Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 July 2018
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Parental Warmth and Hostility and Child Executive Function Problems: A Longitudinal Study of Chinese Families

  • Department of Early Childhood Education, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong

This study examined the longitudinal associations of maternal and paternal warmth and hostility with child executive function problems. Data were collected for two consecutive years from 333 kindergarten children who resided in Hong Kong, China, as well as their mothers, fathers, and class teachers. At Time 1, the average age of children was 57.73 months, and 56% of them were girls. At Time 1, mothers and fathers rated their own parenting practices with their children. At Times 1 and 2, class teachers rated children’s problems in three aspects of executive functions, including updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility. As control variables, at Time 1, parents provided information on child and family demographic factors, and children completed verbal ability tasks. Multilevel modeling revealed that controlling for child and family demographic factors, child verbal abilities, and paternal parenting practices, maternal hostility, but not maternal warmth, was linked to increases in child inhibition and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems. Moreover, paternal hostility, but not paternal warmth, was linked to increases in updating/working memory problems. Theoretically, this study highlighted the importance of considering the contributions of both mothers and fathers, and differentiating between positive and negative aspects of parenting, when examining the development of child executive functions. Practically, this study pointed to the utility of targeting maternal and paternal hostility in family intervention and community education in order to reduce child executive function problems.

Introduction

Emerging research indicates that children’s executive functions, or abilities to regulate thoughts and behaviors to achieve desirable goals (Garon et al., 2008; Best and Miller, 2010; Miyake and Friedman, 2012; Diamond, 2013), have important implications for their adjustment. Children with better executive functions, for example, are more popular among peers, exhibit fewer conduct problems, and perform better in school exams and standardized tests (Ellis et al., 2009; Chung and McBride-Chang, 2011; Jacobson et al., 2011; Chung et al., 2018). Given the importance of executive functions, researchers have been examining how such outcomes are affected by different personal (e.g., brain development and physical activity) and environmental (e.g., family and school experiences) factors (Hughes, 2011; Diamond, 2014). An emerging line of work focuses specifically on parental influences (Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014). Most of this work, however, has relied on cross-sectional data, focused on the roles of mothers, and measured either positive or negative aspects of parenting. Moreover, nearly all studies on parenting and child executive functions are based on European, Canadian, or European American samples. Although about one-fifth of the world population lives in China (United Nations, 2017), we know next to nothing about how the family may affect child executive functions in Chinese families. Grounded in the theories of attachment (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2010; Bernier et al., 2012) and social learning (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Moriguchi, 2012, 2014), the present study examined the longitudinal associations of maternal and paternal warmth and hostility with child executive function problems in a sample of kindergarten children from Hong Kong, China. We focused on early childhood, as younger children are more dependent on their caretakers for protection, nurturance, and simulation, and thus may be more susceptible to parental influences (Halgunseth, 2009).

Theoretical Perspectives on Parenting and Child Executive Functions

Executive functions refer to individuals’ abilities to override more automatic thoughts and responses, and behave in planned, goal-directed manners, especially in novel or ambiguous situations (Diamond, 2013). Executive functions involve three subcomponents (Best and Miller, 2010; Miyake and Friedman, 2012), namely updating/working memory, or the ability to hold and manipulate information in working memory to direct behaviors toward future goals, inhibition, or the ability to suppress unthinking or inappropriate thoughts and responses, and shifting/cognitive flexibility, or the ability to shift focus to the mental framework most relevant to the task at hand and choose from potentially conflicting behavioral alternatives. Although distinguishable, the three subcomponents of executive functions often operate in integrated ways to support higher order processing. According to an integrative model of executive functions (Garon et al., 2008), for example, updating/working memory, which directs attention to relevant information, inhibition, which directs attention away from irrelevant information, and shifting/cognitive flexibility, which shifts attention depending on the demand of the situation, are all central to the management of oneself, completion of tasks, and adjustment to challenges. Not surprisingly, children with better executive functions score higher on a wide range of adjustment indices, including peer competence, behavioral conduct, and academic achievement (Ellis et al., 2009; Chung and McBride-Chang, 2011; Jacobson et al., 2011; Chung et al., 2018).

Multiple theories have been used to explain how parenting may affect child executive functions. The attachment theory, for example, posits that secure relationships with caretakers may “free up” cognitive resources for self-directed exploration of the environment, which involves playing with physical objects and interacting with nonfamilial members (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2010; Bernier et al., 2012). Specifically, securely attached children, who perceive their caretakers as reliable sources of protection and support, spend fewer cognitive resources monitoring their caretakers’ availabilities and worrying about being abandoned. The cognitive resources freed up can be instead invested in self-directed exploration of the environment, resulting in additional opportunities for these children to practice their executive functions. Therefore, parenting behaviors that facilitate and hinder the formation of secure attachment may facilitate and hinder the development of child executive functions, respectively.

On the other hand, the social learning theory posits that children tend to imitate others’ behaviors, especially the behaviors of their parents, whom they are close to and dependent on (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Moriguchi, 2012, 2014). In fact, children’s tendency to imitate their parents is so strong that children may remember and reproduce their parents’ behaviors, even when these behaviors are not linked to any rewards in the first place. Once learnt, these behaviors may also be highly resistant to changes, even when met with punishment. Therefore, parenting behaviors that model good and poor self-regulation may have positive and negative impacts on the development of child executive functions, respectively.

Echoing the theories of attachment and social learning, Blair et al. (2011) emphasized the importance of considering both positive and negative aspects of parenting when studying it as a potential correlate of child executive functions. Parental warmth, which involves parental support, praises, and displays of affection and tranquility, may model good self-regulation, lead to more positive parent–child exchanges, and promote children’s executive function skills. Meanwhile, parental hostility, which involves parental rejection, reprimands, and loss of temper and control, may model poor self-regulation, lead to more negative parent–child interactions, and create children’s executive function problems. A comprehensive review on family influences on the development of child executive functions can be found in Fay-Stammbach et al. (2014).

