Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 August 2015
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Interoception, Contemplative Practice, and Health View all 27 articles

The relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait. The Self-Awareness Questionnaire in healthy subjects

  • 1Laboratory of Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, Second University of Naples, Caserta, Italy
  • 2Hermitage Capodimonte, Napoli, Italy

Interoception is the basic process enabling evaluation of one's own bodily states. Several previous studies suggested that altered interoception might be related to disorders in the ability to perceive and express emotions, i.e., alexithymia, and to defects in perceiving and describing one's own health status, i.e., hypochondriasis. The main aim of the present study was to investigate the relationships between alexithymic trait and interoceptive abilities evaluated by the “Self-Awareness Questionnaire” (SAQ), a novel self-report tool for assessing interoceptive awareness. Two hundred and fifty healthy subjects completed the SAQ, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 items (TAS-20), and a questionnaire to assess hypochondriasis, the Illness Attitude Scale (IAS). The SAQ showed a two-factor structure, with good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.88). We observed significant direct correlations between SAQ, TAS-20 and two of its subscales, and the IAS. Regression analysis confirmed that the difficulty in identifying and expressing emotions is significantly related with awareness for one's own interoceptive feelings and with a tendency to misinterpret and amplify bodily sensations. From a clinical point of view, the assessment of interoceptive awareness by the SAQ could be pivotal in evaluating several psychopathological conditions, such as the somatoform disorders.

Introduction

Interoception is the basic process collecting information coming from one's own body, such as heartbeat, hunger, thirst, breathing, or visceral sensations. Such information is less distinct than that provided by “exteroceptive” somatosensory systems (e.g., touch, or skin temperature), but contributes to maintain homeostasis (Craig, 2003), and allows the brain to build a sense of one's own physical condition and to answer questions such as “how do you feel now?” (Craig, 2009).

The relationships between interoception and emotion are still under debate. Classic and modern theories of emotion maintain that bodily states contribute to or are essential for emotional experience (James, 1884; Lange, 1885; Bard, 1928; Cannon, 1932; Lane and Schwartz, 1987). Several studies using heartbeat perception tasks demonstrated a positive association between interoception and intensity of emotional experience (e.g., Schandry, 1981; Jones, 1994; Wiens et al., 2000; Barrett et al., 2004; Critchley et al., 2004; Wiens, 2005; Herbert et al., 2007; Pollatos et al., 2007a,b). Thus, according to Craig (2004, 2009), individual differences in emotional awareness may be directly related to individual differences in interoception. Similarly, Pollatos et al. (2005) and Dunn et al. (2010) reported that individuals with high sensitivity to interoceptive signals showed higher arousal in response to emotional visual stimuli.

The close relationships between interoception and emotional awareness have recently been supported by a neurofunctional study. Zaki et al. (2012) demonstrated that the anterior insular cortex, which is considered essential for both interoception monitoring and emotional processing (Craig, 2004, 2009; Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2007a), was activated during tasks in which healthy subjects were required to monitor their own heartbeat or to assess their own emotional experience after viewing videos of people recounting emotional stories. On this basis, the anterior insular cortex has been regarded as “a convergence zone” of interoceptive and emotional awareness (Zaki et al., 2012; see also Lamm and Singer, 2010). Enhanced activation in a network including the anterior insular cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and somatosensory cortex has been reported in subjects with high interoceptive awareness (Critchley et al., 2004).

Emotional awareness and processing of interoceptive signals are thought to be impaired in alexithymia (Lane et al., 1998, 2000; Craig, 2004), defined by Sifneos (1973) as a reduced ability to identify and describe one's own emotions in patients with psychosomatic diseases. Alexithymia has been hypothesized to be associated with altered activation and morphology in the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insular cortex (Lane et al., 1998, 2000; Berthoz et al., 2002; Kano et al., 2003; Borsci et al., 2009). Consistent with such findings, Herbert et al. (2011) demonstrated that the ability to process interoceptive signals (assessed on a heartbeat perception task) is negatively associated with all aspects of alexithymia.

