess children's attachment representations. However, these measures are mostly applicable from age 8 onwards, plausibly because interviews rely heavily on children's explicit mentalizing (Fonagy and Luyten, 2009) and verbal-expressive skills, capacities that may be slower to develop in (internationally) adopted children with a history of early adversity. Furthermore, in interviews children are asked directly to talk about real (past or current) relationships, leaving them little room for escape when this theme proves too arousal- or anxiety-provoking or intrusive.

Methods

Aim and Design

This review aims to identify studies making use of NSSTs to understand adopted children's internal reality and to synthesize the results of these studies. The objectives include exploring theoretical perspectives, summarizing empirical findings, and discussing clinical implications. To this end, a mixed-methods systematic literature review was conducted, following the PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) throughout.

Eligibility Criteria

Studies were considered eligible if they conformed to following SPIDER criteria, as advocated by Cooke et al. (2012):

Sample: adopted children aged 31–11;

Phenomenon of Interest: internal reality;

Design: NSST;

Evaluation: aspects of children's internal reality;

Research type: qualitative, quantitative, and mixed.

Additionally, only studies published in English-language peer-reviewed journals or books were considered for inclusion in this review.

Search Strategy

A systematic literature search was conducted in May 2016 through the electronic databases ScienceDirect, Wiley Online Library, Ovid (including PsycArticles), Taylor & Francis Online, and Sage. Based on the eligibility criteria, the search strings [(“adopted child” OR adoptee) AND narratives] and [(“adopted child” OR adoptee) AND “story stems”] were used. After a first screening of titles and abstracts, 18 records were selected. All full-text papers were screened for eligibility, resulting in five records being excluded. Reference searching of the remaining 13 records yielded an additional five papers being identified, resulting in 18 records being included in the present review, comprising six primary qualitative, 10 primary quantitative, and two primary mixed-methods studies. These 18 records report on seven unique studies (see information in Table 1 for which records report on the same study). The PRISMA flowchart is presented in Figure 1, and characteristics of the included studies are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of studies included in review.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart. Figure reproduced with permission under the Creative Commons Attribution License from: Moher et al. (2009).

Quality Assessment of Studies

Assessing the quality of the primary studies was considered as an essential part of the present review, though not a priori as a reason for exclusion (Heyvaert et al., 2017). All included studies were assessed with regard to methodological quality, using the QARI Critical Appraisal Tool for “Interpretive and Critical Research” and “Observational Studies” (Joanna Briggs Institute, 2011) rated independently by two of the authors (NV and DB), followed by joint discussion (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality assessment of studies included in review.

Data Extraction and Synthesis

Data were extracted from the studies and synthesized using an integrated design (Sandelowski et al., 2006), that is, findings from all included studies were extracted and synthesized in an interactive dialogue in which all data from the primary studies, whether qualitative, quantitative, or mixed, were ascribed equal status and were analyzed concurrently (Heyvaert et al., 2013), using tables with emerging thematic headings. A meta-analysis was neither appropriate nor feasible (due to the small number of and large diversity in quantitative studies) for data synthesis.

Results

As shown in Table 1, most studies to date have used NSSTs exclusively in an attachment perspective. In what follows, we first discuss why NSSTs have proven particularly useful in assessing adopted children's IWMs, paying attention to (a) the advantages of NSSTs as an assessment method compared with other methods to gain insight into IWMs, and (b) global vs. more fine-grained coding of adopted children's responses to NSSTs (section Suitability of NSSTs for Assessment of Adopted Children in Attachment Perspective). Next, we provide an overview of findings from studies (a) in which NSSTs were used as a measure of child attachment per se (section Child Attachment), (b) to gain insight into other child characteristics potentially influencing or influenced by child attachment as assessed by NSSTs (section Child Characteristics Potentially Influencing or Influenced by Child Attachment), and (c) investigating the role of parental attachment in developmental change in adopted children's IWMs as assessed by NSSTs (section Role of Parental Attachment in Developmental Change in Children's IWMs). We then broaden the scope to two complementary perspectives that have been suggested in the literature (section Broadening the Scope From Attachment to Affect-Regulatory and Mentalizing Perspectives). Finally, we argue for the value of NSSTs in tailoring treatment to a particular adopted child's needs (section From Assessment to Tailored Treatment).

Suitability of NSSTs for Assessment of Adopted Children in Attachment Perspective

NSSTs as a Route to Understanding IWMs

Unsurprisingly, as all reviewed studies used story stems as an attachment-based instrument to assess adopted children, all authors advocated the value of an attachment perspective in understanding these children's difficulties. NSSTs are based on a natural mode of self-expression for children, which facilitates direct assessment of children's representations in a non-intrusive way (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2003, 2005, 2009; Heller et al., 2006; Page et al., 2008). Notably, in responding to story stems, children are not directly reporting on real (past or current) family experiences, nor are they merely fantasizing (Hodges and Steele, 2000); rather, they are expressing aspects of their (in)ability to cope with and give meaning to these experiences (Hodges et al., 2005, 2009). The latter also implies that, across an array of different story stem completions, NSSTs provide a “window” into a child's ability to imagine what can happen in the hypothetical situations depicted in the different stems (Hodges et al., 2009).

Hodges and Steele (2000) and Hodges et al. (2005) refer to two major advantages of the use of story stems over self-report measures. First, play can reveal aspects of procedural memory, that is, experiences in the child's life that are not part of verbally based memory but are recalled in perceptual, affective, or physical (sensorimotor) form. Procedural memory operates automatically, outside conscious awareness, and therefore requires assessment by means other than conscious verbal recall or description (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2003, 2005), such as play (Steele et al., 2003, 2010). Children who have experienced early adversity may give behavioral evidence of their fearful or aversive expectations of their parents, without necessarily being able to recall specific events (Hodges et al., 2005). Furthermore, the opportunity to play provides an intermediate space in which the child can show his/her ability to cope with relational content that is too painful or anxiety-provoking, for instance, by avoiding or transforming the core dilemma of the story stem, “revealing some of the defenses employed to make thinking about it bearable” (Hodges and Steele, 2000, p. 435).

A second advantage of story stems in the traditions of the MacArthur Story Stem Battery (Bretherton et al., 1990) and the Attachment Story Completion Task (Bretherton et al., 1990), such as the Story Stem Assessment Profile (Hodges et al., 2007), lies in the psychological distancing provided through displacement. Children are asked about a standard doll family rather than trying to replicate their own family configuration (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2003, 2005; Page et al., 2008). Some of the story stems use animal figures, providing further displacement from the child's personal experience. As children with early adversity sometimes seem to experience even dilemmas portrayed with doll figures as too “close to the bone” and anxiety-provoking (Hodges et al., 2003, 2005), attributing difficult emotions or behaviors to animal figures may facilitate the child's narrative expression. In this context, it is important to note that in the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task (MCAST; Green et al., 2000) the child's identification with the doll figures is explicitly emphasized by asking the child to choose dolls to represent him/herself and his/her caregiver.