Empirical Findings on Parenting and Child Executive Functions

Empirical findings on parenting and child executive functions are generally consistent with predictions of the theories of attachment and social learning. However, as we elaborate below, existing research has been mostly based on cross-sectional data, focused on either positive or negative aspects of parenting, and highlighted the contributions of mothers. What is lacking in the literature is longitudinal research that controls for prior levels of child executive functions, includes both parental warmth and hostility in the same analytic model, and is based on data from both mothers and fathers.

Numerous cross-sectional studies have showed that parental warmth and hostility are associated positively and negatively with child executive function skills, respectively (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Schroeder and Kelley, 2010; Hopkins et al., 2013; Linebarger et al., 2014; Sosic-Vasic et al., 2017). Longitudinal work is rarer, but several studies showed that maternal warmth, measured in infancy and toddlerhood, was positively linked to child executive function skills (Matte-Gagné et al., 2015; Meuwissen and Englund, 2016) and negatively linked to child executive function problems, measured in early childhood (Kraybill and Bell, 2013). One study further showed that both positive and negative aspects of maternal parenting, measured in infancy and toddlerhood and included in the same analytic model, were uniquely predictive of children’s overall skills of updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility, measured in early childhood (Blair et al., 2011).

Longitudinal findings controlling for prior levels of child executive functions are more mixed. For example, in Cuevas et al.’s (2014) study, maternal hostility measured when the child was 10-, 24-, and 36-month old was negatively linked to child executive functions measured when the child was 48-month old, even after controlling for child executive functions measured when the child was 36-month old. Moreover, in Devine et al.’s (2016) study, maternal warmth and hostility, measured when the child was 48-month old and included in the same analytic model, were uniquely predictive of child executive function measured when the child was 60-month old, even after controlling for child executive functions measured when the child was 48-month old. However, in Bernier et al.’s (2010) study, the association between maternal warmth (measured when the child was 18-month old) and child executive functions (measured when the child was 26-month old) became nonsignificant, after controlling for child executive functions measured when the child was 18-month old. Taken together, these findings – all of which were based on children’s overall skills of updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility – highlighted the need of additional research that examines the unique roles of parental warmth and hostility in understanding changes in child executive functions over time.

Another need of additional research concerns the roles of fathers. Despite increasing evidence indicating that paternal parenting practices are uniquely linked to child adjustment (Lam and McHale, 2012; Lam et al., 2012; Cabrera et al., 2014), only a handful of studies have examined the potential contributions of fathers to their children’s development in executive functions. For example, in one cross-sectional study (Meuwissen and Carlson, 2015), paternal warmth and hostility were linked positively and negatively to children’s overall skills of updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility, respectively. In another cross-sectional study (Lucassen et al., 2015), with both positive and negative aspects of maternal and paternal parenting being included in the same analytic model, paternal hostility (but not paternal warmth) was linked to child problems in updating/working memory and inhibition (but not shifting/cognitive flexibility). Finally, in a longitudinal study (Towe-Goodman et al., 2014), maternal and paternal warmth, measured when the child was 24-month old and included in the same analytic model, were uniquely linked to child overall skills of updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility, measured when the child was 36-month old. To our best knowledge, no studies have tested whether the warmth and hostility of both mothers and fathers, all included in the same analytic model, are uniquely linked to changes in children’s executive functions over time.

It is worth mentioning that executive functions, especially in the form of inhibition, are highly emphasized in Hong Kong (Kwong et al., 2018), as well as other Chinese communities (Chen et al., 2012). For example, Chinese parents expect their children to master inhibition as early as toddlerhood, but US parents do not expect their children to do so until early childhood (Stevenson et al., 1990; Chen et al., 1998). Also, in kindergartens, Chinese children are expected to stay more focused on tasks, comply with more instructions, and sit still for longer periods of time compared to US children (Tobin et al., 2009). Probably because of these culturally unique expectations, Chinese children mature more quickly in executive functions than do their Western counterparts (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Ellefson et al., 2017). Despite such a strong emphasis on self-regulation early on in Chinese communities, existing research on child executive functions was largely based on European, Canadian, or European American families. We know next to nothing about whether Chinese mothers’ and fathers’ warmth and hostility may affect their children’s executive functions. By examining such research questions in a sample of Chinese families, we sought to move beyond the typical focus on Western families to shed light on the role of parenting in the development of children’s executive functions in the Eastern part of the world.

The Present Study

To recap, existing research on parenting and child executive functions tends to rely on cross-sectional data, measure either positive or negative aspect of parenting, and focus on the roles of mothers. Guided by the theories of attachment (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2010; Bernier et al., 2012) and social learning (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Moriguchi, 2012, 2014), the present study addressed these gaps in the literature by linking maternal and paternal warmth and hostility (separately reported by the mother and the father when the child was 58-month old) to changes in child executive function problems over time (independently reported by the kindergarten class teacher when the child was 58- and 70-month old). To model longitudinal changes, we used child executive function problems at Time 2 as the dependent variables and included child executive function problems at Time 1 as controls (Rovine and Liu, 2012). We further controlled for child gender and age and maternal and paternal education levels, as well as child verbal abilities, as an indicator of child verbal or crystalized intelligence (Norton and Wolf, 2012; Schipolowski et al., 2014), in order to isolate the impact of parenting on child executive functions from those of child and family demographic factors (Diamond, 2013; Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014) and child general cognitive functioning (Bernier et al., 2010; Cuevas et al., 2014; Towe-Goodman et al., 2014).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

Participants were 333 children, as well as their mothers, fathers, and class teachers, from 10 kindergartens in Hong Kong. Kindergartens in Hong Kong varied highly in size, with the number of students in each kindergarten ranging from 100 to 800. To ensure that families from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds were recruited, we used a stratified sampling approach: Based on their median monthly household incomes (Census and Statistics Department, 2015), we first stratified the 18 geographic districts of Hong Kong into high, middle, and low socioeconomic strata. We then randomly called kindergartens (using publicly available contact information) until three kindergartens in each stratum agreed to recruit families for the study. Two kindergartens recruited from the high socioeconomic stratum turned out to be small in size. Therefore, we recruited one more kindergarten from that stratum to strive for a more balanced distribution of families from different socioeconomic backgrounds. We sent invitation letters and consent forms to all second-year students in the 10 kindergartens. Three hundred and thirty three families provided informed and written consent for us to collect data from the children, as well as the mothers, fathers, and class teachers. The class teachers also provided informed and written consent to participate in the study.

