Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 27 May 2015
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Sex differences in multisensory speech processing in both typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum

  • 1The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Department of Pediatrics, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center, Albert Einstein College of Medicine & Montefiore Medical Center, Bronx, NY, USA
  • 2The Gordon F. Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies, Adelphi University, Garden City, NY, USA
  • 3Ferkauf Graduate School of Psychology, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, USA
  • 4The Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, USA
  • 5Division of Neurocritical Care, Department of Neurology, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA

Background: Previous work has revealed sizeable deficits in the abilities of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) to integrate auditory and visual speech signals, with clear implications for social communication in this population. There is a strong male preponderance in ASD, with approximately four affected males for every female. The presence of sex differences in ASD symptoms suggests a sexual dimorphism in the ASD phenotype, and raises the question of whether this dimorphism extends to ASD traits in the neurotypical population. Here, we investigated possible sexual dimorphism in multisensory speech integration in both ASD and neurotypical individuals.

Methods: We assessed whether males and females differed in their ability to benefit from visual speech when target words were presented under varying levels of signal-to-noise, in samples of neurotypical children and adults, and in children diagnosed with an ASD.

Results: In typically developing (TD) children and children with ASD, females (n = 47 and n = 15, respectively) were significantly superior in their ability to recognize words under audiovisual listening conditions compared to males (n = 55 and n = 58, respectively). This sex difference was absent in our sample of neurotypical adults (n = 28 females; n = 28 males).

Conclusions: We propose that the development of audiovisual integration is delayed in male relative to female children, a delay that is also observed in ASD. In neurotypicals, these sex differences disappear in early adulthood when females approach their performance maximum and males “catch up.” Our findings underline the importance of considering sex differences in the search for autism endophenotypes and strongly encourage increased efforts to study the underrepresented population of females within ASD.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs) are diagnosed in considerably greater numbers of males than females, with estimated ratios in the range of four affected males for every female (CDC, 2014). The mechanisms that give rise to this male bias are not well understood and are the subject of much current debate (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 2009; Fombonne, 2009; Werling and Geschwind, 2013). Several biological and non-biological theories have been proposed.

Non-biological models attribute differences in prevalence rate to biases introduced by differences in the presentation of ASD symptoms. Males with ASD have been reported to show more “externalizing behavior” including hyperactivity, aggressive behaviors, and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors and interests (Giarelli et al., 2010; Bölte et al., 2011; Hattier et al., 2011; Mandy et al., 2012; Solomon et al., 2012; Szatmari et al., 2012). On the other hand, females diagnosed with ASD present with more “internalizing behaviors” such as anxiety and depression (Hattier et al., 2011; Solomon et al., 2012). It therefore seems quite likely that the more socially disruptive behaviors in males have a higher likelihood to motivate parents or caretakers to seek clinical evaluations. In females, ASD symptoms are diagnosed when associated with more severe intellectual disabilities. In addition, high functioning ASD in females may be masked by their higher social abilities causing them to remain undiagnosed (Russell et al., 2011; Dworzynski et al., 2012).

Several biological models have been proposed to explain sex differences in ASD prevalence. The Extreme Male Brain (EMB) theory (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 2002) proposes that factors inherent in the male genotype and development that give rise to typically observed sexual dimorphisms in cognition (e.g., empathy and systemizing; Asperger and Frith, 1991) may be exaggerated in people affected with ASD giving rise to disordered social behavior (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003, 2005; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004). This proposed “masculinization” can be observed in overt behavior (Ingudomnukul et al., 2007; Knickmeyer et al., 2008) and at the levels of brain structure and function (Lai et al., 2013) and may be linked to the expression of sex-hormones (Auyeung et al., 2006, 2009). Another model proposes that females with ASD carry a higher mutational load than affected males, but that the minimum liability threshold sufficient to cause ASD is higher in females. Evidence in support of this model has been mixed (Goin-Kochel et al., 2007; Hallmayer et al., 2011), but recently, some fairly convincing support emerged from a study by Robinson et al. (2013). The central premise of the Robinson study was that if a female “protective” effect exists, then a simple prediction would be that siblings of females with very high autistic trait scores (i.e., those above the 90th percentile) should show greater autistic trait scores than siblings of males with similarly high trait scores. This is exactly what was found in this large study of almost 10,000 twin pairs. There is also support for this protective notion from studies in females with Turner syndrome, a chromosomal abnormality where one of the X chromosomes is either missing or partially deleted (Bondy et al., 2012). Skuse and colleagues showed that social difficulties in this syndrome were predicated upon the parent of origin of the functioning X chromosome (Skuse et al., 1997). That is, those girls with a paternally derived X showed better social abilities than those with a maternally derived X, pointing to an imprinted genetic locus (or loci) for social cognitive functions expressed only on the paternal X. Of course, all males inherit their X chromosome from the mother, and so they won't express this socially protective gene (or genes), and their threshold for ASD would therefore be lower than it is in females (Skuse, 2000).

The study of sex differences in ASD traits is challenged by the relatively lower prevalence of females diagnosed with ASD and complicated by developmental changes and biases inherent in the diagnostic process due to differences in the presentation of symptoms. These circumstances may have led to inconsistencies in reports on sex differences in core ASD traits with some studies reporting evidence for sex differences in core diagnostic features (McLennan et al., 1993; Carter et al., 2007; Hartley and Sikora, 2009; Lai et al., 2011; Mandy et al., 2012) and other studies providing evidence to the contrary (Tsai and Beisler, 1983; Pilowsky et al., 1998; Holtmann et al., 2007; Solomon et al., 2012). Biological and non-biological models of sex differences in ASD are clearly not mutually exclusive, and both have important implications for the investigation of genetics, brain function and their relationship to the overt symptoms of ASD. These implications extend to research on ASD traits in the neurotypical population as there is mounting evidence for the heritability of ASD traits in unaffected individuals (Robinson et al., 2011; Lundström et al., 2012) and it has been suggested that “at least some of the genetic and environmental factors associated with ASD are the same as those that cause individual differences in autism-like behavior below the clinical threshold.” (Robinson et al., 2013). If indeed autistic traits are represented on a continuous spectrum that extends into the unaffected population then the sexual dimorphism in ASD characteristics should as well. In other words, if there is a sexual dimorphism of core ASD symptoms in individuals with an ASD diagnosis, then these sex differences would likely also manifest as different distributions of ASD traits in the unaffected population. For example, Matsuyoshi et al. (2014) examined sensitivity to eye gaze direction in 128 unaffected adults (64 females), a task in which individuals with ASD display robust deficits (see Senju and Johnson, 2009 for a review). The investigators found that individual differences in autistic traits predicted performance in this task in male but not female participants suggesting that direct-gaze perception may not constitute an autistic endophenotype in both sexes. Lai et al. (2012) studied four key cognitive domains including mentalizing and emotion perception, executive function, perceptual attention to detail and motor function in 128 male and female adults with and without ASD (32 per group). They found that deficits in mentalizing and facial emotion perception in individuals with ASD compared to controls were similar in both sexes. However, attention to detail and dexterity involving executive function were found to be impaired only in male ASD participants. The authors suggested that performance in the social cognitive domain is equally impaired in male and female individuals with ASD, whereas sex differences are observed in non-social cognitive domains. These findings lend support to the notion of sex differences in the disease phenotype and associated traits in the “normal” population and represent compelling reasons to consider sex differences when studying ASD traits in affected and unaffected samples.