Despite these advantages, there is a large variety across studies in how story stem batteries are composed and in how children's responses to the story stems are coded, rendering direct comparison between study results difficult2. However, even in view of the variety of coding procedures, two approaches are discernible on the whole.

Global vs. More Fine-Grained Perspectives on IWMs

In assessing the attachment representations of adopted children, two different approaches are used. Some studies look at children's global attachment style in a categorical or dimensional way. Others view story stem responses from a more detailed and fine-grained perspective, assessing the component elements subsumed under the general construct of a “secure” or other attachment organization (Hodges et al., 2005, 2009). These latter authors argue that it is not particularly useful merely to establish the child's global attachment style, as children who have experienced early adversity are very unlikely to show “secure” attachment organization and are likely to be “disorganized” (Carlson et al., 1989; Hodges et al., 2005, 2009). Similarly, in examining changes in attachment during placement, to expect to find that a child shifts from an “insecure” to a “secure” category of attachment organization would be too gross a categorization to be useful (Hodges et al., 2005). Developmental recovery in these children can take a long time, and it is likely that some of the effects of their earlier experiences will remain with them permanently or at least well into adulthood (Howe, 1998; Hodges et al., 2005).

Fourteen records included in this review reported on the findings of six unique studies (a) in which NSSTs were used as a measure of child attachment per se (section Child Attachment), (b) to gain insight into other child characteristics potentially influencing or influenced by child attachment as assessed by NSSTs (section Child Characteristics Potentially Influencing or Influenced by Child Attachment), and (c) investigating the role of parental attachment in developmental change in adopted children's IWMs as assessed by NSSTs (section Role of Parental Attachment in Developmental Change in Children's IWMs). All but four of these 14 papers reported on quantitative findings: the Steele et al. (2009, 2010) papers adopted a qualitative approach, whereas the Steele et al. (2003, 2008) papers adopted a mixed-methods approach.

Child Attachment

Attachment Classifications

Three studies, reported in five papers (Pace and Zavattini, 2011; Barone and Lionetti, 2012; Kocovska et al., 2012; Pace et al., 2012, 2014), investigated the distribution of attachment classifications in adopted children (Table 3). Across all three studies, the prevalence of children classified as insecurely attached ranged from 44 to 75%. Of clinical importance is the prevalence of children classified as disorganized, estimated to range between 32 and 37%. These findings are in line with the results of a meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children (van den Dries et al., 2009) showing that 47% of children were securely attached and 31% were disorganized. Importantly, the percentage of adopted children with disorganized attachment is significantly higher than the percentage of disorganized children in normative samples (e.g., Barone et al., 2009; for a meta-analysis, see van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999) and comparable to the percentage of disorganized children growing up in high-risk circumstances as reported by van Ijzendoorn et al. (1999) and Gloger-Tippelt and Kappler (2016).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Distribution of attachment classifications.

Attachment Dimensions

Román et al. (2012) found that both adopted and institutionalized children displayed more indicators of insecurity, avoidance, and disorganization than the control group (children living with their birth families, with no history of maltreatment). These findings are only partially in line with the meta-analytic results of van den Dries et al. (2009), reporting that adopted children showed less disorganization than institutionalized children, but more disorganization than non-adopted biological peers. One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the adopted children studied by Román et al. (2012) spent on average 2 years in institutional care, rendering the differentiation between adopted and institutionalized children less clear.

Pace et al. (2014) found that children classified as securely attached received higher scores on coherence of mind (i.e., narrative coherence, as expressed in quality, quantity, relevance, and manner) compared with children classified as insecurely attached. Hodges et al. (2005) investigated how the attachment dimensions of adopted children changed over time. Two years after adoption, the children showed a decrease in global defense/avoidance scores. However, indicators of attachment disorganization in their stories remained largely unchanged over 2 years.

Fine-Grained Aspects of Attachment

Vorria et al. (2006) concluded that adopted children showed lower scores on story resolution, narrative coherence, and prosocial themes, and a higher score on avoidance, compared with a group of biological children, even after adjusting for children's cognitive developmental level. However, the adopted and comparison groups did not differ significantly with regard to the occurrence of atypical and negative themes.

Hodges et al. (2003, 2005) investigated how fine-grained aspects of attachment evolved in the first 2 years after adoption (Table 4). Overall, aspects of new and more positive representations had begun to develop; however, already-established negative representations persisted. Using a qualitative approach, Steele et al. (2010) illustrated the former with a case study of “Larry,” a boy aged 4.5 years at the time he was first placed with his adoptive parent. At this time he was at first unwilling to acknowledge the distress as outlined in the story stem, followed by increasingly aggressive and catastrophic content instead of helping parental representations. Two years later, this boy's completion of the same story stem was characterized by helping parental representations and realistic/pleasurable domestic life.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Longitudinal fine-grained attachment results from Hodges et al. (2003) and Hodges et al. (2005).

Child Characteristics Potentially Influencing or Influenced by Child Attachment

Five studies, reported in seven papers (Hodges et al., 2003, 2005; Vorria et al., 2006; Barone and Lionetti, 2012; Pace et al., 2012, 2014; Román et al., 2012), investigated one or more factors influencing or influenced by child attachment: (a) child gender, (b) characteristics of the adoption process, (c) developmental covariates, and (d) developmental outcomes.

Child Gender

Pace et al. (2012) reported that the distribution of attachment categories did not differ significantly between boys and girls. However, in a follow-up study, Pace et al. (2014) found that, from a categorical perspective, boys were rated significantly more insecure and disorganized than girls; from a dimensional approach, they showed significantly higher disorganization and a trend for lower coherence of mind scores compared with girls. In the same vein, Román et al. (2012), using attachment dimensions, found that girls showed more security and less insecurity than boys.

Characteristics of the Adoption Process

Pace et al. (2012) found that the distribution of attachment categories did not differ significantly between domestically and internationally adopted children. Similarly, Pace et al. (2012, 2014) reported that the presence/absence of siblings in the family had no significant influence on the distribution of attachment categories. However, Román et al. (2012), comparing adoption of single children vs. siblings, found that although there were no differences in security and avoidance, children adopted with a sibling showed fewer signs of insecurity and disorganization.

