Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 01 June 2016
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00251

Holding Biological Motion in Working Memory: An fMRI Study

  • 1Department of Psychology, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 2Network Center, Women’s Hospital, School of Medicine, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 3Department of Speech and Hearing Science, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
  • 4Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China

Holding biological motion (BM), the movements of animate entities, in working memory (WM) is important to our daily life activities. However, the neural substrates underlying the WM processing of BM remain largely unknown. Employing the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technique, the current study directly investigated this issue. We used point-light BM animations as the tested stimuli, and explored the neural substrates involved in encoding and retaining BM information in WM. Participants were required to remember two or four BM stimuli in a change-detection task. We first defined a set of potential brain regions devoted to the BM processing in WM in one experiment. We then conducted the second fMRI experiment, and performed time-course analysis over the pre-defined regions, which allowed us to differentiate the encoding and maintenance phases of WM. The results showed that a set of brain regions were involved in encoding BM into WM, including the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, inferior parietal lobule, superior temporal sulcus, fusiform gyrus, and middle occipital gyrus. However, only the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, and inferior parietal lobule were involved in retaining BM into WM. These results suggest that an overlapped network exists between the WM encoding and maintenance for BM; however, retaining BM in WM predominately relies on the mirror neuron system.

Introduction

Processing biological motion (BM), the movements of animate entities (Johansson, 1973; see Troje, 2013 for a review), is vital to daily life activities such as social interaction, motor learning, and non-verbal communication (for reviews, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Steel et al., 2014). To process BM information, our visual system has evolved a sophisticated mechanism that can be demonstrated clearly via point-light displays (PLDs; Johansson, 1973). PLDs isolate human kinematics by depicting human activity via a few (e.g., 12) light points. Although highly impoverished, once in motion, these PLDs can be rapidly recognized as coherent, meaningful movements. Moreover, multiple aspects of social information can be extracted from PLDs, including walking direction, gender, interactions with other people, and emotion, even if the PLDs are presented within random dynamic noise (for reviews, see Puce and Perrett, 2003; Blakemore, 2008; Troje, 2013; Steel et al., 2014). However, to form coherent BM representations in the mind and guide our social behavior, we need to not only process the BM in perception but also retain the BM information in working memory (WM; e.g., Wood, 2007, 2011; Shen et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2015), which stores and manipulates a limited set of information (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 2012). Indeed, it has been suggested that the WM capacity of BM, but not other stimuli (e.g., colors, moving rectangles), is positively correlated with one’s empathy level (Gao et al., 2016). Therefore, it is important to elucidate the processing mechanisms of BM in WM.

Currently, there have been few studies exploring the mechanisms of retaining BM in WM. Most of these studies focused on the storage mechanisms of BM and BM-related bindings in WM. It has been demonstrated that BM has a storage buffer in WM that is independent from location, color, and shape, and can retain 3–4 BM stimuli (Smyth et al., 1988; Smyth and Pendleton, 1989, 1990; Wood, 2007, 2011; Shen et al., 2014). However, storing BM-related bindings in WM is quite resource-exhausting; for instance, only 1–2 BM-color bindings can be held in WM (Wood, 2008; Ding et al., 2015). Moreover, in contrast to robust object-based encoding in WM for common visual objects, in which irrelevant information is involuntarily encoded with relevant information (e.g., both shape and color when only the color of colored shapes is required to be stored in WM), there is no object-based encoding for colored BM stimuli (Ding et al., 2015). However, all the aforementioned studies have addressed BM processing in WM via behavioral methods. Thus, the neural mechanisms underlying WM processing of BM remain largely unknown.

To the best of our knowledge, only one study has examined the neural mechanisms of BM processing in WM. Gao et al. (2015) investigated whether the mirror neuron system (MNS) was involved in retaining BM information in WM using PLDs as stimuli. The MNS refers to a neural network that includes the ventral premotor cortex (vPMC), posterior inferior frontal gyrus (pIFG), and rostral inferior parietal lobule (rIPL; e.g., Saygin et al., 2004; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009; Grosbras et al., 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012). The MNS has a “mirror” mechanism in the sense that the same set of neurons is activated both when a person performs an action and when he/she observes another person performing the same action (e.g., Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). It has been demonstrated that the MNS is significantly activated when an individual observes or imagines a BM stimulus (Grèzes et al., 2001; Vaina et al., 2001; Servos et al., 2002; Saygin et al., 2004; Newman-Norlund et al., 2010; for reviews, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Pavlova, 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012), which implies that the observers employ their own cortical motor system to simulate perceived movements and to understand the displayed stimuli (e.g., Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 2001; Oberman et al., 2007). To test the MNS hypothesis, Gao et al. (2015) recruited an electroencephalograph (EEG) index of Mu-suppression, which is suggested to be closely linked to the MNS (Kuhlman, 1978; Oberman and Ramachandran, 2007; Ulloa and Pineda, 2007; Perry et al., 2010; Singh et al., 2011; Hogeveen et al., 2015). If the MNS underlies the BM maintenance, then the BM memory load should modulate the degree of Mu-suppression. Gao et al. (2015) found that Mu-suppression increased as the BM memory load increased from 2 to 4 and reached a plateau at 4 BM stimuli, which confirms the MNS prediction and is consistent with the fact that WM can store a maximum of 3–4 BM stimuli (cf. Shen et al., 2014). Moreover, the difference in Mu-suppression between memorizing 2 and 4 BM stimuli was correlated with the number of stored BM stimuli in WM; this correlation was not observed for non-BM stimuli (e.g., moving circles).