In a previous cross-sectional study of 84 children with ASD and 142 neurotypical children between the ages of 5 and 17, we provided strong support for severe multisensory deficits in audiovisual speech perception during childhood and a subsequent recovery around 12 years of age in children with an ASD (Foxe et al., 2015). These large and consistent deficits in audiovisual gain between 5 and 12 years of age could not be explained by unisensory speech perception deficits alone or differences in eye-gaze as assessed with eye-tracking. These findings raise important questions about the neural mechanisms underlying these observed deficits, their possible heritability and link to biological sex. An advanced understanding of sex differences in ASD may benefit our understanding of the genetic, neurobiological and environmental factors involved in the development of ASD. Basic research on sex differences in ASD has practical implications for therapeutic intervention and may inform clinicians to delineate more personalized treatments for this diverse disorder.

In the current study we assessed sex differences in the perception of auditory, visual and audiovisual speech presented in varying levels of noise in typically developing children between the ages of 5 and 17 years of age. We also explored possible sex differences in speech perception in neurotypical adults. We finally examined sex differences in speech perception performance in a sample of ASD children between 8 and 17 years of age.

Methods

Participants

Our first analysis involved 102 typically developing children in the age range from 5 to 17 years of age. The 55 male participants in our sample had a mean age of Mage = 12.02 years (SDage = 3.2) and a mean full IQ (FIQ) of MFIQ = 112.02 (SDFIQ = 12.01). The 47 females were on average Mage = 11.36 (SDage = 3.55) years of age with a mean FIQ of MFIQ = 107.19 (SDFIQ = 15.95). The data of these participants represent a subset of a larger sample and were selected because these participants were assessed with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI) and therefore allowed the inclusion of FIQ as a covariate. An additional analysis was carried out in a sample of 28 male (Mage = 26.16; SDage = 4.31) and 28 female (Mage = 25.34; SDage = 4.23) neurotypical adults between 20 and 39 years of age. Finally, we analyzed sex differences in a sample of 58 male (Mage = 10.79; SDage = 2.15) and 15 female (Mage = 11.87; SDage = 2.36) children between 8 and 15 years of age who had previously been diagnosed with ASD. It should be noted that the samples of TD and ASD children reported here overlap with the samples reported in Foxe et al. (2015). A breakdown of the demographics of each of the subgroups is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Auditory- alone performance as a function of Sex, Age, FIQ, and SNR in TD children (5–17).

All participants were native English speakers. Participants were excluded from this study if they had a history of seizures or had uncorrected vision problems. TD children were excluded if they had a history of psychiatric, educational, attentional or other developmental difficulties as assessed by a history questionnaire and were also excluded if their parents endorsed six or more items of inattention or hyperactivity on a DSM-IV checklist for attention deficit disorder (with and without hyperactivity). Diagnoses of ASD were obtained by a trained clinical psychologist using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-R (ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-G; Lord et al., 2000). All children had normal or corrected-to normal vision and audiometric threshold evaluation confirmed that all children had within-normal-limits hearing. The parents of all child participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the tenets of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures were approved by the institutional review board(s) of the City College of New York and the Albert Einstein College of Medicine.

Stimuli and Task

Stimulus materials consisted of digital recordings of 300 simple monosyllabic words spoken by a female speaker. This set of words was a subset of the stimulus material created for a previous experiment in our laboratory (Ross et al., 2007a) and used in previous studies (Ross et al., 2011; Foxe et al., 2015). These words were taken from the “MRC Psycholinguistic Database” (Coltheart, 1981) and were selected from a well-characterized normed set based on their written-word frequency (Kucera and Francis, 1967). The subset of words for the present experiment is a selection of simple, high-frequency words from a child's everyday environment and is likely to be in the lexicon of children in the age-range of our sample. The recorded movies were digitally re-mastered so that the length of the movie (1.3 s) and the onset of the acoustic signal were similar across all words. Average voice onset occurred at 520 ms after movie onset (SD = 30 ms). The words were presented at approximately 50 dBA FSPL, at seven levels of intelligibility including a condition with no noise (NN) and six conditions with added pink noise at 53, 56, 59, 62, 65, and 65 dB SPL. Noise onset was synchronized with movie onset. The signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) were therefore NN, −3, −6, −9, −12, −15, −18 dB. These SNRs were chosen to cover a performance range in the auditory-alone condition from 0% recognized words at the lowest SNR to almost perfect recognition performance with no noise. The movies were presented on a monitor (NEC Multisync FE 2111SB) at 80 cm distance from the eyes of the participants. The face of the speaker extended approximately 6.44° of visual angle horizontally and 8.58° vertically (hairline to chin). The words and pink noise were presented over headphones (Sennheiser, model HD 555).