Pace et al. (2012, 2014) found that the distribution of attachment categories did not differ significantly by age at adoption. Similarly, Román et al. (2012) failed to find significant correlations between age at adoption and attachment dimensions. However, Pace et al. (2014), surprisingly, found a significant negative correlation between age at adoption and a dimensional disorganization score (but not coherence of mind or mentalizing skills). Adopting a more fine-grained perspective on attachment, Hodges et al. (2003, 2005) found meaningful differences between early- and late-adopted children (see Table 5). Interestingly, when comparing the effects of age at placement at different time-points, Hodges et al. (2005) found that, overall, positive changes in attachment aspects were larger and took place more quickly in younger vs. older children. Late-adopted children's display of many characteristics of disorganization remained unchanged over 2 years, suggesting that they were still struggling with disorganizing emotional responses, but the reduction in avoidance suggests that they had become more able to represent these in the narrative, acknowledging dilemmas and maintaining better functioning in the face of stress.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Significant and non-significant fine-grained attachment differences between early- and late-adopted children.

Furthermore, Pace et al. (2012, 2014) found that the distribution of attachment categories did not differ significantly by duration of placement (but Pace et al., 2014 did report that longer-placed children showed higher mentalizing scores as assessed by NSST). Interestingly, more detailed analyses by Román et al. (2012) indicated that although age at adoption did not influence dimensional attachment scores, when this variable was entered simultaneously with “time in adoptive family” in a hierarchical regression, both predictors were positively related to security scores in adopted children. This suggests that time spent in the adoptive family is an important aspect for developmental catch-up with regard to attachment. Moreover, Román et al. (2012) reported that having a significant family experience prior to adoption was associated with lower avoidance than having spent the pre-adoption time in institutions. In the subsample of adopted children with family experience before adoption, the amount of time spent with a family before institutionalization was positively related to security. In the same vein, older age at entrance to an institution and shorter experience there were related to higher security and lower insecurity. However, in the subgroup of adopted children who had spent their whole pre-adoption life in an orphanage, none of these variables showed significant relationships with attachment representations.

Developmental Covariates

Pace et al. (2012) found no significant correlations between attachment categories and the child's level of schooling, non-verbal IQ, or language comprehension. In a follow-up study, Pace et al. (2014) again found no associations between secure/insecure classifications and children's level of schooling (or maternal age or level of education). The findings reported by Román et al. (2012), using attachment dimensions, are more mixed. These authors found that although linguistic competence did not influence attachment indicators, children with higher intelligence showed more indicators of secure attachment. In line with these latter findings and from a fine-grained perspective, Vorria et al. (2006) found a positive association between cognitive capacities and story resolution, narrative coherence, and prosocial themes, and a negative association between cognitive capacities and avoidance.

Developmental Outcomes

Barone and Lionetti (2012) found that adopted children classified as disorganized performed worse on emotional competence compared with adopted children with coherent attachment organizations, regardless of security, whereas no difference was found between securely and non-securely attached children. However, Pace et al. (2014) found that children classified as secure received higher scores on mentalizing skills than children classified as insecure. From a fine-grained perspective, Hodges et al. (2005) found that children with more conduct problems showed more adult aggression, catastrophes, denial or distress, and avoidance in their story completions, and less realistic mastery. Furthermore, children with more peer problems showed more denial of distress and catastrophes, and less realistic mastery, as well as fewer instances of children helping other children. In contrast, children with more prosocial behavior showed more realistic mastery, acknowledgment of distress, children helping other children, and realistic/pleasurable domestic life, and less avoidance.

Role of Parental Attachment in Developmental Change in Children's IWMs

Quantitative and qualitative studies have shown that adoptive parents' attachment representations play an important role in determining the newly formed relationship with their adopted child across time (Steele et al., 2003, 2008, 2009, 2010).

Attachment Classifications

Two studies, reported in three papers (Pace and Zavattini, 2011; Barone and Lionetti, 2012; Pace et al., 2012), investigated the concordance between children's attachment categories, assessed 6–18 months after placement, and their adoptive mothers' attachment as indexed by Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George et al., 1985) categories around the time of placement. Barone and Lionetti (2012) found a significant concordance between children's and mothers' attachment of 60% using a three-way classification (secure vs. insecure-avoidant vs. insecure-ambivalent) and 80% using a two-way classification (secure vs. insecure). No significant concordance was found between children's and fathers' attachment. However, the authors reported that all children classified as secure had at least one parent with a secure attachment pattern, and 75% of these children had two parents with secure attachment. In contrast, Pace and Zavattini (2011) and Pace et al. (2012) did not find a significant concordance between children's and mothers' attachment using a two-way classification. Specifically, Pace et al. (2012) reported that 40% of disorganized children were adopted by mothers with a dismissing state of mind, and 10% by mothers with an unresolved state of mind.

Fine-Grained Aspects of Attachment

Two studies, reported in six papers (Steele et al., 2003, 2007a, 2008, 2009, 2010; Pace et al., 2012), tapped into more fine-grained aspects of children's attachment and associations with parental attachment. Pace et al. (2012), in addition to—and, importantly, in contrast to—the categorical approach, found significant associations between maternal AAI states of mind, assessed 40 days after placement, and MCAST global scales, administered 7–8 months after placement. Specifically, maternal “Idealization of mother” was significantly negatively related to children's “Coherence of mind.” In addition, although both maternal “Idealization of mother” and “Anger toward father” were significantly positively related to children's “Global disorganization score,” only the former accounted for unique variance when entered simultaneously as predictors of children's disorganization.

Steele et al. (2003), using another fine-grained coding scheme, found that the story completions of children, gathered immediately after adoption, adopted by “insecure” mothers (as opposed to “secure” mothers; as assessed immediately before adoption) were more likely to show (a) catastrophic fantasies; (b) child aggression; (c) adult aggression; (d) throwing out or throwing away; (e) bizarre or atypical content; (f) child injured/dead; and (g) adult injured/dead. Subsequently, Steele et al. (2003, 2007a) calculated a composite aggressiveness score, “child aggression,” based on these seven themes5. In general, the “child aggression” composite was found to be related to maternal attachment (assessed by the AAI) and parents' representations of their children (assessed by the Parent Development Interview; Aber et al., 1985; 3 months into placement). Detailed findings are presented in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Associations between parental attachment and fine-grained child attachment aspects.

Steele et al. (2008) calculated two composite scores: a “disorganization composite,” comprising (a) catastrophic fantasies; (b) bizarre/atypical material; (c) bad-to-good shifts; (d) extreme aggression; (e) magic omnipotence; and (f) child appearing parent-like or role reversal; and an “insecure composite,” comprising (a) child endangered; (b) child injured/dead; (c) adult unaware; (d) adult actively rejecting; (e) adult injured/dead; (f) excessive compliance; (g) extreme aggression; (h) neutralization; and (i) throwing away. Subsequently, for both composites, the sample was divided into three equal groups of children with “low,” “medium,” and “high” scores. Steele et al. (2008) reported that when one or both parents were securely attached, as assessed by the AAI at the time of adoptive placement, children were less likely to belong to the high insecure or disorganized group 2 years later. By contrast, when neither parent's AAI was secure (i.e., Insecure—either Dismissing or Preoccupied), 86% of the children scored in the highest group for disorganization.