Data collection took place in the second semesters of the academic years of 2014–2015 (Time 1) and 2015–2016 (Time 2), which were separated by about 12 months. At Time 1, mothers and fathers separately rated their own parenting practices using self-administered questionnaires, and provided child and family demographic information. Children also completed tasks on verbal abilities with trained administrators from our research team. At Times 1 and 2, class teachers rated children’s executive function problems using self-administered questionnaires. The retention rate across Times 1 and 2 was 89%.

Maternal and paternal education levels averaged 3.12 (SD = 1.15) and 3.14 (SD = 1.21), respectively, on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (elementary school education) to 5 (postgraduate education). Fifty-six percent of children were girls (n = 185). The mean ages of children were 57.73 (SD = 4.53) and 70.07 months (SD = 4.55) at Times 1 and 2, respectively. At each time point, each parent received a supermarket coupon of HK$50 (or about US$6), and each teacher received a supermarket coupon of HK$100 (or about US$12), after completing the questionnaire. Each child received a gift of HK$5 (or about US$1) after completing the tasks. The present study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Education University of Hong Kong.

Measures

Following Foster and Martinez’s (1995) recommendations, all English measures were forward and backward translated to Chinese by two independent translators, before two local family researchers resolved the discrepancies and finalized the items. The finalized items were further reviewed, discussed, and fine-tuned in a pilot study with 20 parents and 10 teachers of kindergarten children, to ensure the clarity of the questions and instructions.

Parental warmth and hostility were measured using the 7-item warmth/acceptance and the 3-item verbal hostility subscales from the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (Robinson et al., 2001). At Time 1, on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always), mothers and fathers rated how often they praised and were affectionate with their children (e.g., “I give praises when this child is good,” “I express affection by hugging, kissing, and holding this child”), and how often they lost temper at and had conflict with their children (e.g., “I explode in anger towards this child,” “I yell or shout when this child misbehaves”). Item ratings were averaged, with higher scores indicating higher levels of warmth and verbal hostility. The reliability and validity of the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire had been evidenced in samples of Chinese parents (Wu et al., 2002; Lee E.H. et al., 2013). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alphas of maternal and paternal warmth were 0.85 and 0.84, and those of maternal and paternal verbal hostility were 0.67 and 0.68, respectively.

Child executive function problems were measured using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF; Gioia et al., 2003; Sherman and Brooks, 2010). We opted to use the BRIEF, as it had been the most commonly used rating scale of executive functions in the literature (Toplak et al., 2012). It included five subscales, including the 10-item emotion regulation, 16-item inhibitory control, 10-item shifting, 17-item working memory, and 10-item planning and organizing subscales. At Times 1 and 2, on a 3-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 3 (often), class teachers rated problems that children had in modulating their emotional responses, resisting impulses, and stopping their behaviors at the appropriate time, moving freely from one situation or activity to another, holding information in mind, and managing current and future-oriented task demands. Item ratings were averaged, with higher scores indicating higher levels of executive function problems.

The five subscales of the BRIEF could be summarized as three composite scores, namely emergent metacognition (composed of the working memory and planning and organizing subscales, indicating the ability to initiate, decide, plan, and implement future-oriented problem solving), inhibitory self-control (composed of the emotional control and inhibition subscales, indicating the ability to modulate emotions and responses through inhibition), and flexibility (composed of the emotional control and shifting subscales, indicating the ability to move flexibly among behaviors), which roughly captured the three aforementioned subcomponents of executive functions (i.e., updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility; Lucassen et al., 2015; Garon et al., 2016; Skogan et al., 2016). The reliability and validity of the BRIEF had been evidenced in samples of Chinese children (Chan et al., 2009; Qian et al., 2010). In the present study, at Time 1, the Cronbach’s alphas of emotion regulation, inhibitory control, shifting, working memory, and planning and organizing were 0.89, 0.92, 0.90, 0.95, and 0.90, respectively. At Time 2, the Cronbach’s alphas of emotion regulation, inhibitory control, shifting, working memory, and planning and organizing were 0.92, 0.95, 0.93, 0.96, and 0.94, respectively.

Child verbal abilities were measured using a task on rapid automatized naming (Shu et al., 2006). Rapid automatized naming, or the ability to name as fast as possible highly familiar visual stimuli, such as digits, letters, colors, and objects, has been consistently linked to verbal abilities in different cultures (Norton and Wolf, 2012). At Time 1, children were presented with five rows of five digits printed in random order on a piece of paper (e.g., 2, 4, 6, 7, and 9). Children were then asked to read the digits aloud as fast as possible. Each child did the task twice, and the mean time (in seconds) to complete the two trials was computed. Lower scores indicated higher levels of verbal ability. The reliability and validity of the task had been evidenced in samples of Chinese children (Chan et al., 2006; Yeung et al., 2013). In the present study, the test–retest reliability was 0.82.