The main experiment consisted of three randomly intermixed conditions: In the auditory-alone condition (A) the auditory words were presented in conjunction with a still image of the speakers face; in the audiovisual condition (AV) the auditory words were presented in conjunction with the corresponding video of the speaker articulating the words. Finally, in the visual alone condition (V) only the video of the speaker's articulations was presented. The word stimuli were presented in a fixed order and the condition (the noise level and whether it was presented as A, V, or AV) was assigned to each word randomly. Stimuli were presented in 15 blocks of 20 words with a total of 300 stimulus presentations. There were 140 stimuli for the A and AV conditions respectively (20 stimuli per condition and intelligibility level) and 20 stimuli for the V condition that was presented without noise.

Participants were instructed to watch the screen and report which word they heard (or saw in the V-alone condition). If a word was not clearly understood, participants were encouraged to make their best guess. An experimenter, seated approximately 1 m distance from the participant at a 90° angle to the participant-screen axis, monitored participant's adherence to maintaining fixation on the screen. Only responses that exactly matched the presented word were considered correct. Any other response was recorded as incorrect.

Eye Tracking

Eye movements were recorded using an EyeLink 1000 system (SR Research, Ontario, Canada), at a sampling rate of 500 Hz. As described previously (Foxe et al., 2015), a small target sticker was placed on the participants' forehead, allowing the system to compensate for head movements of up to 20 cm. In order to prevent larger head movements the children had to place their heads on a comfortable chin rest. The eye tracking system was calibrated using a nine-point calibration before each set of 5 blocks of stimuli (or more often if necessary). Using the default settings, saccades and fixations were defined by the EyeLink system. Data were collected for 90 (59 male, 41 female) typically developing and 68 (58 male, 10 female) ASD participants. In the typically developing group, three datasets had to be removed (all female), while for ASD 6 datasets had to be removed (3 male, 3 female).

Custom Matlab scripts (Mathworks, Natick, MA USA) were used to analyze the Eye-tracking data. We determined the proportion of fixations on the different parts of the speaker's face. This was accomplished by selecting three rectangular patches, covering the whole face, only the mouth, or only the eyes and determining the proportion of fixations within these patches. Since lips and jaw move during speech production, the mouth region was defined vertically from the bottom of the lower jaw to just below the nose (the nasolabial angle of the philtrim). These measures were taken from the still image of the speaker before articulation started (i.e., with the mouth closed). The proportion of fixations in the different groups was statistically compared using a z-statistic for proportions (two-tailed), while numbers of fixations were compared using a t-test for independent samples (two-tailed). Data were analyzed for all conditions combined.

In addition to the statistical analyses, fixation distribution maps were in accordance with previous studies (e.g., Frey et al., 2011). These maps provide information about how consistent fixation locations are between participants and throughout the different trials. A value of 1 indicates that all participants fixated the same location during each fixation. Lower values indicate that participants either are not consistent in where they look or fixate different parts of the face in successive fixations. For example, if there are two small objects of interest in a scene, which are consistently fixated (equally often) by all participants, then the fixation map will have two peaks with a height of about 0.5.

Analyses of Task Performance

Analyses of speech perception performance were carried out in the subgroups of TD children, TD adults and ASD children. We first investigated possible sex differences in speech perception performance in typically developing children of the ages five to seventeen. We submitted percent correct responses in the A and AV conditions as well as AV-gain respectively to separate repeated measures analyses of variance (RM-ANOVA) with factors SNR (5 levels of signal to noise ratio), between- subjects factor SEX (male, female) and Age (in years) as well as FIQ (full IQ) as covariates. The conditions with NN and −3 dB SNR were not included in the analysis to avoid possible ceiling effects (Ross et al., 2011). However, in order to provide the reader with an easy- to interpret characterization of the group differences, we displayed A and AV speech perception performance as well as AV-gain as it unfolded over all intelligibility conditions (Figure 1). Since the speech reading condition (V) was only presented without noise a separate univariate ANOVA was carried with Sex as a main factor and Age and FIQ serving as covariates. For all ANOVAs we assured the absence of violations of assumptions of equality of variances and equality of covariance matrices (Box test). Violations of the sphericity assumption of the RM-ANOVA were corrected by adjusting the degrees of freedom with the Greenhouse-Geisser correction method. All results relevant to our hypotheses are reported in the main text of the results section and the full statistical report can be found in the tables. We expected significant main effects of condition, SNR level, group and age as well as an interaction between condition and SNR level replicating previous findings (Ross et al., 2007a,b, 2011; Ma et al., 2009; Foxe et al., 2015). Audiovisual enhancement (or AV-gain) was operationalized here as the difference in performance between the AV and the A-alone condition (AV – A). In a second step similar analyses were carried out in the subgroup of neurotypical adults to determine if possible sex differences persist into adulthood. Information on IQ was not available and did not serve as a covariate in this test. For the assessment of sex differences in ASD children IQ information was only available for a small subset of ASD females (see Table 1) and therefore only age was included as a covariate.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Performance of male and female TD children. Estimated Marginal Means (at Age 11.72 and FIQ: 109.79) for TD males and females (ages 5–17) for auditory (A), visual (D), and audiovisual (B) conditions as well as gain (C) controlled for the effect of age and FIQ under seven listening conditions.

Results

Sex Differences in TD Children and Adults

Auditory Alone

As reported previously (Ross et al., 2007a,b, 2011; Foxe et al., 2015) and can be seen in Figure 1, performance in the A condition showed a monotonous, close to linear increase from near zero percent correct at the lowest SNR (males: M = 0.71%, SD = 5.27%; females: M = 1.5%, SD = 2.26%) to approximately 90% correct word identification when no noise was added (males: M = 86.9%, SD = 2.45%; females: M = 88.2%, SD = 9.6%). Visual inspection revealed that females performed slightly better at intermediate SNRs. These small differences in performance, however, only approached significance [F(1, 98) = 3.77; p = 0.055; η2p = 0.037]. Both covariates Age [F(1, 98) = 23.17; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.19] and FIQ [F(1, 98) = 7.8; p = 0.006; η2p = 0.07] had significant main effects on performance. Age [F(3, 294.66) = 8.68; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.08] and FIQ [F(3, 294.66) = 3.16; p = 0.025; η2p = 0.03] showed significant interactions with SNR. Under the restricted range of SNR levels, SNR did not show an independent main effect [F(3, 98) = 0.88; p = ns.]. We tested whether the effect of sex was also present in our sample of healthy 28 adult men and 28 adult women between the ages of 20–38 years but could not find statistical evidence for group differences [main effect Sex: F(1, 53) = 0.001; p = ns.] (see Table 3). Even in the group of adults, age had a significant main effect on performance [main effect Age: F(1, 53) = 7.43; p = 0.009; η2p = 0.12]. Interestingly, the RM- ANOVA returned a significant interaction between SNR and Sex [F(2.87, 152.33) = 3.55; p = 0.017; η2p = 0.06]. However, a subsequent inspection of the age- corrected performance revealed that interactions were not uniform across SNRs.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Auditory- alone performance as a function of Sex, Age, and SNR in TD adults.