Finally, in a qualitative approach, Steele et al. (2009) presented two cases by means of the mothers' pre-adoptive AAI transcript excerpts and their children's story stem completion examples to illustrate their findings: one in which the mother's AAI was classified as autonomous-secure and one in which the mother was classified as insecure-dismissing. Although both children showed indices of Insecurity in their story stem completions early in the placement, at 2-year follow-up, the story stem completions of the adopted child placed with the secure parent showed many themes indicative of Security, whereas those of the adopted child placed with the insecure-dismissing parent continued to show a preponderance of themes indicative of Insecurity, Disorganization, and Defensive Avoidance (Steele et al., 2009). In this regard, Steele et al. (2010) hypothesized about what it is that parents with secure-autonomous states of mind do to facilitate these positive changes in their adopted children. First, they stated that parents' secure-autonomous states of mind might be associated with open and flexible processing of affect, which in turn helps the child to modulate negative affect. Furthermore, Steele et al. (2010) proposed that in parents with secure-autonomous states of mind complementary processes are at play, involving awareness of their own and the child's mental states, and the ability to envision a way forward that brings parent and child together. This stance nurtures a similar ability for the child to integrate a range of feeling states because the new parental environment supports exploration and integrative efforts. Although, to the best of our knowledge, both perspectives—affect-regulatory and mentalizing—have not been thoroughly tested empirically to date, Steele et al.'s (2010) theorizing sets the stage for broadening the scope on the use and usefulness of NSSTs.

Broadening the Scope From Attachment to Affect-Regulatory and Mentalizing Perspectives

Affect-Regulatory Perspective: from Considering Challenging Situations to Experiencing and Dealing With Challenging Emotions

As explained above, story stems consist of mildly stressful scenarios that are part of everyday childhood experience (Steele et al., 2003; Page et al., 2008). As such, story stems not only tap into children's attachment representations by evoking something about the content of the child's perceptions and experiences of relationships with significant others. Story stems also immediately evoke the affects and the coping and defensive strategies children have available to manage emotionally challenging situations. As such, a child's story stem responses also shows us something about his/her way of dealing with emotionally challenging situations, such as how s/he modulates anxiety and aggression, attempts to master conflict, and makes defensive maneuvers (e.g., by disengaging from the task, or changing the constraints of the story; Steele et al., 2010). In this regard, Steele et al. (2010) point to the given that the IWM concept, and its meaning-making capacities, has been extended from a mere representational level toward a set of rules for how to interpret emotions and engage in behavioral strategies for managing them. Perhaps the most important function of IWMs is to regulate the individual's experience of intense emotion and to direct the individual's behavioral and psychological responses. In the same vein, Román et al. (2012) emphasize how inner representations serve as a guide for perceiving oneself and others, as well as interpreting one's emotions and regulating one's emotional behavior. In sum, the representations make up the initial content of IWMs, informing one's sense of self, others, and what to expect, but also immediately evoke affect-regulation and behavioral strategies, particularly when distressed—that is, when the attachment system is activated.

Adopting a Mentalizing Perspective

In addition to the attachment and affect-regulatory perspectives, a third perspective on NSSTs, that of mentalizing (Fonagy et al., 2002), has recently been suggested (Steele et al., 2010; Pace et al., 2014). Mentalizing refers to the capacity to understand one's own and others' behaviors as motivated from an internal world (feelings, thoughts, desires, anxieties, etc.) and the ability to guide one's actions accordingly. In “good enough” circumstances, that is, an environment that is relatively free of obstacles, the process of developing mentalizing capacities is part of ordinary parent–child interactions. Fonagy and Allison (2012) describe how secure attachment relationships ensure normal family discourse and playful interactions offering the necessary opportunities to learn about the links between internal states and actions that scaffold the development of mentalizing.

However, early adversity, as is experienced by many adopted children, interferes with the development of mentalizing capacities in two possible ways. First, these children have suffered at least one discontinuity in their early attachment relationships, and thus have had less consistent scaffolding opportunities regarding mentalizing. Second, in cases of cumulative adverse experiences (e.g., abuse or neglect) the attachment system becomes disorganized (for a comprehensive review, see Cicchetti and Valentino, 2006) and mentalizing capacities remain underdeveloped (Fonagy and Luyten, 2009) or become conflicted as a form of decoupling, inhibition, or even a phobic reaction to mentalizing (Pace et al., 2014). In the context of adversity, caregivers will be inaccurate in their reflections of the child's state of mind and the behavior of the caregiver will evoke a fear response in the child, increasing the risk of the child developing a disorganized representation of self and interfering with the capacity to reflect on mental states. For these children, to explore and think about the mind of frightening/frightened, and therefore hostile and harmful, parents would be extremely dangerous, so they block their ability to reflect, implicitly assume malignity in other people's actions, and apply this perspective to new relationships (Pace et al., 2014). Hence, such children frequently show contradictory behaviors toward their adoptive parents, oscillating between compliance, dependence, passivity, withdrawal, rejection, hostility, and provocation (Steele et al., 2010). These behaviors can make it hard to build an attachment relationship based on trust and a sense of belonging and protection in their adoptive families (Slade, 2007; Steele et al., 2007b).

Consequently, adoptive parents have to be equipped with “high quality adoptive parenting” (Pace et al., 2012, p. 47) capabilities. In this context, as discussed above, research to date has emphasized the importance of a secure attachment state of mind in parents for managing the conflicting and vulnerable representations that adopted children bring with them into their new families (Levy and Orlans, 2003; Schofield and Beek, 2005). Interestingly, “high quality adoptive parenting” may also be understood within the mentalizing framework as parents who are highly reflective and can rely on their capacity to think about their own and their child's internal reality (Sharp et al., 2006; Steele et al., 2010; Pace et al., 2014). Little research to date has specifically investigated this mentalizing perspective in adopted children using story stems. One exception is the study by Pace et al. (2014), in which MCAST story stem completions were also coded for mentalizing skills.

From Assessment to Tailored Treatment

As discussed earlier, NSSTs, in particular more fine-grained approaches, have proven valuable (attachment perspective) or at least promising (affect-regulatory and mentalizing perspectives) in systematically assessing adopted children's internal reality from multiple perspectives (Hodges et al., 2005; Heller et al., 2006). These techniques may also be helpfully used to examine the effects following major change in the child's external situation (Hodges et al., 2005; Heller et al., 2006). Such changes, besides placement changes, can include therapeutic intervention with a parent and also therapeutic help for children themselves (Hodges et al., 2005). Importantly, this implies that NSSTs allow us to map areas of difficulty so that parents and professionals have a clearer idea of the child's needs and vulnerabilities (Hodges et al., 2005; Pace et al., 2014).