However, although Gao et al. (2015) linked the MNS to the maintenance of BM information in WM, the neural substrates supporting BM processing in WM remain unclear. Specifically, the poor spatial resolution of EEG prevents a thorough examination of the specific neural substrates for retaining BM in WM. Gao et al. (2015) also did not localize the source of Mu. Furthermore, in addition to the MNS, it has been demonstrated that the superior temporal sulcus (STS), which is a critical region in social cognition in humans (e.g., Allison et al., 2000; Keysers and Perrett, 2004; Mar, 2011; Gao et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2014), is among the core regions in charge of BM processing in perception (e.g., Saygin, 2007; Grossman et al., 2000, 2005; for reviews, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Grosbras et al., 2012; Pavlova, 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012). For instance, BM stimuli induce larger STS activation compared to non-BM stimuli (e.g., scrambled or inverted BM stimuli; Grossman et al., 2000; Grèzes et al., 2001; Puce and Perrett, 2003; Saygin et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2005; Saygin, 2007). However, Gao et al. (2015) only implied that the MNS is involved in the WM maintenance of BM. Thus, it remains unclear whether other cortical regions (particularly the STS) are also involved in BM maintenance and whether distinct brain regions are involved at different processing phases of WM, which was investigated in this study.

To this end, we adopted high spatial-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the neural substrates supporting the WM processing of BM in a change detection task, which is commonly used in WM studies (e.g., Luck and Vogel, 1997; Wheeler and Treisman, 2002; Xu, 2002; Wood, 2007, 2011; Shen et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2015, 2016). In a typical change detection task, WM processing involves at least three distinct phases: Encoding, maintenance, and retrieval (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006, 2007). Here, we focused on the encoding and maintenance phases1, and used time-course analysis to differentiate distinct WM phases (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006). We required participants to memorize BM stimuli via the PLDs technique, and examined the cortical regions that were modulated by the memory load of BM stimuli. Based on our previous EEG study of BM, which revealed that the largest load effect was observed between 2 and 4 BM stimuli (Gao et al., 2015; as is commonly used in other WM studies; e.g., Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Gao et al., 2009; Luria and Vogel, 2011), we required participants to retain 2 or 4 BM stimuli in the task. Additionally, the BM stimuli in the memory array were selected from distinct categories with relatively long exposure times (usually at least 1 s). Under this setting, participants are likely to verbally recode the BM stimuli (e.g., labeling a BM stimulus as walking) instead of storing the motion information of the BM. To avoid this verbal strategy without impairing the storage of real BM in WM, most WM studies on BM have used a dual-task setting in which a secondary articulatory suppression task (e.g., repeating three digits throughout the course of a trial) was added to the BM memory task (e.g., Wood, 2007, 2011; Shen et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2015, 2016). It is of note that this method has also been well-accepted in WM studies (including fMRI studies) exploring other visual stimuli by assuming that the articulatory suppression task consumes resources from verbal WM and that visual stimuli require a distinct type of resource in visual WM (e.g., Vogel et al., 2001; Todd and Marois, 2004; Allen et al., 2009; see Baddeley, 2012, for a review). Our recent EEG study (Gao et al., 2015) further supports the necessity of adding a secondary verbal task to the BM memory task: Whereas significant Mu-suppression in the WM maintenance phase of BM was observed when a secondary verbal task was added, Mu-suppression vanished when the secondary task was removed. Consequently, in this study, we followed the procedures of previous WM studies of BM and required participants to covertly rehearse three numerical digits throughout the trial.

Regions of interest (ROIs) are required to conduct the time-course analysis. Because this is the first neuroimaging study to directly explore the neural substrates of BM in WM, we initially conducted a pilot study to determine possible ROIs that contribute to the WM processing of BM. We then conducted a formal experiment to measure the effects of BM load on these ROIs. Furthermore, the purpose of the current study was to reveal all potential neural substrates for holding BM in WM, including cortical regions specific to BM as well as cortical regions for processing common visual information. Therefore, we used a common fixation baseline in which participants fixated on a red cross in the center of the screen while covertly rehearsing three digits (referred to henceforth as the fixation-baseline)2, which has been used in previous WM studies (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006), instead of using scrambled PLDs or inverted PLDs (for reviews see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Pavlova, 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012).