Audiovisual

Visual inspection of Figure 1B reveals that speaker articulation substantially improved speech intelligibility. Participants correctly identified approximately 20% of the words at the lowest SNR (males: M = 17.84%, SD = 10.6%; females: M = 22.32%, SD = 10.62%) and approximately 90% without noise (males: M = 88.71%, SD = 8.83%; females: M = 94.25%, SD = 8.84%). Females performed better across all SNR conditions which was confirmed by a significant main effect of Sex with substantially larger effect size than the group differences in the A condition [F(1, 98) = 17.65; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.15]. Again, factors Age [F(1, 98) = 72.14; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.42] and FIQ [F(1, 98) = 9.79; p = 0.002; η2p = 0.09] had significant main effects on performance. The parametric variation of noise produced a monotonic linear increase in performance between best and worst listening conditions which was confirmed by a significant main effect of SNR [F(3.45, 338.35) = 3.05; p = 0.023; η2p = 0.03]. The RM-ANOVA did not return interactions other than between SNR and Sex [F(3.45, 338.35) = 2.77; p = 0.034; η2p = 0.027]. For a full report, please refer to Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Audiovisual performance as a function of Sex, Age, FIQ and SNR in TD children.

In TD adults there was no evidence for sex differences in the AV condition [F(1, 53) = 0.23; p = ns.] and there was no significant effect of factor Age [F(1, 53) = 1.24; p = ns.] (see Table 5 for the full report).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Audiovisual performance as a function of Sex, Age, and SNR in TD adults.

Audiovisual Gain

Conforming with previous reports (Ross et al., 2007a,b, 2011; Foxe et al., 2015), audiovisual gain showed an inverted u-shaped curvilinear relationship with a maximum at intermediate SNRs at −9 dB in male and −12 dB in female participants (see Figure 1C). While substantial AV-gain was achieved at the lowest SNR (17% in males, 21% in females), AV-gain decreased as AV-performance approached ceiling. While AV-gain was very similar in male and female participants at SNRs above −12 dB, it was larger in females at the three lowest SNRs which was reflected in a significant main effect of factor Sex on AV-gain [F(1, 98) = 5.39; p = 0.022; η2p = 0.05]. Factor Age had a significant main effect on performance [F(1, 98) = 17.49; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.15] whereas FIQ did not [F(1, 98) = 0.91; p = ns.]. The RM-ANOVA also returned a significant interaction between factors Age and SNR [F(3.32, 325.32) = 3.81; p = 0.008; η2p = 0.037]. Please refer to Table 6 for a full report. We found no evidence for differences between males and females in our adult sample [F(1, 53) = 0.11; p = ns.] (Table 7).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Audiovisual gain (AV-A) as a function of Sex, Age, FIQ, and SNR in TD children.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Audiovisual gain (AV-A) as a function of Sex, Age, and SNR in TD adults.

Speechreading

Females (M = 13.79%, SD = 7.82) performed significantly better than males (M = 8.29%, SD = 7.79) under conditions where only visual articulation was provided and when performance was adjusted for the effect of age and FIQ [F(1, 98) = 8.59; p = 0.001; η2p = 0.11] (see Figure 1D). The effect of age was strong [F(1, 98) = 18.86; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.16], but the main effect of factor FIQ did not reach significance [F(1, 98) = 1.95; p = ns.]. Much to our surprise, we found that male TD adults (M = 13.09%; SD = 8.1) performed better in the speechreading condition than TD females (M = 8.14%; SD = 8.1) [F(1, 56) = 6.8; p = 0.027].

Performance Differences between ASD Males and Females

Included in this analysis were 58 ASD males and 15 ASD females between the ages of 8 and 15 years of age. The statistical analysis was equivalent to the on in the report on TD children above. Important to note here is that on the basis of the direction of the sex effect in our TD sample and our relatively small subsample of ASD females statistical tests were carried out one-sided.

Auditory Alone

The graph depicting A performance in male and female participants diagnosed with ASD (Figure 2A) shows that both groups performed very similarly in this condition. This was confirmed by the RM- ANOVA that returned no significant main effect of factor Sex [F(1, 70) = 0.99; p = ns.]. Besides a significant effects of factor Age [F(1, 70) = 7; p = 0.01, η2p = 0.09] and an interaction with SNR [F(2.59, 181.08) = 5.9; p = 0.001, η2p = 0.08] no other effect was observed. A full RM-ANOVA report can be found in Table 8.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Performance of male and female ASD children. Estimated Marginal Means (at Age 11.01) for ASD males and females (ages 8–15) for auditory (A), visual (D), and audiovisual (B) conditions as well as gain (C) controlled for the effect of age under seven listening conditions.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Auditory- alone performance as a function of Sex, Age and SNR in ASD children (8–15 years).

Audiovisual

In this test, the assumption of homogeneity of variances was violated at the lowest SNR. We therefore conducted a test including performance at −15, −12, −9, −6, and −3 dB SNRs. Unlike the performance in the auditory alone condition, sex differences were evident in the presence of visual speech which was consistent at all SNRs except without noise, when performance was near ceiling levels (see Figure 2B). The group difference was confirmed by a significant main effect of Sex in the RM- ANOVA [F(1, 70) = 3.48; p = 0.033; η2p = 0.047]. Besides significant effects of factors Age [F(1, 70) = 19.96; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.22] and SNR [F(3.8, 265.38) = 12.34; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.15] no other sources reached our significance criterion. The complete RM-ANOVA report can be found in Table 9.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Audiovisual performance as a function of Sex, Age, and SNR in ASD children (8–15 years).