Available studies on the relationships between alexithymia and interoception, however, did not take into account the distinct facets of this complex construct. In this respect, Terasawa et al. (2013) underscored that tasks assessing heartbeat detection (e.g., Schandry, 1981) provide a measure of interoceptive “sensitivity,” i.e., of the accuracy in objective tests of bodily sensations, but are not suitable to investigate other aspects of interoception, such as interoceptive “sensibility” and interoceptive “awareness.” Interoceptive “sensibility” would express one's own tendency to be focused on internal states, whereas interoceptive “awareness” (IA) would imply cognitive appreciation of interoceptive sensations, and the ability of expressing bodily feelings. Interoceptive “sensibility” and IA are best addressed by means of self-report questionnaires (Terasawa et al., 2013). The distinction of the three aspects of interoception can help explaining the reason why studies using self-report questionnaires or “objective” heartbeat detection tasks provide divergent findings about the relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait. In fact, contrary to findings reported by Herbert et al. (2011), Ernst et al. (2014) observed a positive correlation between IA (assessed by a self-report tool, the Body Perception Questionnaire; Porges, 1993) and alexithymic trait in a small sample of healthy subjects in a study on neurotransmitter concentration in the insula and in the anterior cingulate cortex.

A positive correlation between IA and alexithymic trait could be consistent with the view that close attention toward interoceptive sensations could hamper interpretation of one's own emotional feelings (Biondi, 1991). Accordingly, in their review Kano and Fukudo (2013) suggested that persons with alexithymia tend to over-report physical symptoms and interpret even low-intensity emotion-related sensations as signs of illness. This hypothesis is consistent with the results of a PET study showing stronger awareness of visceral sensations in alexithymics, with greater activity in the posterior insula and in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (Kano et al., 2007). Moreover, Tominaga et al. (2014) found a strong correlation between alexithymia (assessed by Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 items; Bagby et al., 1994) and hypochondriasis and somatoform disorders (assessed by Somatosensory Amplification Scale; Barsky et al., 1988; Nakao and Barsky, 2007). Taken together, such observations would show a significant direct correlation between measures of IA and alexithymic trait, when self-report assessment tools are used.

The aim of the present study was to investigate the hypothesis of a direct association between IA and alexithymic trait in a large sample of healthy subjects. In a systematic review of the available self-report measures assessing body-awareness, Mehling et al. (2009) concluded that no instruments assess all aspects of interoception, but mainly explore anxiety or emotions, without sufficient details on physical sensations and with a few items on body awareness. For these reasons, Mehling et al. (2012) developed the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) to evaluate dimensions of body awareness such as quality of body sensations, attention regulation for body sensations, emotional awareness for physiological signs of emotion or tendency to evaluate one's own body as safe and trustworthy. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is no tool evaluating perception of a wide range of bodily sensations. For this purpose, we devised a new self-report measure of IA to be used in a large non-clinical sample. This questionnaire, termed “Self-Awareness Questionnaire” (SAQ), is short and easy to administer and refers to commonly felt bodily sensations. This novel self-report tool is based on the “How do you feel questionnaire” (Grossi et al., 2014), which allowed to demonstrate that poor interoceptive awareness is associated with insular damage in stroke patients, and includes further items derived from other available questionnaires (e.g., Barsky et al., 1988; Porges, 1993).

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited 250 healthy subjects among students and academic staff of the Departments of Psychology and Political Science at the Second University of Naples. To be included in the present study, subjects had to meet the following criteria: (1) lack of current or past history of alcohol or drug abuse, (2) lack of current or past history of major psychiatric diseases, (3) lack of history of brain injury, stroke, or any other major clinical condition, (4) lack of past or current use of psychoactive medications. The eligibility criteria were assessed by means of a brief semi-structured clinical interview.

All individuals were naïve to the scopes and purposes of the study and gave their written informed consent to participate without any reward.

Materials and Methods

All participants completed three questionnaires assessing: (i) IA, (ii) the ability to identify and describe emotions, and (iii) attitudes associated with hypochondriasis. To assess IA, and to specifically investigate how and how frequently subjects feel signals arising from their own body, we used an extended version of the “How do you feel questionnaire” (Grossi et al., 2014). The questionnaire included 35 items (Appendix 1) to be rated on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = never; 1 = sometimes; 2 = often; 3 = very often; 4 = always). The total score ranges 0–140, with higher scores meaning higher IA. In a preliminary study on an independent sample of 50 healthy students, we required participants to rate whether each item was clearly comprehensible and whether it assessed common physical sensations. All items were considered simple to comprehend, and suitable to address bodily sensations; all 35 items were thus included in the SAQ.