Other control variables, including child gender and age and maternal and paternal education levels were provided by mothers and fathers at Time 1.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of all major variables. Table 2 presents the correlations among them. Focusing on the longitudinal associations between parenting and child executive functions, maternal warmth at Time 1 was negatively correlated with child updating/working memory problems at Time 2. Maternal hostility at Time 1 was positively correlated with child updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems at Time 2. Paternal warmth at Time 1 was negatively correlated with child updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems at Time 2. Paternal hostility at Time 1 was positively correlated with child updating/working memory problems at Time 2. It is worth noting that child updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems were strongly and positively correlated with one another at both time points. They also showed moderate stability across Times 1 and 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of all key variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Pearson correlations among all key variables.

Before conducting the main analyses, we examined potential attrition bias by comparing families that provided data at both time points versus families that dropped out after Time 1 (Miller and Wright, 1995). Independent sample t-tests indicated that, at Time 1, the two groups did not differ in maternal or paternal warmth or hostility, or child updating/working memory, inhibition, or shifting/cognitive flexibility problems (ts = -0.15–1.07; n.s.). With respect to the control variables, independent sample t- and χ2 tests indicated that, at Time 1, the two groups did not differ in child age or verbal abilities (ts = 0.32 and 0.98, respectively; n.s.), or child gender composition (χ2 = 1.00; n.s.). The two groups did differ in maternal (t = -2.19; p < 0.05) and paternal (t = -2.42; p < 0.05) education levels, suggesting that families that dropped out after Time 1 had more educated parents. Therefore, as we detail below, full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to correct for potential biases in parameter and standard error estimation in our analyses (Schafer, 1997).

Main Analyses

Using SAS 9.3, we ran separate multilevel models for child updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems at Time 2. In many ways, multilevel models are similar to the more commonly used, least-square multiple regression models (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002; Bickel, 2007). But, one major difference lies in their assumptions about the dependence of the individual cases being analyzed: Multiple regression models assume the cases to be unrelated to one another in terms of the dependent variables, whereas multilevel models do not. Given that the same teacher provided ratings on the dependent variables for multiple children in our sample, the case independence assumption of multiple regression models was unlikely to hold. Therefore, we analyzed our data using multilevel models, which allowed the cases to be related to one another by specifying a correlation matrix among the error residuals. As noted, FIML, which treats the mean and variance of the observed cases as parameters, and estimates particular parametric values that would make the overall results the most probable (Schafer, 1997), was used to accommodate our missing data. FIML, along with multiple imputations, is often recommended as the best ways of dealing with nonrandom missing data in multiple regression models (Newman, 2003; Schlomer et al., 2010). However, recent simulation studies have indicated that, in multilevel models with nonrandom missing data, FIML is more effective than multiple imputations in correctly estimating standard errors (Larsen, 2011; Shin et al., 2017).

To examine the unique impact of mothers and fathers, we included maternal and paternal warmth and hostility at Time 1 in the same analytic models. Moreover, to model the changes in child executive functions over time, we controlled for corresponding child executive function problems at Time 1 (Rovine and Liu, 2012). Finally, to rule out other child and family factors as alternative explanations, we controlled for child gender, age, and verbal abilities, and maternal and paternal education levels. Appendix presents the equations of our multilevel models. Figure 1 shows the path diagram of the analytic models.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Path diagram representing the analytic model of child executive function problems.

Table 3 presents the parameter coefficients of our multilevel models. Like the unstandardized coefficients in multiple regression models (B), the gamma coefficients in multilevel models (γ) indicate how many units of the dependent variable will change per one unit increase in the predictor variables. Our results indicated that maternal hostility was uniquely linked to eases in child inhibition problems and increases in child shifting/cognitive flexibility problems. In other words, children exposed to more hostility from their mothers showed more inhibition and child shifting/cognitive flexibility problems in the following year. Moreover, paternal hostility was uniquely linked to increases in child working memory problems, meaning that children exposed to more hostility from their fathers showed more updating/working memory problems in the following year. Worth mentioning is that prior levels of executive function problems were significant and positive predictors of all three outcome measures. Overall, the models explained 18, 29, and 10% variance in updating/working memory, inhibition, and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems, respectively, representing moderate to large effect sizes (Cohen, 1988).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Gamma coefficients (γ) and standard errors (SE) of multilevel models of child executive function problems.

Discussion

Despite increasing understanding of parental influences on child executive functions (Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014), prior research on this topic has rarely controlled for prior levels of child executive functions, included both positive and negative aspects of parenting practices in the same analytic model, and collected data from both mothers and fathers. Moreover, although some 20% of the world population lives in China (United Nations, 2017) and Chinese communities place particular emphasis on child self-regulation (Chen et al., 2012; Kwong et al., 2018), we know next to nothing about whether Chinese parents’ parenting practices may affect the executive functions of their children. This study was the first to examine how mothers’ and fathers’ warmth and hostility were linked to changes in their children’s executive function problems in Chinese families. Partially consistent with our expectations, maternal hostility was linked to increases in child inhibition and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems, and paternal hostility was linked to increases in child updating/working memory problems. On a theoretical level, our findings highlighted the importance of considering the contributions of both mothers and fathers, and differentiating between positive and negative aspects of parenting, in understanding the development of child executive functions. On a practical level, our findings pointed to the utility of targeting maternal and paternal hostility in family intervention and community education in order to reduce child executive function problems.