Audiovisual Gain

The overall difference between males and females with ASD was also apparent in the graph depicting audiovisual gain for both groups across SNRs (Figure 2C). The overall difference between males and females was significant [F(1, 70) = 2.91; p = 0.047; η2p = 0.04]. Factors Age [F(1, 70) = 14.06; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.17] and SNR [F(3.35, 234.54) = 4.15; p = 0.003; η2p = 0.06] and the interaction between Age and SNR [F(3.35, 234.54) = 3.29; p = 0.009; η2p = 0.045] were also observed to explain a significant amount of variance in audiovisual gain. A full RM-ANOVA report can be found in Table 10.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Audiovisual gain (AV-A) as a function of Sex, Age, and SNR in ASD children (8–15 years).

Speechreading

In the speechreading condition male participants with ASD (M = 4.9%, SD = 6.01) performed slightly (M = 4.05%, SD = 6.08) but not significantly better than females [F(1, 70) = 0.23; p = ns.]. The bar graph in Figure 2D depicts performances in speechreading corrected for age.

Eye-Movement Analyses

There were no differences in viewing behavior between female and male participants in the TD (t(85) = −0.569, p = ns.) and ASD (t(60) = −0.069, p = ns.) groups. Proportions of fixations (Figure 3) on different parts of the face as well as number of fixations were not statistically distinguishable between sexes (Table 11). We also found no differences in the mean fixation times between TD males (734 ms) and TD females (678 ms) t(85) = 0.54, p = ns. and between ASD males (586 ms) and ASD females (549 ms) t(85) = 0.44, p = ns.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Fixation maps for fixations during presentation of the speech stimuli. Data for participants from all age ranges were combined. Brighter colors indicate a higher consistency of fixations. The theoretical maximum value is 1. This value can only be reached if all participants fixate on exactly the same spot during all trials and during each fixation.

TABLE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Comparison of Eye-Tracking between males and females by diagnostic group.

Therefore, it is very unlikely that the behavioral results can be explained by one group attending the stimuli less.

Comparison of Performances of Non-Adult Subgroups

With this descriptive analysis we aim to provide the opportunity for a more direct comparison of performance differences between males and females and the association with diagnostic status. For this, we averaged performance in each condition over the lower five SNRs with the exception of V- performance which was presented only without noise. We restricted the age- range to 8–15 years but also included children for which we had no IQ scores. We did not control for FIQ in this analysis because we did not obtain IQ scores for all the females in our ASD sample. We used a General Linear Model to establish estimated performance scores that were corrected for age. Figure 4 (see also Table 12) shows a bar graph depicting mean performance for A, AV, and V conditions as well as ****AV-gain for n = 98 typically developing children (n = 43 females, n = 55 males) and n = 73 children diagnosed with ASD (n = 15 females; n = 58 males).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison between males and females by diagnostic group. Estimated Marginal Means (at Age 11.02) for TD and ASD males and females (ages 8–15) for A, V, and AV conditions as well as AV-A controlled for the effect of age.

TABLE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Table 12. Comparison between males and females by diagnostic group.

Visual inspection of the bar graphs reveals that, conforming with our findings reported earlier, both ASD diagnosis and gender were consistently associated with performance with overall lower performance in association with ASD diagnosis and male sex (with the exception of male performance in the V-condition). These associations were most pronounced in the AV condition. The graphs also suggest that the association between ASD diagnosis and lower performance was more pronounced in males than in females. In other words, the difference between TD males and ASD males was larger than the difference between TD females and ASD females in all conditions except speechreading. While ASD females still performed consistently lower than TD males, differences to TD males appeared small. For a statistical verification of these small differences a larger sample size for ASD females is warranted.

Discussion

In this study, we assessed whether male and female participants differed in their ability to benefit from visual speech when target words were presented under varying background noise conditions. We tested sex differences in a sample of TD children and adults, and children diagnosed with ASD. In the TD child sample, females were significantly superior in recognizing speech in noise under auditory alone conditions than were males. Even larger performance differences were found under multisensory conditions, with the females benefitting significantly more from the addition of visual speech than the males, particularly under low intelligibility conditions (i.e., higher background noise). The females also performed better under pure speechreading conditions. These sex differences in children were fully absent in the sample of adult participants with the exception of the speechreading condition, in which case the males were slightly but significantly better at speechreading than the females. We then tested whether male/female performance differences were present in a sample of ASD children and found that ASD females performed significantly better under audiovisual conditions than ASD males, a difference that was not apparent for the auditory-alone condition in which no visual articulatory information was provided. Similarly, we found no evidence for sex differences in the ASD sample in speechreading, thus ruling out a purely unisensory account of differences in multisensory gain. Further, eye-tracking data made it clear that these sex differences were not due to different gaze patterns.

Clearly, multisensory speech perception is an important aspect of social communication. Therefore, possible answers to the observed sex differences might be found in sex differences in the development of social communication skills in general. Indeed, there is an extensive literature on the development of social communication in males and females which most frequently shows that females display greater, or at least earlier, development of skills in this domain. On average, females start to talk earlier than males (Fenson et al., 1994) and score higher on tests of verbal fluency (Hyde and Linn, 1988). Girls and women exhibit more eye contact than males (Hall, 1985), show greater ability to detect and understand emotional facial expressions (Rosenthal et al., 1979; Happe, 1995; Baron-Cohen et al., 1997, 1999) and there is accumulating evidence that preadolescent girls show relatively higher abilities in tasks assessing social understanding such as inferring other people's mental states (Theory of Mind; Hatcher et al., 1990; Bosacki and Astington, 1999; Calero et al., 2013).