To assess the ability to identify and describe emotions, we used the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 items (TAS-20; Bagby et al., 1994), the most widely used self-report tool to assess the Alexithymia construct. The 20 items explore three factors reflecting the main aspects of the alexithymia: difficulty in identifying feelings; difficulty in describing feelings; externally oriented thinking. Each item has to be rated on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 = “completely agree” to 5 = “strongly disagree”). The total score ranges 20–100, with higher scores indicating higher levels of alexithymia. The Italian version of TAS-20 has been demonstrated to show good test-retest reliability (0.86) and adequate internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha: 0.75) in a wide sample of healthy adults and of medical and psychiatric outpatients (Bressi et al., 1996).

The Illness Attitude Scale (IAS; Kellner, 1987) investigates attitude, fear and beliefs associated with hypochondriac behavior, and includes 27 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (from 0 = “no” to 4 = “most of the time”). The total score ranges 0–108, with higher scores indicating more severe hypochondriac symptoms. The IAS is a reliable instrument, distinguishing hypochondriac patients from psychiatric patients and healthy individuals (Kellner, 1987). The IAS has been translated in several languages and its psychometric properties are well established (Sirri et al., 2008); an Italian version of the scale has been used in studies on clinical samples (e.g., Fava et al., 2000), but its psychometric properties have not been assessed specifically.

The study was approved by the Local Ethics Committees.

Statistical Analysis

Quality of data for SAQ questionnaire was evaluated by computing percentage of missing or invalid items; a percentage <5% is considered as an index of acceptable data quality. Moreover, data quality was assessed using mean, median, skewness, kurtosis and extent of ceiling and floor effects. Floor and ceiling effects <15% were defined as optimal (Cronbach, 1951). Internal consistency of the SAQ and of its dimensions was evaluated by Cronbach's alpha (McHorney and Tarlov, 1995); a value ≥0.70 was considered acceptable (Scientific Advisory Committee of the Medical Outcomes Trust, 2002). Scaling assumptions referring to the correct grouping of items were checked using alpha correction if item is removed (item is removed if the alpha value increases).

Furthermore, we run an Exploratory Factor Analysis, by Principal Axis Factor analysis (PAF), to explore the latent structure of the scale and to perform data reduction; we extracted the components explaining an amount of variance greater than a single item do (i.e., with Eigenvalues > 1; Kaiser, 1960). We computed factor loadings after oblimin rotation, allowing factors to correlate.

Thirdly, we assessed correlations of SAQ with TAS-20 (total and subscales scores for both measures) and IAS total score by computing Pearson's correlation coefficients (Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was adopted to reduce type-I errors).

Last, to investigate whether IA explained a significant portion of variance of alexithymia, a regression analysis was performed on TAS total score, using the SAQ total score as a predictor. The specific contribution of SAQ in predicting TAS scores was also assessed by means of hierarchical multiple regression analysis performed on TAS total scores, in which we first entered demographic data (age and gender), then IAS total score, and last SAQ total score as independent variables.

Results

Two hundred and fifty healthy subjects (175 females, 75 males) participated in the present study. Table 1 shows the participants' demographic features and data about the psychometric variables.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants' demographic and psychometric variables.

Data collected for each item of the SAQ were computable and there was no missing value. There was no floor or ceiling effect; skewness was 0.78 and kurtosis was 0.30.

Descriptive analyses on normality distribution and communality indexes from PAF, suggested removing 7 of the 35 items (Items: 1, 2, 5, 8, 20, 22, 29). The mean score on the resulting 28-item SAQ “awareness index” (max value = 112) for the present sample was 23.33 (SD = 11.67). PAF extracted three factors with an eigenvalue higher than 1. The screen test indicated a two-factor solution, which accounted for about 29% of the total variance. Factor loadings obtained after oblimin rotation are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor analysis of SAQ.