Experiment for Defining ROIs

Participants

Seventeen (11 male) neurologically normal participants (19–31 years old; 22.5 ± 3.4 years old) were paid to participate in this study. All participants were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and had not previously participated in an MRI experiment. Two male participants and one female participant were removed from further analysis because of chance level performance on the memory task. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Board of Zhejiang University and granting agency. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Stimulus and Procedure

In line with our previous BM studies of WM (Shen et al., 2014; Ding et al., 2015; Gao et al., 2015, 2016), six BM stimuli were selected from the Vanrie and Verfaillie (2004) database: Cycle, jump, paint, spade, walk, and wave. Each stimulus consisted of 30 distinct frames that were each displayed twice in succession, leading to a 1-s animation under a refresh rate of 60 Hz. Stimuli were displayed at a visual angle of approximately 1.35° × 1.35° and positioned at five potential locations that were uniformly distributed within an invisible circle (radius 4.02° of visual angle) aligned in the center of the screen.

Each trial began with three white digits presented on a black background (0, 0, 0 in RGB space) for 500 ms (Figure 1). Participants were instructed to rehearse these digits (e.g., by stating “nine,” “two,” and “three” instead of “nine hundred twenty-three”) covertly throughout the trial to prevent verbal coding of the stimuli (e.g., Vogel et al., 2001; Gao et al., 2015). Next, a red fixation cross (0.41° × 0.41° visual angle) was presented for 1,500 ms to prime the upcoming memory task. Then, the memory array was presented for 2,000 (load 2) or 4,000 (load 4) ms (i.e., 1 s per BM) to ensure participants had sufficient encoding time. Following a delay period of 6,000 ms during which a red fixation cross was presented, a red BM probe was presented in the center of the screen. Participants judged whether the BM had appeared in the memory array by pressing one of the two buttons within 2,000 ms, followed by a 1,000 ms feedback. Finally, a red digit was presented in the center of the screen for 2,000 ms, and participants had to determine whether it was among the three rehearsed digits, followed by a 1,000 ms feedback. The red colors in both the BM probe and digit probe were used to inform participants that the displayed item was the probe. Participants were instructed to respond as accurately as possible without worrying about the response time.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. A schematic illustration of a single trial. Each trial began with the presentation of three digits (500 ms). After a fixation of 1500 ms two or four BM stimuli were presented for 2000 or 4000 ms, respectively. A 6000 ms fixation was followed by a 2000 ms probe. Participants judged whether the probe had appeared in the memory array, and was given feedback. Finally, the digit probe was presented. The participants responded within 2000 ms, and were given feedback.

Additionally, after each memory-task trial there was a fixation-baseline trial, which was further followed by a 2,000 ms blank interval. For the fixation-baseline trial, three white digits were first presented for 500 ms. Participants were instructed to rehearse these digits covertly, as described above. Then, a red fixation cross was presented for 7,500 ms, during which participants continued to rehearse the digits.

The entire experiment included two runs, each with 16 pseudo-randomly presented trials (eight memory-task trials for load 2 and 4 each) and 16 fixation-baseline trials. Each run lasted 7 min and 48 s. Before the formal experiment, participants completed a practice session (16 trials) outside the scanner to become familiar with the task.

Data Acquisition

Participants lay in a supine position inside a GE 3T scanner and viewed the display through a mirror. Foam pads were used to prevent head movement. Displays were presented via a visual and audio stimulation system for fMRI (Shenzhen Sinorad Medical Electronics Co., Ltd, Type SA-9900). Stimuli were created and presented in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and Psychophysics Toolbox software (Brainard, 1997). A trigger pulse from the scanner synchronized the onset of stimulus presentation at the beginning of the image acquisition.

Information about the hemodynamic response was obtained using single-shot, T2-weighted, echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequences. The acquisition parameters for the anatomical images were as follows: Repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms; echo time (TE) = 30 ms; flip angle (FA) = 90°; and field of view (FOV) = 200 mm × 200 mm. We acquired 33 axial slices (thickness = 4 mm, gap = 1 mm, in-plane resolution = 64 × 64). In addition, T1-weighted sagittal images were collected for slice localization, with acquisition parameters as follows: Short time inversion recovery (STIR) sequence; TR = 1767.42 ms; TE = 20.616 ms; FA = 90°; FOV = 200 mm × 200 mm; 33 slices, slice thickness = 4 mm, gap = 1 mm, and in-plane resolution = 512 × 512.