It has been suggested that differences in social communication may have their origins at the earliest stages of development during intrauterine exposure to sex hormones (Auyeung et al., 2006, 2009; Chapman et al., 2006) thereby affecting brain structure and function relevant to social communication. Female newborns look longer at animated faces than mobile mechanical objects whereas newborn males showed the opposite pattern (Connellan et al., 2000). These genetic/epigenetic/hormonal origins of sex differences may be further enhanced by differential socialization, especially by parents (Stern and Karraker, 1989). Mothers have more verbal communication with their daughters than with their sons (Leaper et al., 1998) and parents show preferential acknowledgement of their infant daughter's emotional displays than their son's (Malatesta and Haviland, 1982). These factors may explain why female toddlers and infants show greater nonverbal communication skills (Clarke-Stewart, 1973; Fenson et al., 1994), vocabulary acquisition (Huttenlocher et al., 1991) and frequency of social initiations (Klein and Durfee, 1978). The evidence for differences in integration abilities between males and females remains far from conclusive, although women have been shown to be better at lip reading when target words were presented in a sentence context (Johnson et al., 1988; Watson et al., 1996). Similarly, some investigators have reported increased sensitivity to the so-called McGurk effect in women (Aloufy et al., 1996; Öhrström and Traunmüller, 2004). The McGurk illusion refers to a rather dramatic multisensory perceptual phenomenon whereby presentation of incongruent visual articulatory inputs can greatly modify the speech sound that is heard (McGurk and MacDonald, 1976; Saint-Amour et al., 2007). Irwin et al. (2006) showed that women displayed greater influence of visual speech on heard speech than men, but did not find evidence for sex differences in speechreading abilities.

Apart from greater abilities in AV-speech perception in ASD girls than ASD boys, our findings suggest that AV-benefit is not as affected by ASD in females as in males when stratifying performance by sex. Small relative decrements in AV-benefit in ASD females compared to TD males may exist, but did not reach statistical significance. ASD females do show lower performance than their unaffected counterparts, but they appear to be affected to a lesser extent than their male peers. Given our evidence for a sexual dimorphism in audiovisual speech perception in the TD population, and the apparent role of audiovisual speech perception for social communication (e.g., reading speech cues from faces) one may be inclined to interpret these findings within the framework of the EMB- theory. However, a closer look reveals that our pattern of results does not entirely conform to the EMB- theory. Although, to our knowledge, this prediction has not been explicitly stated by EMB proponents, it is implicit in the theory that if ASD brings about an increase or exaggeration of a masculine phenotype then one would expect that the effect of the disorder should have a greater impact on females than on males since males with ASD approach the extreme end of the proposed “maleness” spectrum and the difference between affected and unaffected males might therefore be subject to a ceiling effect. In contrast to this prediction, our data show larger differences in males than in females. However, the interpretation of our findings in light of the EMB theory should be exercised with caution, since it is not entirely clear whether the ability to read visual speech cues can be interpreted as an act of “empathizing” as has been proposed for the ability to read emotional expressions on people's faces (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005).

The pattern of our findings suggests a more parsimonious model of the observed sex differences. AV-speech perception can be regarded as an instance of language and social communication for which, as discussed above, previous evidence for a sexual phenotypic difference has been provided. The ability to integrate visual speech might therefore have genetic as well as environmental origins such as differential parental socialization resulting in an earlier development of these abilities in females and a sustained advantage into the late teenage years (Ross et al., 2011) relative to age-matched males. This developmental trajectory flattens toward early adulthood when females reach their performance maximum which allows males to “catch up” explaining the absence of performance differences in adults. ASD imposes a developmental delay in both males and females that resolves in the early teenage years rather than an irreversible and sustained impairment (Foxe et al., 2015). The fact that females with ASD are affected to a lesser extent might be afforded by “protective factors” inherent in the female genotype (Skuse et al., 1997; Skuse, 2000; Robinson et al., 2013). Unfortunately, the exploration of the developmental course of AV speech processing stratified by sex was not possible within the framework of this study due to the low subject numbers in our female ASD sample and should be subject to future studies.

High-density recordings of electrophysiological brain activity have revealed that the neural integration of multisensory inputs is impaired in children with ASD (Russo et al., 2010; Brandwein et al., 2013, 2015), and it is also the case that ASD children are not as effective at deploying attention to a relevant unisensory stream when there are competing multisensory inputs (Murphy et al., 2014). These studies included only a small proportion of female participants with ASD, precluding consideration of the role of sex in these deficits. An important question then is whether these deficits are equivalently seen in both male and female participants with ASD, or if they are sexually dimorphic as we see in the present study. These electrophysiological studies presented simple stimuli of no obvious higher-order communicative or social significance, and as such the findings were interpreted as representing the breakdown of basic sensory and attentional processes, although these could well have cascading consequences for higher-order functions such as multisensory speech perception. It is instructive that even fundamental deficits in multisensory integration processes and in the basic sensory processing of auditory tonal stimuli were found to be related to the severity of clinical symptoms in ASD children (Brandwein et al., 2015). This would suggest to us the possibility that while impaired communication among sensory cortices is part of the broader autism phenotype, protective factors may serve to “rescue” multisensory speech processing functions in females with ASD. Alternatively, it is also possible that even basic multisensory integrative processing is spared in females with ASD. Future work will be required to determine the extent to which this sparing is observed for other types of multisensory integrative processes, and whether it extends to non-social processing.

A limitation of this study is that the generalization of our findings from our ASD sample is only possible to the population of high functioning individuals with ASD. Using tasks adapted to individuals with low functioning ASD, future research may determine whether sex differences can also be observed in this population.

In conclusion this study provides evidence for sex differences in the ability to integrate heard and seen speech under noisy environmental conditions in a large sample of typically developing children and teenagers between the ages of 5 and 17 years. These differences were absent in a sample of healthy adults. We further show that multisensory speech processing is less affected in ASD females than males. Our findings underline the importance of considering sex differences in the search for autism endophenotypes. An advanced understanding of sex differences in ASD may benefit our understanding of the genetic, neurobiological and environmental factors involved in the development of ASD. Basic research on sex differences in ASD has practical implications for therapeutic intervention and may inform clinicians to delineate more personalized treatments for this diverse disorder.

Author Contributions

JF, LR, and SM conceived the project, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. VDB and LR collected the data. VDB provided support with editing the manuscript. LR constructed the stimulus set, and HPF conducted and analyzed the eye-tracking component of the experiments. All authors discussed the results, commented on the manuscript and approved the final version.

Funding

Primary support for this work was provided through a grant from the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health (MH085322 to JF and SM). Additional support during protocol development was provided by Cure Autism Now (JF) and The Wallace Research Foundation (JF and SM). The Human Clinical Phenotyping Core, where the children enrolled in this study were recruited and clinically evaluated, is a facility of the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (RFK-IDDRC) which is funded by a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD P30 HD071593).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to express their sincere gratitude to Dr. Alice Brandwein, Ms. Sarah Ruberman, Mr. Frantzy Acluche and Mr. Gregory Peters who provided invaluable assistance during data collection. We thank Dr. Juliana Bates for her clinical insights and for performing a large proportion of the clinical phenotyping in our ASD cohort.