The first factor (F1) was the most relevant (eigenvalue after rotation = 5.81), and mainly (but not exclusively) included items related to visceral sensations (items 3, 6, 7, 12, 13, 17, 19, 21, 23, 26, 27, 30, 32, 33, 35). The second factor (F2, eigenvalue = 4.50) mainly included items referring to somatosensory sensations (items 4, 9, 10, 11, 14, 15, 16, 18, 24, 25, 28, 30, 31, 34). The SAQ awareness index significantly correlated with both F1 (r = 0.89, p < 0.01) and F2 factors (r = 0.86, p < 0.01); the two factors significantly correlated with each other (r = 0.55, p < 0.01). The SAQ awareness index and its two factors showed good internal consistency (F1: alpha = 0.85; F2: alpha = 0.81; total alpha = 0.88). Correlation analyses (Table 3) revealed that the SAQ awareness index (and its two factors F1 and F2) significantly correlated with the TAS-20 total score and its two subscales “difficulty identifying feelings” and “difficulty describing feelings,” but not with the TAS-20 subscale assessing “externally oriented thinking.” In addition, the SAQ awareness index and its factors F1 and F2 significantly correlated with the IAS total score. All such correlations were positive, meaning that high scores on the SAQ and on its factors were associated to high scores on the questionnaires assessing two specific facets of alexithymia and hypochondriasis. By the same token, high IAS total scores were associated with high scores on TAS-20 and with high scores on two components of TAS-20, “difficulty identifying feelings” and “difficulty describing feelings.”

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation analyses between SAQ with IAS and TAS-20.

The results from the regression analysis showed that Interoception was a moderately significant predictor of Alexithymia, explaining 13% of the variance (beta = 0.37, p < 0.001). The results from the hierarchical regression models showed that: age and gender were not significant predictors of total TAS score [step# 1: R2 = 0.002; F(2, 247) = 1.28; p = 0.28; beta for age = 0.08, beta for gender = 0.06]; hypochondria was a significant predictor of alexithymia [step# 2: R2 = 0.12; F(3, 246) = 11.96; p < 0.001; beta = 0.35]; IA, as evaluated by the SAQ, was still a significant predictor of TAS scores accounting for a further 8% of the variance [step# 3: R2 = 0.20; F(4, 245) = 16.21; p < 0.001; beta for gender = 0.18, beta for IAS = 0.21, beta for SAQ = 0.33].

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationships between bodily awareness, i.e., “interoceptive awareness” after Terasawa et al. (2013), and the ability to identify and to describe emotions. For this purpose we used a self-report questionnaire (SAQ) specifically assessing how and how frequently participants felt signals from their own body. The SAQ demonstrated a good internal consistency; items clustered into two factors, the first mainly related to visceral feelings, and the second mainly related to somatosensory feelings.

In exploring the relationships between IA and emotion processing, we observed significant positive relationships of SAQ awareness index and of its two factors with TAS-20. In particular, the two subscales of the TAS-20 investigating “difficulty in identifying feelings” and “difficulty in describing feelings” showed strong positive relationships with the SAQ awareness index and its two factors. It is important to underscore that the correlation of IA with alexithymic trait was positive in the present study, consistent with findings reported by Ernst et al. (2014), who observed that a high IA, as assessed by a self-report index, correlated with high alexithymic trait. Although studies assessing interoceptive “sensitivity” (e.g., via the heartbeat detection task) reported an inverse correlation (Herbert et al., 2011), our and Ernst et al.'s findings clearly support the idea that IA is directly correlated with alexithymic trait.

The association between hypochondriasis and interoception might suggest that high IA is related to a strong concern for one's own bodily sensations. Along these lines, Salkovskis and Warwick (1986) maintained that cognitive processes concerning body, health and illness might increase attention toward bodily signals. Barsky et al. (1990) subsumed this hypothesis in the Somatosensory Amplification model, according to which hypochondriac subjects tend to focus on their somatic sensations and experience them as intense and disturbing. This group of subjects are hypervigilant about their own body, concentrate on their own physical sensations and consider them as dangerous.

The significant positive correlations among the SAQ awareness index (and its two factors), TAS-20 scores and IAS total score suggest that focusing one's own attention on bodily feelings can hamper interpretation of one's own emotional feelings, and of bodily sensations that are direct expressions of such feelings. In other words, these results might be compatible with the idea that IA, emotion processing and hypochondriasis trait are different facets of a condition in which individuals focus on their own body signals and do not recognize the emotional nature of such signals. These individuals might be prone to interpret their physiological modifications as a cue that something life threatening is going on. Increased attention to one's own somatic signals might lead to a corresponding inattention to one's own emotions. These findings seem to confirm the interconnection between these conditions. However, from the present study we cannot infer the causal directionality of such correlations, and it remains entirely plausible that high scores of TAS-20 factors may contribute to the development of somatoform disorders through attention, amplification and misinterpretation of somatic sensations with emotional arousal (Kano and Fukudo, 2013).