The Unique Role of Maternal and Paternal Hostility

The attachment theory posits that optimal parenting allows children to explore their environments and practice their executive functions, without having to constantly monitor their parents’ availabilities and worry that their parents would abandon them (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2010; Bernier et al., 2012). On the other hand, the social learning theory posits that children tend to observe, remember, and reproduce their parents’ behaviors, and that children’s positive and negative development in executive functions may be the result of children’s internalization of their parents’ modeling of good and poor self-regulation, respectively (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Moriguchi, 2012, 2014). In support of these theories, and previous studies linking maternal (Blair et al., 2011; Kraybill and Bell, 2013; Matte-Gagné et al., 2015; Meuwissen and Englund, 2016) and paternal (Towe-Goodman et al., 2014) warmth in infancy and toddlerhood to child executive function skills in early childhood, our univariate analyses demonstrated that parental warmth and hostility were negatively and positively associated with child executive function problems in the following year, respectively. More importantly, expanding on prior work showing that maternal hostility was linked to decreases in child overall skills in executive functions (Cuevas et al., 2014; Devine et al., 2016), our multivariate analyses demonstrated that, controlling for maternal warmth and paternal parenting practices, maternal hostility was uniquely linked to increases in child inhibition and shifting/cognitive flexibility problems. Moreover, controlling for paternal warmth and maternal parenting practices, paternal hostility was uniquely linked to increases in child updating/working memory problems. It is worth mentioning that, by linking parent-reported parenting practices to teacher-reported child outcomes, we addressed some biases due to common method variance (Podsakoff et al., 2003). Furthermore, by controlling for child gender, age, and verbal abilities (as an indicator of general cognitive functioning; Norton and Wolf, 2012; Schipolowski et al., 2014), and maternal and paternal education levels, we ruled out some child and family factors as alternative explanations of our findings. The internal validity of our findings was thus high.

There may be at least two reasons why parental hostility, but not warmth, was uniquely linked to our outcome measures. First, our measure of child executive functions focused on deficiency rather than competence (Gioia et al., 2003; Sherman and Brooks, 2010). Emerging research indicates that the absence of problems does not necessarily indicate the presence of skills, and that child deficiency and skills may be differentially related to family negativity and positivity (Golden and Lashley, 2014; Shulman, 2016). In order to test whether positive and negative aspects of parenting are more closely related to child executive function skills and problems, respectively, future studies should measure both positive and negative aspects of parenting, and both deficiency and competence in child executive functions, to examine the unique associations among them. Second, evidence exists that negative social interactions may be more impactful than positive ones. Research on marital relationships, for example, has established that it takes five acts of spousal love in order to counteract the negative impact of one act of spousal hostility (Gottman, 1993; Holman and Jarvis, 2003). Recent work on parent–child relationships also highlights the importance of maintaining of a high ratio between positive versus negative behaviors of parents toward their children (Zemp et al., 2014). Future research that uses behavioral observations to measure the relative frequency of parental warmth versus hostility is needed to examine the differential impact of positive and negative parenting on child executive functions. More generally, however, our findings highlighted the importance of testing the unique impact of positive and negative aspects of parenting in studying the development of child executive functions.

At present, it is hard to produce a definitive explanation as to why maternal and paternal hostility were linked to changes in different executive function problems. However, one possible explanation is that the mother–child relationship may represent a more potent socialization context than the father–child relationship, especially in the early years. Despite the increasing involvement of fathers in childcare in most industrialized societies, mothers continue to spend more time with their children than do fathers (Parke and Buriel, 2006; Lam et al., 2012). Therefore, maternal influences on child development may be more pervasive (Grossmann et al., 2008). In fact, consistent with such views, our bivariate analyses indicated that mothers’ hostility was linked to all three executive function problems in their children, but that fathers’ hostility was only linked to updating/working memory problems in their children. Future researchers should examine whether the extent of parental involvement may moderate the link between parenting and child executive functions.

Another possible explanation is that, regardless of how much time they spend on childcare, mothers and fathers tend to do different things with their children. Data from both the United States (Roeters and Gracia, 2016) and Hong Kong (Kwok et al., 2013) indicate that mothers are more involved in discipline and day-to-day care taking, but that fathers are more involved in play and leisure activities. When parents, often mothers, hostilely ask their children to stop doing something, or to move from one situation or activity to another, their children’s inhibition (i.e., abilities to suppress unthinking or inappropriate thoughts and responses) and shifting/cognitive flexibility (i.e., abilities to shift focus to the most relevant mental framework) may be more affected. On the other hand, when parents, often fathers, express hostility when playing ball games and completing picture puzzles with their children, their children’s updating/working memory (i.e., abilities to hold and manipulate information to achieve goals) may be more affected. As our measure of parental warmth and hostility did not tap onto the social contexts in which these parenting practices had occurred, our data did not allow us to examine these hypotheses. Further studies should test whether the extents of parental involvement in different aspects of child lives, such as discipline versus leisure, may moderate the impact of parenting on child executive functions. On a more general level, however, our findings highlighted the importance of considering the contributions of both mothers and fathers to the development of child executive functions.

Although not the focus of our study, the three subcomponents of executive functions were highly correlated with one another, providing some support to the view that different aspects of executive functions operate in integrated ways to support higher order processing (Garon et al., 2008). In fact, the latent structure of executive functions in early childhood remains an unsolved question in the literature (Lee K. et al., 2013; Chevalier, 2014; Spiegel et al., 2017). However, given that the same teacher provided ratings for multiple children in our study and that the number of clusters (i.e., teachers) was smaller than the number of items in the BRIEF, our data did not allow for a multilevel factor analysis on the outcome measure (Reise et al., 2005; Wright, 2017). Future studies seeking to examine the latent structure of child executive functions should ensure a sufficient case-to-variable ratio, on all levels of analysis, when collecting data.

The practical implications of our findings are nonetheless clear: Through family intervention and community education, mothers and fathers should be informed of the potential negative impact of hostility on their children’s development. Mothers and fathers should also learn how to control angry feelings toward and avert unrestrained conflict with their children. Further, practitioners may consider such methods as mindfulness training (Meppelink et al., 2016) and behavioral coaching (Duncombe et al., 2016) to help mothers and fathers to deal with their hostility when interacting with their children.