Hodges et al. (2009), for instance, used “instrumental case studies” (Stake, 2005) to illustrate that previously maltreated children can show diverse story completions to the same story stem, reflecting the way the child has adapted to the specific characteristics of the early childhood environment and is able or unable to make use of his/her new environment. More compellingly, Page et al. (2008), by means of a fraternal twin case study (already reported on in Heller et al., 2006) described how both children, despite their common history of severe maltreatment and multiple placements, showed distinct profiles of developmental vulnerabilities and strengths based on their story stem responses; the authors argued how such individual profiles can aid in informing individually tailored therapeutic intervention. In sum, with such detailed individual understanding of the child's internal reality, parents and professionals can more sensitively tailor their parenting and their support to fit children's individual needs, thus assisting the children's developmental recovery (Hodges et al., 2005, 2009; Hodges, 2007; Page et al., 2008).

Discussion

The findings of this narrative mixed-methods review of 18 papers in which NSSTs were used to gain insight into aspects of adopted children's (aged 3–11) internal reality may be summarized as follows. The review provides some preliminary conclusions about the suitability and value of NSSTs, as well as points to several promising avenues for research and clinical application of NSSTs. However, the small number of studies as well as the large differences in methodology among these studies need to be taken into account when discussing the implications of this body of literature carefully and critically. Of special note in this regard are: (a) different theoretical perspectives and a trend toward a shifting paradigm, rendering (b) NSSTs not merely valuable as a content-based but also as a performance-based measure; (c) the value of NSSTs not only from a disorder-oriented but also from a person-centered perspective in diagnosing and assessing adopted children's attachment vulnerabilities and changes therein; (d) preliminary findings with regard to factors influencing adopted children's attachment representations as assessed by NSSTs. (e) We formulate avenues for future research which may aid in building a more consistent body of knowledge in this domain, which may, in turn, inform clinical practice. (f) Finally, we discuss limitations of the present review.

Toward a Shifting Paradigm in Theoretical Perspectives on NSSTs

Most studies using NSSTs with adopted children to date have used these techniques exclusively as attachment-based instruments to gain insight into adopted children's IWMs. In this regard, NSSTs have proven particularly valuable in an age group for which few self-report attachment measures are available (Bosmans and Kerns, 2015), as the opportunity to play allows children to more easily express non-conscious aspects of their internal reality stored in procedural memory, and the possibility of displacement allows children to more easily express content that would otherwise be too anxiety-provoking. The latter advantage is, however, not claimed by the developers and users of the MCAST (Green et al., 2000). In this context, it is important to note that the paucity of studies using NSSTs with adopted children, and the fact that in two of the three studies (reported in five papers) using the MCAST children's narrative responses were coded exclusively to investigate attachment classifications6, preclude any firm conclusions as to the importance of displacement. However, based on our own clinical experience as well as others' (Hodges and Steele, 2000; Hodges et al., 2005; Davies, 2011), we strongly advocate allowing children, particularly children who have experienced early adversity, the opportunity to approach potentially anxiety-provoking material indirectly, for instance, through the use of displacement.

The finding that all but a few of the reviewed studies used NSSTs to exclusively assess adopted children's attachment representations is striking, as several authors have suggested that both an affect-regulatory and a mentalizing perspective on NSST responses hold promise in making sense of adopted children's internal reality. To date, only one study (Pace et al., 2014) has explicitly extended the use of NSST to a mentalizing perspective.

NSSTs as Content-Based As Well As Performance-Based Measures

The discussion about theoretical perspectives as described above goes hand in hand with different, though potentially complementary, perspectives on what children's NSST responses reveal precisely. Most authors emphasize that NSSTs are based on a natural mode of self-expression for children, facilitating direct assessment of representations in a non-intrusive way; whereas some point to the way affect and affect-regulation strategies and mentalizing processes can be shown in a here-and-now playful context. This raises the question whether children's story stem responses “merely” offer the clinician a window into the content of a child's internal reality and his/her expectations of caregiving others, and thus function as a projective technique; or whether they also bring affect, affect-regulation, and mentalization processes into action by bringing the child into a mildly stressful situation which s/he has to cope with here and now, and thus also function as a performance-based assessment technique. Both perspectives may be of additional value to clinical and research practice.

NSSTs as Disorder-Oriented As Well As Person-Centered Perspective in Diagnosing and Assessing Attachment Vulnerabilities and Changes Therein

With regard to an attachment perspective, NSSTs as an assessment instrument may be of interest to researchers and clinicians alike, as children's narrative responses can be coded at three different levels—categorical, dimensional, and fine-grained—providing three complementary perspectives on the child's IWMs. In sum, the findings from the studies included in the present review reveal that despite the large variety in story stem battery composition and coding procedures and regardless of the attachment level, adopted children are at increased risk for showing signs of insecure or disorganized attachment at the time of placement. This converges with findings of studies using other attachment measures, such as the Strange Situation (e.g., Rutter et al., 2007; van den Dries et al., 2012; for a meta-analysis, see van den Dries et al., 2009), showing that adopted children exhibit more indicators of insecurity or disorganized attachment than their biological counterparts, but not more (or, even less) than their peers in institutions. These findings support the value of NSSTs in diagnosing attachment problems in adopted children.

Furthermore, findings from longitudinal studies using NSSTs suggest that, besides the relative endurance of negative IWMs, adopted children do develop more positive representations of self and other over time, which become particularly noticeable in more fine-grained aspects of attachment. Hence, an added value of NSSTs is that fine-grained analyses of a child's story stem responses can reveal precise areas of attachment-related strengths and difficulties of a particular child to a level of detail not achievable with other currently used attachment measures. From the perspective of assessing adopted children's vulnerabilities in the spectrum of attachment and changes therein, fine-grained analyses of NSST responses are particularly suitable and valuable to gain insight into these children's internal models of self and others at the time of placement, which will often be “disintegrated,” reflecting “a mind where fear and aggression often predominate” (Steele et al., 2010, p. 38), and also to track developmental change as the child settles into the adoptive family. The significance of such developmental catch-up in attachment representations is attested by the consistent finding that child attachment as assessed by NSSTs—whether operationalized categorically or in a more fine-grained way7—is associated with socio-emotional developmental outcomes, such as emotion comprehension, mentalizing skills, conduct and peer problems, and prosocial behavior, a finding that is in line with extant research using other attachment measures (e.g., Stams et al., 2002; Jaffari-Bimmel et al., 2006). For clinicians and researchers working with such children, it is of paramount importance that changes in aspects of a child's internal representations that take place in small steps, visible to a good observer, even long before a shift in attachment category is discernible can be mapped systematically. Such observations may be considered as windows into the child's experience that may aid the clinician in identifying opportunities to scaffold the child's representations and regulation strategies. In this context, Robinson (2016) has developed a “report format” in which the clinician can describe how the child experiences for instance, family roles or problem solving strategies, emphasizing strengths and articulating challenges; as well as subsequent recommendations on how to support the child at home as well as in treatment. It is in this context that fine-grained analyses of NSST responses may be of added value.