References

Aloufy, S., Lapidot, M., and Myslobodsky, M. (1996). Differences in susceptibility to the “blending illusion” among Native Hebrew and English speakers. Brain Lang. 53, 51–57. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asperger, H., and Frith, U. T. (1991). “‘Autistic psychopathy’ in childhood,” in Autism and Asperger Syndrome (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 37–92. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511526770.002

CrossRef Full Text

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychol. Sci. 20, 144–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02279.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Chapman, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., and Hackett, G. (2006). Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 155(Suppl. 1), S123–S130. doi: 10.1530/eje.1.02260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 248–254. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., and Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 813–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., and Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science 310, 819–823. doi: 10.1126/science.1115455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., O'Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., and Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 407–418. doi: 10.1023/A:1023035012436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., and Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 361–374. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Scott, F. J., Allison, C., Williams, J., Bolton, P., Matthews, F. E., et al. (2009). Prevalence of autism-spectrum conditions: UK school-based population study. Br. J. Psychiatry 194, 500–509. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.059345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Duketis, E., Poustka, F., and Holtmann, M. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism 15, 497–511. doi: 10.1177/1362361310391116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bondy, C. A., Hougen, H. Y., Zhou, J., and Cheng, C. M. (2012). Genomic imprinting and Turner syndrome. Pediatr. Endocrinol. Rev. 9(Suppl. 2), 728–732. doi: 10.1016/j.ics.2006.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosacki, S., and Astington, J. (1999). Theory of mind in preadolescents: relations between language and theory-of-mind development. Dev. Psychol. 5, 1311–1320.

PubMed Abstract

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Frey, H. P., Bates, J. C., Shulman, L. H., et al. (2015). Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. J Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 230–244. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2212-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Russo, N. N., Altschuler, T. S., Gomes, H., et al. (2013). The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: high-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1329–1341. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calero, C. I., Salles, A., Semelman, M., and Sigman, M. (2013). Age and gender dependent development of Theory of Mind in 6- to 8-years old children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:281. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, A. S., Black, D. O., Tewani, S., Connolly, C. E., Kadlec, M. B., and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Sex differences in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 86–97. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0331-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). (2014). “Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder—Autism and developmental disability monitoring network, 11 sites United States, 2010,” in Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries, Vol. 63, 1–21.

PubMed Abstract

Chapman, E., Baron-Cohen, S., Auyeung, B., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., and Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 135–148. doi: 10.1080/17470910600992239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: characteristics and consequences. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 38, 1–109. doi: 10.2307/1165928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 33, 497–505. doi: 10.1080/14640748108400805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., and Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behav. Dev. 23, 113–118. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(00)00032-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dworzynski, K., Ronald, A., Bolton, P., and Happe, F. (2012). How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 788–797. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., and Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59, 1–173. discussion: 174–185. doi: 10.2307/1166093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiological surveys of pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2, 33–68. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., et al. (2015). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 25, 298–312. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frey, H.-P., Wirz, K. T., Willenbockel, V., Betz, T., Schreiber, C., Troscianko, T., et al. (2011). Beyond correlation: do color features influence attention in rainforest? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:36. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giarelli, E., Wiggins, L. D., Rice, C. E., Levy, S. E., Kirby, R. S., Pinto-Martin, J., et al. (2010). Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children. Disabil. Health J. 3, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goin-Kochel, R. P., Abbacchi, A., Constantino, J. N., and Autism Genetic Resource Exchange Consortium. (2007). Lack of evidence for increased genetic loading for autism among families of affected females: a replication from family history data in two large samples. Autism 11, 279–286. doi: 10.1177/1362361307076857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A. (1985). Nonverbal Sex Differences: Communication Accuracy and Expressive Style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hallmayer, J., Cleveland, S., Torres, A., Phillips, J., Cohen, B., Torigoe, T., et al. (2011). Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 1095–1102. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happe, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Dev. 66, 843–855. doi: 10.2307/1131954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartley, S. L., and Sikora, D. M. (2009). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: an examination of developmental functioning, autistic symptoms, and coexisting behavior problems in toddlers. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1715–1722. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0810-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatcher, R., Hatcher, S., Berlin, M., Okla, K., and Richards, J. (1990). Psychological mindedness and abstract reasoning in late childhood and adolescence: an exploration using new instruments. J. Youth Adolesc. 19, 307–326. doi: 10.1007/BF01537075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hattier, M. A., Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., and Horovitz, M. (2011). The effects of gender and age on repetitive and/or restricted behaviors and interests in adults with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 2346–2351. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtmann, M., Bolte, S., and Poustka, F. (2007). Autism spectrum disorders: sex differences in autistic behaviour domains and coexisting psychopathology. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 361–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00361.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27:236. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, J. S., and Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 104:53. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.104.1.53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingudomnukul, E., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., and Knickmeyer, R. (2007). Elevated rates of testosterone-related disorders in women with autism spectrum conditions. Horm. Behav. 51, 597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, J. R., Whalen, D. H., and Fowler, C. A. (2006). A sex difference in visual influence on heard speech. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 582–592. doi: 10.3758/BF03208760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, F. M., Hicks, L. H., Goldberg, T., and Myslobodsky, M. S. (1988). Sex differences in lipreading. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 26, 106–108. doi: 10.3758/BF03334875