It is important to take into account that our conclusions are based on observations gathered from a sample of relatively young and educated healthy adults, and this might limit generalization of our findings. However, most available scales, including the Italian version of the TAS-20 used in this study, have been developed and tested on similar samples. Moreover, in our study we did not assess validity and reliability of the SAQ questionnaire and did not investigate its relationships with objective measures of interoception (e.g., the heartbeat detection task). We also acknowledge that the inclusion of clinical samples would have added further informative values to the present study, allowing ascertaining whether the same relationships among IA, alexithymia and hypochondriasis also apply in individuals with somatoform disorders. These potential limitations might trigger further extensive investigation, in which the different aspects of interoception are assessed concurrently, and levels of anxiety and of depressive symptoms are controlled, since they could modulate alexithymia, and particularly the “difficulty identifying emotion” and “difficulty describing emotion” factors of TAS-20 (Tominaga et al., 2014).

Despite the above limitations, our study demonstrated relevant interactions between IA, alexithymia and hypochondriasis. IA is necessary for maintaining homeostasis, but excessive attention to the body can interfere with the ability to interpret bodily signals correctly. From a clinical point of view, the assessment of IA could be pivotal in evaluating several psychopathological conditions. Misinterpretation of physical sensations, rather than physical sensations in themselves, can determine concerns in individuals with a high level of attention to their own body; this could result in a vicious circle involving metacognition and evaluation of one's own thinking and symptoms (Marcus et al., 2007). These observations suggest that IA could contribute in maintaining such syndromes. The SAQ might reveal useful in comprehending the role of IA in clinical samples of patients with somatoform disorders.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., and Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bard, P. (1928). A diencephalic mechanism for the expression of rage with special reference to the sympathetic nervous system. Am. J. Physiol. 84, 490–515.

Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Aronson, K. R. (2004). Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 684–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsky, A. J., Goodson, J. D., Lane, R. S., and Cleary, P. D. (1988). The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosom. Med. 50, 510–519. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198809000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsky, A. J., Wyshak, G., and Klerman, G. L. (1990). The somatosensory amplification scale and its relationship to hypochondriasis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 24, 323–334. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(90)90004-A

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthoz, S. M., Artiges, E., Van der Moortele, P. F., Poline, J., Consoli, S. M., and Martinot, J. L. (2002). Effect of impaired recognition and expression of emotions on fronto-cingulate cortices: an fMRI study of men with alexithymia. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 961–967. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biondi, M. (1991). La Psicosomatica Nella Pratica Clinica. Roma: Pensiero Scientifico Editore.

Google Scholar

Borsci, G., Boccardi, M., Rossi, R., Rossi, G., Perez, J., Bonetti, M., et al. (2009). Alexithymia in healthy women: a brain morphology study. J. Affect. Disord. 114, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressi, C., Taylor, G., Parker, J., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia, E., et al. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-items Toronto Alexithymia Scale: an Italian multicenter study. J. Psychosom. Res. 41, 551–559. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(96)00228-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, W. B. (1932). The Wisdom of the Body. New York, NY: W.W. Norton and Co.

Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 500–505. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2004). Human feelings: why are some more aware than others? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 39–241. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rothstein, P., Ohlman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural system supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16, 297–334. doi: 10.1007/BF02310555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: how interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1835–1844. doi: 10.1177/0956797610389191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, J., Böker, H., Hättenschwiler, J., Schüpbach, D., Northoff, G., Seifritz, E., et al. (2014). The association of interocpetive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 857–863. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fava, G. A., Grandi, S., Rafanelli, C., Fabbri, S., and Cazzaro, M. (2000). Explanatory therapy in hypochondriasis. J. Clin. Psychiatry 61, 317–322. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v61n0414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossi, D., Di Vita, A., Palermo, L., Sabatini, U., Trojano, L., and Guariglia, C. (2014). The brain network for self-feeling: a symptom-lesion mapping study. Neuropsychologia 63, 92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., Herbert, C., and Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J. Pers. 79, 1149–1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00717.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 214–227. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind IX, 188–205. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G. E. (1994). “Perception of visceral sensation: a review of recent findings, methodologies, and future directions,” in Advance Psychophysiology, Vol. 5, eds J. R. Jennings and P. K. Ackles (London: Jessica Kingsley publisher), 55–192.

Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 141–151. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kano, M., and Fukudo, S. (2013). The alexithymic brain: the neural pathways linking alexithymia to physical disorders. Biopsychosoc. Med. 7:1. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-7-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kano, M., Fukudo, S., Gyoba, J., Kamachi, M., Tagawa, M., Mochizuki, H., et al. (2003). Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people with alexithymia: an H2 150-PET study. Brain 126(Pt 6), 1474–1484. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kano, M., Hamaguchi, T., Itoh, M., Yanai, K., and Fukudo, S. (2007). Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain 132, 252–263. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellner, R. (1987). Abridged Manual of the Illness Attitude Scale (Mimeographed). Albuquerque, NM: Department of Psychiatry, University of New Mexico.

Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 579–591. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0251-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Reimann, E. M., Axelrod, B., Yun, L. S., Holmes, A., and Schwartz, G. E. (1998). Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness: evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulated cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 525–535. doi: 10.1162/089892998562924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., and Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am. J. Psychiatry 144, 133–143.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., Riedel, R., Shapiro, D. E., and Kaszniak, A. W. (2000). Pervasive emotion recognition deficit common to alexithymia and the repressive coping style. Psychosom. Med. 62, 492–501. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200007000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, C. G. (1885). “The mechanism of the emotions,” in The Classical Psychologists, ed B. Rand (Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin), 672–684.

Marcus, D. K., Gurley, J. R., Marchi, M. M., and Bauer, C. (2007). Cognitive and perceptual variables in hypochondriasis and health anxiety: a systematic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHorney, C. A., and Tarlov, A. R. (1995). Individual-patient-monitoring in clinical practice: are available health surveys adequate? Qual. Life Res. 4, 293–307. doi: 10.1007/BF01593882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Gopisetty, V., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Hecht, F. M., and Stewart, A. (2009). Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS ONE 4:e5614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., and Stewart, A. (2012). The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE 7:e48230. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakao, M., and Barsky, A. J. (2007). Clinical application of somatosensory amplification in psychosomatic medicine. Biopsychosoc. Med. 1:17. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Gramann, K., and Schandry, R. (2007a). Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 9–18. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Matthias, E., and Schandry, R. (2007b). Heart rate response after emotional presentation is modulated by interoceptive awareness. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 63, 117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 54–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. (1993). Body Perception Questionnaire. Laboratory of Developmental Assessment, University of Maryland. Available online at: http://stephenporges.com/index.php/publicationss/21-body-perception-questionnaires

Salkovskis, P. M., and Warwick, H. (1986). Morbid preoccupation, health anxiety and reassurance: a cognitive-behavioral approach to hypochondriasis. Behav. Res. Ther. 24, 597–602. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90041-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18, 483–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scientific Advisory Committee of the Medical Outcomes Trust. (2002). Assessing health status and quality-of-life instruments: attributes and review criteria. Qual. Life Res. 11, 193–205. doi: 10.1023/A:1015291021312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristic mechanisms in psychosomatic patients. Psychother. Psychosom. 22, 255–262. doi: 10.1159/000286529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirri, R., Grandi, S., and Fava, G. A. (2008). The illness attitude scale. A clinicmetric index for assessing hypochondriacal fears and beliefs. Psychother. Psychosom. 77, 337–350. doi: 10.1159/000151387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terasawa, Y., Shibata, M., Moriguchi, Y., and Umeda, S. (2013). Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 259–266. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tominaga, T., Choi, H., Nagoshi, Y., Wada, Y., and Fukui, K. (2014). Relationship between alexithymia and coping strategies in patients with somatoform disorders. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 10, 55–62. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S55956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 18, 442–447. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000168079.92106.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiens, S., Mezzacappa, E. S., and Katkin, E. (2000). Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cogn. Emot. 14, 417–427. doi: 10.1080/026999300378905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., Davis, J. I., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interoception and emotional experience. Neuroimage 62, 493–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

SELF-AWARENESS QUESTIONNAIRE (Original Italian version)

1. Mi capita che quando qualcuno tossisce, viene da tossire anche a me.

    (When somebody coughs, I feel like coughing too)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

2. Mi capita di sentire molto fastidio anche per una piccola ferita

    (I am excessively bothered even by a small wound)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

3. Mi capita di sentire il mio battito cardiaco pulsare nelle orecchie

    (I feel my heart beat in my ears)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