Factors Influencing Adopted Children's Attachment Representations as Assessed by NSSTs

This review points to a number of promising, albeit to date not sufficiently investigated, avenues for research and/or clinical application of NSSTs with adopted children. First, although studies using a categorical approach to NSSTs have yielded inconsistent findings, studies from a more fine-grained attachment perspective suggest that adoptive parents' (mostly mothers') secure state of mind plays an important role in fostering the development of more positive IWMs in their children. However, these findings need to be interpreted with caution, as only a few studies have investigated the association between parental and child attachment representations, and none of these studies have used data-analytic procedures to warrant firm conclusions as to causality. Moreover, there is a surprising lack of studies investigating the association between paternal and child attachment representations.

Second, child and adoption characteristics such as gender and age at adoption seem to have some effect on child attachment, although specific hypotheses require further testing as findings are inconsistent across studies. For instance, whereas age at adoption yields inconsistent results, more detailed analyses, such as those of Román et al. (2012), disentangling age at adoption from variables such as time in adoptive family or pre-adoption family experience, provide promising insights into risk and protective factors for child attachment development.

Third, findings regarding the influence of child cognitive factors on attachment remain equivocal to date and thus warrant further investigation. More generally, studies using NSSTs as predictors or outcomes of adopted children's development remain relatively scarce, although such studies may yield important insights into these children's developmental trajectories.

In general, the findings of the present review should be considered preliminary, due to (a) the limited number of studies, (b) the large variety in the composition of story stem batteries as well as in coding procedures, and (c) the large variety in study designs, rendering direct comparison of results difficult. In many cases, only two or three studies included the same variables assessed at comparable time points to investigate the same research questions, so aggregate findings in the present review are few, and narrative, though not statistical, aggregation was feasible.

Promising Avenues for Research and Clinical Application of NSSTs

Based on the findings of the present review, a number of recommendations for future research and clinical applications of NSSTs may be outlined. First, the different theoretical perspectives should be explored and investigated more thoroughly, and researchers and clinicians using NSSTs should locate themselves within these theoretical paradigms in a more explicit way.

Furthermore, empirical and clinical work in the domain of attachment should be situated as categorical, dimensional and/or fine-grained, in order to aid the reader to better grasp the diversity of knowledge available in this domain. In this regard, the findings of the present review support the added value of a fine-grained perspective on (changes in) attachment over and above a categorical or dimensional approach.

Finally, with regard to adopted children's attachment representations, much more research that is designed in theoretically, empirically, and methodologically consistent ways is needed to investigate factors influencing and influenced by child attachment. The present review's finding that NSSTs to date have been applied with considerable heterogeneity in study design and investigated variables is of particular concern and should be addressed in future research in order to enable a more systematic body of knowledge to emerge. In this regard, it is important, both as researchers and as clinicians, to remember that adverse experiences are not unique to the early lives of some adopted children. Among other children in out-of-home care, such as foster children, a subgroup has been exposed to similar early adversities, making them vulnerable to at-risk or maladaptive developmental outcomes. Without discarding the possible differences between adoption and foster care policies and practices, and fully aware of the importance of considering research findings within their specific socio-cultural context, we do also advocate a more systematic body of knowledge about the developmental trajectories of children with a history of early adversity, whether adopted or in foster care or another type of out-of-home care. The findings of the present review suggest that NSSTs, in particular, may be a fruitful avenue to pursue in research on and clinical care for such vulnerable children. Yet, an exploratory search and screening of extant research, conducted at the design stage of the present review, indicated that studies in which NSST findings were reported separately for adopted and foster children were scant, rendering it, at present, nearly impossible to disentangle possible differences between these groups.

Limitations of the Review

As discussed, the present review is based on a relatively small number of studies characterized by considerable heterogeneity in theoretical perspective as well as with regard to methodological aspects. Hence, the findings about aspects of adopted children's internal reality should be considered preliminary and be interpreted with caution.

Another limitation of the present review at design level should be mentioned. The search was conducted in a relatively small number of databases, and only English-language peer-reviewed publications were considered for inclusion, to the exclusion of possible relevant publications in other languages or gray literature.

Conclusion

In sum, although NSST research with adopted children is still in its infancy in terms of the relative paucity of studies and the lack of consensus with regard to composition and coding of story stems, rendering direct comparison of results difficult, there is a relatively robust evidence base for NSSTs as a measure to assess and track developmental change in adopted children's attachment representations. In this regard, a fine-grained perspective on children's NSST responses has been argued to be particularly valuable in tailoring treatment to a particular child's needs and vulnerabilities and thus fostering development. Future research and clinical application of NSSTs in several avenues that have proven promising may further our knowledge and understanding of, and thus our ability to aid, these often vulnerable children. Based on the present review, we therefore strongly advocate a larger and importantly, more consistent body of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies using NSSTs, in order to investigate aspects of adopted children's internal reality in terms of attachment representations, affect and affect-regulation, and mentalization more in depth; as well as to track changes in these aspects to gain a better understanding of these children's experiences and to better tailor treatment.

Author Contributions

NV designed the study. DB and ET conducted the literature searches. NV and DB assessed study quality. All three authors contributed to data extraction and synthesis from the primary studies. NV and ET drafted the first version of the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the reviewers for their thoughtful and constructive comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Footnotes

1. ^ We opted to also include studies on early childhood as most NSSTs have been developed for children from 4 years of age.

2. ^ As this was not a primary focus of the present review, and owing to space limitations, we do not discuss composition of story stem batteries and NSST coding procedures in detail. Detailed information regarding these issues is available upon request.

3. ^ The global insecure category is a distilled sum of disorganized, insecure-ambivalent, and insecure-avoidant rates reported in primary studies, with the exception of Kocovska et al. (2012) who only reported percentages for secure and disorganized attachment classifications.

4. ^ This is a follow-up study of Pace and Zavattini (2011) and Pace et al. (2012), therefore only the percentages of the most recent publication are reported.

5. ^ Eight themes in Steele et al. (2007a).

6. ^ Pace et al. (2014) also coded children's responses to the MCAST to investigate attachment dimensions.

7. ^ As noted in the Results, there were no studies investigating the association between NSST attachment dimensions and developmental outcomes.