Limitations and Conclusions

This study had several limitations. First, despite our use of longitudinal data, efforts to address common method variance, and consideration of multiple confounding variables, our correlational design limited our ability to draw conclusive remarks on casual relationships. Intervention research that uses randomized experimental designs to manipulate parenting practices and measure subsequent child outcomes is needed to confirm the causal links between parenting and child executive functions. Second, although our sample included families from a wide range of socioeconomic backgrounds, it was not representative of all families with kindergarten children from Hong Kong. Importantly, we did not collect information about whether the participating children had been diagnosed with special needs. Further studies should recruit probabilistic, representative samples, with children with special needs being oversampled, to test the generalizability of our findings to the larger population, as well as to children with special needs. Third, a general lack of studies on parenting and child executive functions with Eastern samples motivated us to conduct ours with Chinese families. More research with non-Western samples is needed. Importantly, although our findings were consistent with some prior work based on European, Canadian, and European American families (Cuevas et al., 2014; Devine et al., 2016), it remains unclear if the strength of the relationship between parenting and child executive functions may vary across Eastern and Western communities. It awaits further investigation to use cultural comparative designs to test culture and ethnicity as potential moderators in understanding parental influences on child executive functions (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Ellefson et al., 2017).

Fourth, our analytic models only explained modest to moderate amounts of variance in the outcome measures. This may not be surprising, given prior research showing that child executive functions vary as a function of numerous personal and environmental factors (Cohen, 1988), including brain development, physical fitness, school environments, other family processes (Hughes, 2011; Diamond, 2014; Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014). Additional research should be directed at examining how different personal and environmental factors may jointly affect child executive functions. Finally, our measure of child executive functions was solely based on teachers’ ratings. Since both parents and teachers place great importance on executive functions early on in Chinese communities (Stevenson et al., 1990; Chen et al., 1998; Tobin et al., 2009), their ratings might be correlated due to their own experiences of cultural socialization. In fact, rating measures of executive functions aim more to assess children’s success in goal pursuit in unstructured conditions, and thus are quite different from the more commonly used, performance-based measures, which aim more to assess children’s efficiency in cognitive abilities (Toplak et al., 2012). More generally, considering that teachers’ and parents’ reports, naturalistic and laboratory observations, and structured executive function tasks each provide unique information about children’s executive functions (Garon et al., 2008; Diamond, 2013), future investigators should use multiple methods and multiple sources of information to more comprehensively capture the construct of child executive functions.

In the face of these limitations, our study had important theoretical and practical implications. Theoretically, our findings highlighted the importance of considering both positive and negative aspects of maternal and paternal parenting in understanding the development of child executive functions. Practically, our findings pointed to the utility of helping both mothers and fathers to control their temper at and manage their conflict with their children in order to reduce their children’s executive function problems.

Ethics Statement

The present study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Education University of Hong Kong.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the completion of this manuscript by designing the study, collecting and analyzing the data, and writing up the findings.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from Research Grants Council, University Grants Committee, Hong Kong, China, to CBL (ECS 28401714).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., and Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: a closer look at the caregiving environment. Dev. Sci. 15, 12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, R. (2007). Multilevel Analysis for Applied Research: It’s Just Regression!. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., et al. (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Dev. 82, 1970–1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Fitzgerald, H. E., Bradley, R. H., and Roggman, L. (2014). The ecology of father-child relationships: an expanded model. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 6, 336–354. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Census and Statistics Department (2015). Population and Household Statistics 2014. Hong Kong: Census and Statistics Department.

Chan, A. S., Cheung, M. C., Han, Y. M., Sze, S. L., Leung, W. W., Man, H. S., et al. (2009). Executive function deficits and neural discordance in children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1107–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., and Chung, K. K. H. (2006). Exploring the reading–writing connection in Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Read. Writ. 19, 543–561. doi: 10.1007/s11145-006-9008-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., and Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Dev. Psychol. 34, 677–686. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Yang, F., and Fu, R. (2012). “Culture and temperament,” in Handbook of Temperament, eds M. Zentner and R. L. Shiner (New York, NY: Guilford), 462–478.

Google Scholar

Chevalier, N. (2014). The development of executive function: toward more optimal coordination of control with age. Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 239–244. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, K. K. H., Lam, C. B., and Cheung, K. C. (2018). Visuomotor integration and executive functioning are uniquely linked to Chinese word reading and writing in kindergarten children. Read. Writ. 31, 155–171. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-9779-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, K. K. H., and McBride-Chang, C. (2011). Executive functioning skills uniquely predict Chinese word reading. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 909–921. doi: 10.1037/a0024744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analyses for the Social Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cuevas, K., Deater-Deckard, K., Kim-Spoon, J., Watson, A. J., Morasch, K. C., and Bell, M. A. (2014). What’s mom got to do with it? Contributions of maternal executive function and caregiving to the development of executive function across early childhood. Dev. Sci. 17, 224–238. doi: 10.1111/desc.12073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, R. T., Bignardi, G., and Hughes, C. (2016). Executive function mediates the relations between parental behaviors and children’s early academic ability. Front. Psychol. 7:1902. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2014). Want to optimize executive functions and academic outcomes? Simple, just nourish the human spirit. Minn. Symp. Child Psychol. 37, 205–232. doi: 10.1002/9781118732373.ch7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncombe, M. E., Havighurst, S. S., Kehoe, C. E., Holland, K. A., Frankling, E. J., and Stargatt, R. (2016). Comparing an emotion-and a behavior-focused parenting program as part of a multsystemic intervention for child conduct problems. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 45, 320–334. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.963855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellefson, M. R., Ng, F. F. Y., Wang, Q., and Hughes, C. (2017). Efficiency of executive function: a two-generation cross-cultural comparison of samples from Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Psychol. Sci. 28, 555–566. doi: 10.1177/0956797616687812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, M. L., Weiss, B., and Lochman, J. E. (2009). Executive functions in children: associations with aggressive behavior and appraisal processing. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 945–956. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9321-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., and Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: a review. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 258–264. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, S. L., and Martinez, C. R. Jr. (1995). Ethnicity: conceptual and methodological issues in child clinical research. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 24, 214–226. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2402_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., and Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 134, 31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garon, N. M., Piccinin, C., and Smith, I. M. (2016). Does the BRIEF-P predict specific executive function components in preschoolers? Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 5, 110–118. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2014.1002923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gioia, G., Espy, K. A., and Isquith, P. K. (2003). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Preschool Version (BRIEF-P). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Golden, C. J., and Lashley, L. (2014). “Neuropsychological forensic evaluations,” in Forensic Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Violent Offender, eds V. B. En and V. Van Hasselt (New York, NY: Springer International Publishing), 3–31. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-04792-8_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M. (1993). A theory of marital dissolution and stability. J. Fam. Psychol. 7, 57–75. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.7.1.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Kindler, H., and Zimmermann, P. (2008). “A wider view of attachment and exploration: the influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 857–879.