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, R. P., and Durfee, J. T. (1978). Effects of stress on attachment behavior in infants. J. Genet. Psychol. 132, 321–322.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Knickmeyer, R. C., Wheelwright, S., and Baron-Cohen, S. B. (2008). Sex-typical play: masculinization/defeminization in girls with an autism spectrum condition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1028–1035. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0475-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kucera, H. F., and Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational Analysis of Present-Day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE 6:e20835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N., Chakrabarti, B., Wheelwright, S. J., Auyeung, B., et al. (2012). Cognition in males and females with autism: similarities and differences. PLoS ONE 7:e47198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Suckling, J., Ruigrok, A. N., Chakrabarti, B., Ecker, C., et al. (2013). Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism. Brain 136(Pt 9), 2799–2815. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., and Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 34, 3–27. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 205–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M., and Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundström, S., Chang, Z., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., Larsson, H., Anckarsäter, H., et al. (2012). Autism spectrum disorders and autisticlike traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 46–52. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, W. J., Zhou, X., Ross, L. A., Foxe, J. J., and Parra, L. C. (2009). Lip-reading aids word recognition most in moderate noise: a Bayesian explanation using high-dimensional feature space. PLoS ONE 4:e4638. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malatesta, C. Z., and Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: the socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Dev. 53, 991–1003. doi: 10.2307/1129139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., Salter, G., Seigal, A., and Skuse, D. (2012). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from a large sample of children and adolescents. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1304–1313. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuyoshi, D., Kuraguchi, K., Tanaka, Y., Uchida, S., Ashida, H., and Watanabe, K. (2014). Individual differences in autistic traits predict the perception of direct gaze for males, but not for females. Mol. Autism 5, 1–4. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGurk, H., and MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 746–748. doi: 10.1038/264746a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLennan, J. D., Lord, C., and Schopler, E. (1993). Sex differences in higher functioning people with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 23, 217–227. doi: 10.1007/BF01046216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J. W., Foxe, J. J., Peters, J. B., and Molholm, S. (2014). Susceptibility to distraction in autism spectrum disorder: probing the integrity of oscillatory alpha-band suppression mechanisms. Autism Res. 7, 442–458. doi: 10.1002/aur.1374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhrström, N., and Traunmüller, H. (2004). Audiovisual perception of Swedish vowels with and without conflicting cues. Proceedings FoNEtIk 2004, 40–43.

Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Shulman, C., and Dover, R. (1998). The autism diagnostic interview-revised and the childhood autism rating scale: differences between diagnostic systems and comparison between genders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 28, 143–151. doi: 10.1023/A:1026092632466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E. B., Koenen, K. C., McCormick, M. C., Munir, K., Hallett, V., Happé, F., et al. (2011). Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5%, and 1%). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 1113–1121. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, E. B., Lichtenstein, P., Anckarsater, H., Happe, F., and Ronald, A. (2013). Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 5258–5262. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211070110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., and Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to Nonverbal Communication: The PONS Test: Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Google Scholar

Ross, L. A., Molholm, S., Blanco, D., Gomez−Ramirez, M., Saint−Amour, D., and Foxe, J. J. (2011). The development of multisensory speech perception continues into the late childhood years. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 2329–2337. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07685.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. A., Saint-Amour, D., Leavitt, V. M., Javitt, D. C., and Foxe, J. J. (2007a). Do you see what I am saying? Exploring visual enhancement of speech comprehension in noisy environments. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1147–1153. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. A., Saint-Amour, D., Leavitt, V. M., Molholm, S., Javitt, D. C., and Foxe, J. J. (2007b). Impaired multisensory processing in schizophrenia: deficits in the visual enhancement of speech comprehension under noisy environmental conditions. Schizophr. Res. 97, 173–183. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, G., Steer, C., and Golding, J. (2011). Social and demographic factors that influence the diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 46, 1283–1293. doi: 10.1007/s00127-010-0294-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, N., Foxe, J. J., Brandwein, A. B., Altschuler, T., Gomes, H., and Molholm, S. (2010). Multisensory processing in children with autism: high-density electrical mapping of auditory-somatosensory integration. Autism Res. 3, 253–267. doi: 10.1002/aur.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saint-Amour, D., De Sanctis, P., Molholm, S., Ritter, W., and Foxe, J. J. (2007). Seeing voices: high-density electrical mapping and source-analysis of the multisensory mismatch negativity evoked during the McGurk illusion. Neuropsychologia 45, 587–597. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A., and Johnson, M. H. (2009). Atypical eye contact in autism: models, mechanisms and development. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 1204–1214. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H. (2000). Imprinting, the X-chromosome, and the male brain: explaining sex differences in the liability to autism. Pediatr. Res. 47, 9–16. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200001000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H., James, R. S., Bishop, D. V., Coppin, B., Dalton, P., Aamodt-Leeper, G., et al. (1997). Evidence from Turner's syndrome of an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cognitive function. Nature 387, 705–708. doi: 10.1038/42706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, M., Miller, M., Taylor, S. L., Hinshaw, S. P., and Carter, C. S. (2012). Autism symptoms and internalizing psychopathology in girls and boys with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 48–59. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1215-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, M., and Karraker, K. H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: a review of gender labeling studies. Sex Roles 20, 501–522. doi: 10.1007/BF00288198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szatmari, P., Liu, X. Q., Goldberg, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Paterson, A. D., Woodbury-Smith, M., et al. (2012). Sex differences in repetitive stereotyped behaviors in autism: implications for genetic liability. Am. J. Med. Genet. 159B, 5–12. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, L. Y., and Beisler, J. M. (1983). The development of sex differences in infantile autism. Br. J. Psychiatry 142, 373–378. doi: 10.1192/bjp.142.4.373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, C. S., Qiu, W. W., Chamberlain, M. M., and Li, X. (1996). Auditory and visual speech perception: confirmation of a modality-independent source of individual differences in speech recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100(Pt 1), 1153–1162. doi: 10.1121/1.416300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werling, D. M., and Geschwind, D. H. (2013). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 26, 146–153. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, cross-modal, development, gender, audio-visual, ASD, language, speech perception

Citation: Ross LA, Del Bene VA, Molholm S, Frey H-P and Foxe JJ (2015) Sex differences in multisensory speech processing in both typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum. Front. Neurosci. 9:185. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00185

Received: 20 March 2015; Accepted: 11 May 2015;
Published: 27 May 2015.

Edited by:

Gaurav Jain, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, USA

Reviewed by:

Karen M. Smith, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, USA
Stephanie Ameis, University of Toronto, Canada

Copyright © 2015 Ross, Del Bene, Molholm, Frey and Foxe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lars A. Ross and John J. Foxe, The Sheryl and Daniel R. Tishman Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory Albert Einstein College of Medicine, 1225 Morris Park Avenue, Van Etten Building - Wing 1C, Bronx, NY 10461, USA, lars.ross@einstein.yu.edu; john.foxe@einstein.yu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.