8. ^ Studies included in the present review are marked.

References

Aber, J. L., Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., and Kaplan, M. (1985). The Parent Development Interview (Unpublished manuscript).

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Bremner, J. D., Walker, J. D., Whitfield, C., Perry, B. D., et al. (2006). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood: a convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256, 174–186. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barone, L., Del Giudice, M., Fossati, A., Manaresi, F., Actis Perinetti, B., Colle, L., et al. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Manchester child attachment story task: an Italian multicentre study. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 33, 185–190. doi: 10.1177/0165025409103134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Barone, L., and Lionetti, F. (2012). Attachment and emotional understanding: a study on late-adopted pre-schoolers and their parents. Child Care Health Dev. 38, 690–696. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01296.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bick, J., and Dozier, M. (2008). “Helping foster parents change: the role of parental state of mind,” in Clinical Applications of the Adult Attachment Interview, eds H. Steele and M. Steele (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 452–470.

Google Scholar

Bosmans, G., and Kerns, K. A. (2015). Attachment in middle childhood: progress and prospects. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2015, 1–14. doi: 10.1002/cad.20100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Separation, Vol. 2. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment, 2nd Edn. Vol. 1. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bretherton, I., Oppenheim, D., Buchsbaum, H., and Emde, R. N. (1990). The MacArthur Story Stem Battery. Unpublished manuscript.

Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., and Cassidy, J. (1990). “Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship: an attachment story completion task for 3-year-olds,” in Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention, eds M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and E. M. Cummings (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 273–308.

Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., and Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Dev. Psychol. 25, 525–531. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.4.525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chisholm, K., Carter, M. C., Ames, E. W., and Morison, S. J. (1995). Attachment security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Dev. Psychopathol. 7, 283–294.

Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role of self-organization in the promotion of resilience in maltreated children. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 797–815.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D., and Valentino, K. (2006). “An ecological transactional perspective on child maltreatment: failure of the average expectable environment and its influence upon child development,” in Developmental Psychopathology: Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation, 2nd Edn. Vol. 3, eds D. Cicchetti and D. J. Cohen (New York, NY: Wiley), 129–201.

Cooke, A., Smith, D., and Booth, A. (2012). Beyond PICO: the SPIDER tool for qualitative evidence synthesis. Qual. Health Res. 22, 1435–1443. doi: 10.1177/1049732312452938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, D. (2011). Child Development: A Practitioner's Guide, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Dozier, M. (2005). Challenges of foster care. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 27–30. doi: 10.1080/14616730500039747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Allison, E. (2012). “What is mentalization? The concept and its foundations in development research,” in Minding the Child: Mentalization-Based Interventions with Children, Young People and Their Families, eds N. Midgley and I. Vrouva (Hove: Routledge), 11–34.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. J., and Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press.

Fonagy, P., and Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 1355–1381. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, C., Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1985). Adult Attachment Interview protocol. Version 7.1. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Gloger-Tippelt, G., and Kappler, G. (2016). Narratives of attachment in middle childhood: do gender, age, and risk-status matter for the quality of attachment? Attach. Hum. Dev. 18, 570–595. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2016.1194440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, J., Stanley, C., Smith, V., and Goldwyn, R. (2000). A new method of evaluating attachment representations in young school-age children: the Manchester child attachment story task. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2, 48–70. doi: 10.1080/146167300361318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Heller, S. S., Boris, N. W., Fuselier, S. H., Page, T., Koren-Karie, N., and Miron, D. (2006). Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins follow-up: from 18 months to 8 years. Attach. Hum. Dev. 8, 63–86. doi: 10.1080/14616730600585177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyvaert, M., Hannes, K., and Onghena, P. (2017). Using Mixed Methods Synthesis for Literature Reviews. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.

Heyvaert, M., Maes, B., and Onghena, P. (2013). Mixed methods research synthesis: definition, framework, and potential. Qual. Quant. 47, 659–676. doi: 10.1007/s11135-011-9538-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, J. (2007). “Understandings the child's development: using story stems to understand the legacy of past family experiences,” in Paper presented at the British Agencies for Adoption and Fostering, Conference on the Assessment Framework.

Hodges, J., Hillman, S., and Steele, M. (2007). Story Stem Assessment Profile: Coding Manual and Protocol. London: Anna Freud Centre.

*Hodges, J., and Steele, M. (2000). Effects of abuse on attachment representations: narrative assessments of abused children. J. Child Psychother. 26, 433–455. doi: 10.1080/00754170010003674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hodges, J., Steele, M., Hillman, S., Henderson, K., and Kaniuk, J. (2003). Changes in attachment representations over the first year of adoptive placement: narratives of maltreated children. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 8, 351–367. doi: 10.1177/1359104503008003006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hodges, J., Steele, M., Hillman, S., Henderson, K., and Kaniuk, J. (2005). “Change and continuity in mental representations of attachment after adoption,” in Psychological Issues in Adoption, eds D. M. Brodzinsky and J. Palacios (Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers), 93–116.

Google Scholar

*Hodges, J., Steele, M., Kaniuk, J., Hillman, S., and Asquith, K. (2009). “Narratives in assessment and research on the development of attachments in maltreated children,” in Child Psychotherapy and Research: New Approaches, Emerging Findings, eds N. Midgley, J. Anderson, E. Grainger, T. Nesic-Vuckovic, and C. Urwin (New York, NY: Routledge), 200–213.

Google Scholar

Howe, D. (1998). Patterns of Adoption: Nature, Nurture and Psychosocial Development. Oxford: Blackwell Science.

Google Scholar

Howe, D. (2001). Age at placement, adoption experience and adult adopted people's contact with their adoptive and birth mothers: an attachment perspective. Attach. Hum. Dev. 3, 222–237. doi: 10.1080/14616730110058025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hushion, K., Sherman, S. B., and Siskind, D. (2006). Understanding Adoption. Clinical Work with Adults, Children and Parents. Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson.

Google Scholar

Jaffari-Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Mooijaart, A. (2006). Social development from infancy to adolescence: longitudinal and concurrent factors in an adoption sample. Dev. Psychol. 42, 1143–1153. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joanna Briggs Institute (2011). Critical Appraisal Tools. Available online at: From: http://joannabriggs.org

Juffer, F., and Tieman, W. (2009). Being adopted. Internationally adopted children's interest and feelings. Int. Soc. Work 52, 635–647. doi: 10.1177/0020872809337682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juffer, F., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Adoptees do not lack self-esteema: a meta-analysis of studies on self-esteem of transracial, international and domestic adoptees. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1067–1083. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kocovská, E., Puckering, C., Follan, M., Smillie, M., Gorski, C., Barnes, J., et al. (2012). Neurodevelopmental problems in maltreated children referred with indiscriminate friendliness. Res. Dev. Disabil. 33, 1560–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, T. M., and Orlans, M. (2003). “Creating and repairing attachments in biological, foster, and adoptive families,” in Attachment Process in Couple and Family Therapy, eds S. M. Johnson and E. Wiffen (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 165–190.

Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pace, C. S., Cavanna, D., Velotti, P., and Cesare Zavattini, G. (2014). Attachment representations in late-adopted children: the use of narrative in the assessment of disorganisation, mentalising and coherence of mind. Adopt. Foster. 38, 255–270. doi: 10.1177/0308575914543235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pace, C. S., and Zavattini, G. C. (2011). ‘Adoption and attachment theory' the attachment models of adoptive mothers and the revision of attachment patterns of their late-adopted children. Child Care Health Dev. 37, 82–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01135.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pace, C. S., Zavattini, G. C., and D'Alessio, M. (2012). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment patterns: a short-term longitudinal pilot study using a sample of late-adopted children and their adoptive mothers. Attach. Hum. Dev. 14, 45–61. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.636658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, T. F. (2001). The social meaning of children's narratives: a review of the attachment-based narrative story stem technique. Child Adolesc. Social Work J. 18, 171–187. doi: 10.1023/a:1011006402275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Page, T. F., Heller, S. S., and Boris, N. W. (2008). A social-emotional assessment method for young children in foster and residential care: the attachment-based narrative story-stem technique. Resident. Treat. Children Youth 23, 139–162. doi: 10.1080/08865710609512721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, B. D. (2002). Childhood experience and the expression of genetic potential: what childhood neglect tells us about nature and nurture. Brain and Mind 3, 79–100. doi: 10.1023/a:1016557824657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, B. D. (2004). Maltreated Children: Experience, Brain Development, and the Next Generation. New York: W. W. Norton.

Robinson, J. (2016). “Tapping working models of family relationships in 4–7 year olds through story stem narratives in a marriage and family clinic setting, Master class lecture,” in Paper Presented at the 5th World Congress of the World Association for Infant Mental Health (Prague).

Google Scholar

Robinson, J. L. (2007). Story stem narratives with young children: moving to clinical research and practice. Attach. Hum. Dev. 9, 179–185. doi: 10.1080/14616730701453697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Román, M., Palacios, J., Moreno, C., and López, A. (2012). Attachment representations in internationally adopted children. Attach. Hum. Dev. 14, 585–600. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.727257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I: disinhibited attachment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 17–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01688.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandelowski, M., Voils, C. I., and Barroso, J. (2006). Defining and designing mixed research synthesis studies. Res. Schools 13:29.

Google Scholar

Schofield, G., and Beek, M. (2005). Providing a secure base: parenting children in long-term foster family care. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 3–26. doi: 10.1080/14616730500049019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, C., Fonagy, P., and Goodyer, I. M. (2006). Imagining your child's mind: psychosocial adjustment and mothers' ability to predict their children's attributional response styles. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 197–214. doi: 10.1348/026151005x82569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, A. (2007). Reflective parenting programs: theory and development. Psychoanal. Inq. 26, 640–657. doi: 10.1080/07351690701310698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stake, R. E. (2005). “Qualitative case studies,” in The Sage Handbook of Qualitative Research, 3rd Edn. eds N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 443–466.

Google Scholar

Stams, G. J., Juffer, F., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2002). Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: the case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Dev. Psychol. 38, 806–821. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, H., and Steele, M. (2005). “The construct of coherence as an indicator of attachment security in middle childhood. The friends and family interview,” in10 Attachment in Middle Childhood, eds K. A. Kerns and R. A. Richardson (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 137–160.

*Steele, M., Henderson, K., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Hillman, S., and Steele, H. (2007a). “In the best interests of the late-placed child: a report from attachment representations and adoption outcome study,” in Developmental Science and Psychoanalysis: Integration and Innovation, eds L. Mayes, P. Fonagy, and M. Target (London: Karnac Books), 159–182.

Google Scholar

*Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Hillman, S., and Henderson, K. A. Y. (2003). Attachment representations and adoption: associations between maternal states of mind and emotion narratives in previously maltreated children. J. Child Psychother. 29, 187–205. doi: 10.1080/0075417031000138442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., and Steele, H. (2010). Mental representation and change: developing attachment relationships in an adoption context. Psychoanal. Inq. 30, 25–40. doi: 10.1080/07351690903200135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Steele, H., Asquith, K., Hillman, S., et al. (2009). “Attachment representations and adoption outcome: on the use of narrative assessments to track the adaptation of previously maltreated children in their new families,” in International Advances in Adoption Research for Practice, eds G. M. Wrobel and E. Neil (West Sussex: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.), 193–215.

Google Scholar

Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Steele, H., D'Agostino, D., Blom, I., et al. (2007b). “Intervening with maltreated children and their adoptive families: identifying attachment-facilitative behaviors, in Attachment Theory in Clinical Work With Children: Bridging the Gap Between Research and Practice, eds D. Oppenheim, and D. F. Goldsmith (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 58–89.

Google Scholar

*Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Steele, H., Hillman, S., and Asquith, K. (2008). Forecasting outcomes in previously maltreated children, in Clinical Applications of the Adult Attachment Interview, eds H. Steele and M. Steele (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 427–451.

Google Scholar

van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). Fostering security? a meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 31, 410–421. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Alink, L. R. (2012). Infants' responsiveness, attachment, and indiscriminate friendliness after international adoption from institutions or foster care in China: application of emotional availability scales to adoptive families. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 49–64. doi: 10.1017/s0954579411000654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Juffer, F. (2006). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 2006: adoption as intervention. Meta-analytic evidence for massive catch-up and plasticity in physical, socio-emotional, and cognitive development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1228–1245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01675.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Dev. Psychopathol. 11, 225–250.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

*Vorria, P., Papaligoura, Z., Sarafidou, J., Kopakaki, M., Dunn, J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., et al. (2006). The development of adopted children after institutional care: a follow-up study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1246–1253. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01666.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adopted children, narrative story stem techniques, attachment representations, mixed-methods synthesis, tailored treatment

Citation: Tang E, Bleys D and Vliegen N (2018) Making Sense of Adopted Children's Internal Reality Using Narrative Story Stem Techniques: A Mixed-Methods Synthesis. Front. Psychol. 9:1189. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01189

Received: 29 August 2017; Accepted: 20 June 2018;
Published: 10 July 2018.

Edited by:

Markus Paulus, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany

Reviewed by:

JoAnn Robinson, University of Connecticut, United States
Louise Bunce, Oxford Brookes University, United Kingdom
Melanie Tamara Kungl, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Tang, Bleys and Vliegen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eileen Tang, eileen.tang@kuleuven.be

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.