Google Scholar

Halgunseth, L. (2009). Family engagement, diverse families, and early childhood education programs: an integrated review of the literature. Young Child. 64, 56–58.

Google Scholar

Holman, T. B., and Jarvis, M. O. (2003). Hostile, volatile, avoiding, and validating couple-conflict types: an investigation of Gottman’s couple-conflict types. Pers. Relatsh. 10, 267–282. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopkins, J., Lavigne, J. V., Gouze, K. R., LeBailly, S. A., and Bryant, F. B. (2013). Multi-domain models of risk factors for depression and anxiety symptoms in preschoolers: evidence for common and specific factors. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 705–722. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9723-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C. (2011). Changes and challenges in 20 years of research into the development of executive functions. Infant Child Dev. 20, 251–271. doi: 10.1002/icd.736

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C. H., and Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 123, 35–50. doi: 10.1002/cd.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, L. A., Williford, A. P., and Pianta, R. C. (2011). The role of executive function in children’s competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychol. 17, 255–280. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.535654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraybill, J. H., and Bell, M. A. (2013). Infancy predictors of preschool and post-kindergarten executive function. Dev. Psychobiol. 55, 530–538. doi: 10.1002/dev.21057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwok, S. Y., Ling, C. C., Leung, C. L., and Li, J. C. (2013). Fathering self-efficacy, marital satisfaction and father involvement in Hong Kong. J. Child Fam. Stud. 22, 1051–1060. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9666-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwong, S. C., Lam, C. B., Li, X., Chung, K. K. H., Cheung, R. Y. M., and Leung, M. (2018). Fit in but stand out: a qualitative study of parents’ and teachers’ conceptualization of child socioemotional competence. Early Child. Res. Q. 44, 275–287. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, C. B., and McHale, S. M. (2012). Developmental patterns and family predictors of adolescent weight concerns: a replication and extension. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 524–530. doi: 10.1002/eat.20974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, C. B., McHale, S. M., and Crouter, A. C. (2012). Parent-child shared time from middle childhood to late adolescence: developmental course and adjustment correlates. Child Dev. 83, 2089–2103. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01826.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, R. (2011). Missing data imputation versus full information maximum likelihood with second-level dependencies. Struct. Equ. Model. 18, 649–662. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.607721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, E. H., Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., and Wang, Y. (2013). Bidirectional relations between temperament and parenting styles in Chinese children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 37, 57–67. doi: 10.1177/0165025412460795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K., Bull, R., and Ho, R. M. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Dev. 84, 1933–1953. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linebarger, D. L., Barr, R., Lapierre, M. A., and Piotrowski, J. T. (2014). Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children’s executive functioning. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 35, 367–377. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucassen, N., Kok, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Jaddoe, V. W., Hofman, A., et al. (2015). Executive functions in early childhood: the role of maternal and paternal parenting practices. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 33, 489–505. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matte-Gagné, C., Bernier, A., and Lalonde, G. (2015). Stability in maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 2610–2619. doi: 10.1007/s10826-014-0063-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meppelink, R., de Bruin, E. I., Wanders-Mulder, F. H., Vennik, C. J., and Bögels, S. M. (2016). Mindful parenting training in child psychiatric settings: heightened parental mindfulness reduces parents’ and children’s psychopathology. Mindfulness 7, 680–689. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0504-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meuwissen, A. S., and Carlson, S. M. (2015). Fathers matter: the role of father parenting in preschoolers’ executive function development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 140, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meuwissen, A. S., and Englund, M. M. (2016). Executive function in at-risk children: importance of father-figure support and mother parenting. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 44, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, R. B., and Wright, D. W. (1995). Detecting and correcting attrition bias in longitudinal family research. J. Marriage Fam. 57, 921–929. doi: 10.2307/353412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y. (2012). The effect of social observation on children’s inhibitory control. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 113, 248–258. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriguchi, Y. (2014). The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: a brief review. Front. Psychol. 5:388. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, D. A. (2003). Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing data: a simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organ. Res. Methods 6, 328–362. doi: 10.1177/1094428103254673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, E. S., and Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 427–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parke, R. D., and Buriel, R. (2006). “Socialization in the family: ethnic and ecological perspectives,” in Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 6th Edn, Vol. 3, ed. N. Eisenberg (New York, NY: Wiley), 429–504.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qian, Y., Shuai, L., Cao, Q., Chan, R. C., and Wang, Y. (2010). Do executive function deficits differentiate between children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and ADHD comorbid with oppositional defiant disorder? A cross-cultural study using performance-based tests and the behavior rating inventory of executive function. Clin. Neuropsychol. 24, 793–810. doi: 10.1080/13854041003749342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raudenbush, S. W., and Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Reise, S. P., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., and Kim, K. (2005). An illustration of multilevel factor analysis. J. Pers. Assess. 84, 126–136. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8402_02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., and Hart, C. H. (2001). “The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire (PSDQ),” in Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques: Instruments and Index, Vol. 3, eds B. F. Perlmutter, J. Touliatos, and G. W. Holden (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 319–321.