4. Mi capita di sentire eccessivamente caldo rispetto agli altri

    (I feel very hot in comparison to others)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

5. Mi capita di sentire la testa vuota

    (I feel my head empty)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

6. Mi capita di sentire eccessivamente il dolore

    (I feel pain excessively)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

7. Mi capita di sentire una stretta allo stomaco

    (I feel my stomach tightening)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

8. Mi capita di sentire un pizzicore alla gola

    (I feel a tickle in my throat)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

9. Mi capita di sentire un improvviso stimolo della fame

    (I feel a sudden hunger pang)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

10. Mi capita di sentire mal di schiena

    (I feel my back ache)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

11. Mi capita di sentire formicolii

    (I feel pins and needles)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

12. Mi capita di sentire che mi manca l'aria

    (I feel that I can't get enough air into my lungs)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

13. Mi capita di avere il batticuore

    (I have an extra-strong heartbeat)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

14. Mi capita di sentirmi gonfio dopo un pasto

    (I happen full and bloated after eating)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

15. Mi capita di sentire un improvviso stimolo della pipì

    (I have a sudden urge to urinate)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

16. Mi capita di sentirmi infuocare

    (I feel as if I am on fire)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

17. Mi capita di sentire bruciore di stomaco

    (I feel a burning sensation in my stomach)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

18. Mi capita di sentire mal di pancia

    (I feel a pain in my stomach)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

19. Mi capita di sentire eccessivamente freddo rispetto agli altri

    (I feel very cold in comparison to others)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

20. Mi capita di sentire prurito

    (I feel itchy)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

21. Mi capita di sentire che devo vomitare

    (I feel as if I have to throw up)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

22. Mi capita di sentire un improvviso stimolo della cacca

    (I happen a sudden urge to defecate)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

23. Mi capita di sentirmi agghiacciare

    (I feel chilled)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

24. Mi capita di sentire le gambe pesanti

    (I feel my legs are heavy)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

25. Mi capita di sentire la gola secca

    (I feel my throat dry)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

26. Mi capita di sentire un peso al petto

    (I have a heavy feeling in my chest)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

27. Mi capita di sentire un tonfo al cuore

    (I feel my heart thudding)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

28. Mi capita di sentire un improvviso stimolo della sete

    (I feel sudden thirst pangs)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

29. Mi capita di sentire un dolore che si sposta da una parte all'altra del corpo

    (I feel a pain that seems to migrate around the body)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

30. Mi capita di sentire l'affanno senza aver fatto alcuno sforzo

    (I feel breathless without engaging in any type of exertion or effort)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

31. Mi capita di sentire le orecchie bollenti

    (I feel my ears burning)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

32. Mi capita di sentire un nodo alla gola

    (I feel a lump in my throat)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

33. Mi capita di sentirmi svenire

    (I feel faint)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

34. Mi capita di sentire le mani sudate

    (I feel my palms sweaty)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

35. Mi capita di sentire difficoltà ad ingoiare

    (I have difficulty swallowing)

    □ never □ sometimes □ often □ very often □ always

ISTRUZIONI.

Per cortesia legga attentamente ogni domanda e scelga la risposta che meglio descrive quanto spesso avverte ogni sensazione. Indichi una sola risposta. Non ci sono risposte corrette o errate.

(Please read each item carefully and tick the one box that best describes how often you feel each sensation. Tick one only. There are no right or wrong answers.)

Keywords: interoceptive awareness, emotion, alexithymia, hypochondriasis, health, insular cortex

Citation: Longarzo M, D'Olimpio F, Chiavazzo A, Santangelo G, Trojano L and Grossi D (2015) The relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait. The Self-Awareness Questionnaire in healthy subjects. Front. Psychol. 6:1149. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01149

Received: 23 December 2014; Accepted: 23 July 2015;
Published: 07 August 2015.

Edited by:

Olga Pollatos, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Glenn Carruthers, Macquarie University, Australia
Neil Gerald Muggleton, National Central University, Taiwan

Copyright © 2015 Longarzo, D'Olimpio, Chiavazzo, Santangelo, Trojano and Grossi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mariachiara Longarzo and Dario Grossi, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, Second University of Naples, Viale Ellittico 31, 81100 Caserta, Italy, mariachiara.longarzo@unina2.it; dario.grossi@unina2.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.