Google Scholar

Roeters, A., and Gracia, P. (2016). Child care time, parents’ well-being, and gender: evidence from the American Time Use Survey. J. Child Fam. Stud. 25, 2469–2479. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0416-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovine, M., and Liu, S. (2012). “Structural equation modeling approaches to longitudinal data,” in Longitudinal Data Analysis: A Practical Guide for Researchers in Aging, Health, and Social Sciences, eds J. T. Newsom, R. N. Jones, and S. M. Hofer (New York, NY: Routledge), 243–270.

Google Scholar

Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., and Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: a comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychol. Sci. 17, 74–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data. London: CRC press. doi: 10.1201/9781439821862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schipolowski, S., Wilhelm, O., and Schroeders, U. (2014). On the nature of crystallized intelligence: the relationship between verbal ability and factual knowledge. Intelligence 46, 156–168. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., and Card, N. O. (2010). Best practices for missing data management in counseling psychiatry. J. Couns. Psychol. 57, 1–10. doi: 10.1037/a0018082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, V. M., and Kelley, M. L. (2010). Family environment and parent-child relationships as related to executive functioning in children. Early Child Dev. Care 180, 1285–1298. doi: 10.1080/03004430902981512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaver, P. R., and Mikulincer, M. (2010). New directions in attachment theory and research. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 27, 163–172. doi: 10.1177/0265407509360899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, E. M., and Brooks, B. L. (2010). Behavior rating inventory of executive function–preschool version (BRIEF-P): test review and clinical guidelines for use. Child Neuropsychol. 16, 503–519. doi: 10.1080/09297041003679344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, T., Davison, M. L., and Long, J. D. (2017). Maximum likelihood versus multiple imputation for missing data in small longitudinal samples with nonnormality. Psychol. Methods 22, 426–449. doi: 10.1037/met0000094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., and Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 122–133. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, C. (2016). Research and Practice in Infant and Early Childhood Mental Health. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing, doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-31181-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skogan, A. H., Egeland, J., Zeiner, P., Øvergaard, K. R., Oerbeck, B., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., et al. (2016). Factor structure of the behavior rating inventory of executive functions (BRIEF-P) at age three years. Child Neuropsychol. 22, 472–492. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.992401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosic-Vasic, Z., Kröner, J., Schneider, S., Vasic, N., Spitzer, M., and Streb, J. (2017). The association between parenting behavior and executive functioning in children and young adolescents. Front. Psychol. 8:472. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, J. A., Lonigan, C. J., and Phillips, B. M. (2017). Factor structure and utility of the behavior rating inventory of executive function–preschool version. Psychol. Assess. 29, 172–185. doi: 10.1037/pas0000324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., Chen, C., Stigler, J. W., Hsu, C., and Kitamura, S. (1990). Contexts of achievement: a study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 55, 1–123. doi: 10.2307/1166090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobin, J., Hsueh, Y., and Karasawa, M. (2009). Preschool in Three Cultures Revisited: China, Japan, and the United States. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. (2012). Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Towe-Goodman, N. R., Willoughby, M., Blair, C., Gustafsson, H. C., Mills-Koonce, W. R., and Cox, M. J. (2014). Fathers’ sensitive parenting and the development of early executive functioning. J. Fam. Psychol. 28, 867–876. doi: 10.1037/a0038128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations (2017). World Population Prospects 2017. Available at: https://esa.un.org/unpd/wpp/Download/Standard/Population/

Wright, A. G. C. (2017). The current state and future of factor analysis in personality disorder research. Pers. Disord. 8, 14–25. doi: 10.1037/per0000216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, P., Robinson, C. C., Yang, C., Hart, C. H., Olsen, S. F., Porter, C. L., et al. (2002). Similarities and differences in mothers’ parenting of preschoolers in China and the United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 481–491. doi: 10.1080/01650250143000436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, P. S., Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W. O., Chung, K. K. H., and Wong, Y. K. (2013). A model of reading comprehension in Chinese elementary school children. Learn. Individ. Differ. 25, 55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zemp, M., Merrilees, C. E., and Bodenmann, G. (2014). How much positivity is needed to buffer the impact of parental negativity on children? Fam. Relat. 63, 602–615. doi: 10.1111/fare.12091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Equations for the Multilevel Model of Teachers’ Ratings of Child Executive Function Problems

Level 1 (Child-Level) Equation

Child Executive Function Problemsij = β0j + β1j × Maternal Warmth + β2j × Maternal Hostility + β3j × Paternal Warmth + β4j × Paternal Hostility + β5j × Prior Executive Function Problems + β6j × Child Gender + β7j × Child Age + β8j × Child Verbal Abilities + β9j × Maternal Education + β10j × Paternal Education + rij.

Level 2 (Teacher-Level) Equations

β0j = γ00+ u0jβ1j = γ10.β2j = γ20.β3j = γ30.β4j = γ40.β5j = γ50.β6j = γ60.β7j = γ70.β9j = γ90.β10j = γ100.

Keywords: Chinese families, early childhood, executive functions, longitudinal, parenting

Citation: Lam CB, Chung KKH and Li X (2018) Parental Warmth and Hostility and Child Executive Function Problems: A Longitudinal Study of Chinese Families. Front. Psychol. 9:1063. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01063

Received: 18 September 2017; Accepted: 06 June 2018;
Published: 04 July 2018.

Edited by:

Natasha Kirkham, Birkbeck University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Jeffrey Coldren, Youngstown State University, United States
Ora Oudgenoeg-Paz, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2018 Lam, Chung and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chun Bun Lam, ianlam@eduhk.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.