fMRI Data Pre-processing

fMRI data processing and analysis were performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) 8 (Friston et al., 1994)3. Functional images were corrected for slice acquisition time differences and then were corrected for head motion using a rigid body correction in SPM. None of the participants moved more than 3 mm in translational or 3° in rotational dimensions. Functional images then were co-registered with the anatomical images, which were segmented into gray matter and white matter. Anatomical images were spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, and normalization parameters were applied to the functional images. Normalized images were smoothed using a Gaussian filter with a full width at half maximum of 8 mm.

fMRI Data Analysis

Individual data were analyzed by creating a general linear model in SPM 8. For each memory load condition, three regressors were defined: Encoding [2 (for load 2) or 4 (for load 4) seconds of the memory array], maintenance (6 s of the delay after the memory array), and retrieval (2 s of the probe). Because we used two memory load conditions with different exposure times, each processing phase had two types of regressors: One for memory load 2, one for memory load 4. Additionally, there was a regressor for the fixation-baseline trial (the last 6 s of the fixation-baseline). This procedure resulted in seven regressors in total. All regressors were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function.

Statistical analyses were based on random-effects analysis of variance (ANOVA). To remove the effect of rehearsing digits on the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal of processing BM in WM, we compared the BOLD activity between the WM phases (i.e., encoding and maintenance, separately) and the fixation-baseline. Additionally, because the exposure times of the memory array were different for loads 2 and 4, we did not compare the BOLD activity between loads 2 and 4, to avoid potential confounding due to unbalanced exposure times. Instead, we conducted the comparison separately for load 2 and load 4 to obtain all possible ROIs devoted to BM processing in WM. This comparison resulted in four contrasts: Comparison between encoding for load 2 and fixation-baseline (Encoding-Two), comparison between encoding for load 4 and fixation-baseline (Encoding-Four), comparison between maintenance for load 2 and fixation-baseline (Maintenance-Two), and comparison between maintenance for load 4 and fixation-baseline (Maintenance-Four). A one-sample t-test was conducted for each contrast. A suprathreshold statistical map (punc < 0.005, uncorrected) of each condition was used to plot ROIs, which were defined by spheres with 6-mm radii centered at the maxima of each suprathreshold cluster (see Table 1). Each ROI contained 33 voxels.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. MNI coordinates of the coordinates of ROI center defined with suprathreshold cluster.

Accuracy on the memory task was used to examine the effect of WM load. Cowan’s formula (cf. Rouder et al., 2011) was employed to estimate the WM capacity of BM: K = S (HF), where K is WM capacity, S is the number of stimuli, H is the hit rate, and F is the false alarm rate. We calculated the K-values for each memory load of each participant, and set the largest K-value (Kmax) as the estimated capacity of WM for each participant (e.g., Shen et al., 2014).

Results

Behavioral Results

The overall accuracy of the digit task was relatively high (M = 0.90; SD = 0.12), suggesting that participants concentrated on covert digit rehearsal. For the accuracy of the BM task, a paired t-test revealed that the accuracy for load 2 (M = 0.92; SD = 0.10) was significantly higher than that for load 4 [M = 0.85; SD = 0.08; t(13) = 2.874, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.818]. By using Cowan’s formula, we found that participants could maintain 2–3 BM (Kmax = 2.75) in WM.

fMRI Results

Figure 2 shows the suprathreshold statistical map in each condition. Brain regions activated in the Encoding-Two condition included the bilateral superior parietal lobe [SPL, Brodmann Area (BA) 7], bilateral fusiform gyrus (FG, BA37), and bilateral middle occipital gyrus (MOG, BA19). Brain regions activated in the Encoding-Four condition included the bilateral SPL (BA7) and left FG (BA37). Brain regions activated in the Maintenance-Two condition included the bilateral middle frontal gyrus (MFG, BA6), left MFG (BA9), and left rIPL (BA40). Brain regions activated in the Maintenance-Four condition included the bilateral MFG (BA9), left MFG (BA46), right pIFG (BA47), and left rIPL (BA40). Please see Table 1 for details.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Brain regions activated in experiment for defining ROI (punc < 0.005, k > 10). (A) ROIs for encoding two BM stimuli, (B) ROIs for encoding four BM stimuli, (C) ROIs for retaining two BM stimuli, (D) ROIs for retaining four BM stimuli.

Next, we combined the ROIs of the same anatomical region in each hemisphere to generate a comprehensive list of ROIs for the time-course analysis. This procedure resulted in 13 different ROIs: left MFG (BA46), right pIFG (BA47), left MFG (BA9), right MFG (BA9), left MFG (BA6), right MFG (BA6), left rIPL (BA40), left SPL (BA7), right SPL (BA7), left FG (BA37), right FG (BA37), left MOG (BA19), and right MOG (BA19). Additionally, to have a direct comparison with BM perception studies, we added two ROIs (6-mm radius) revealed by a meta-analysis as neural substrates supporting BM perception (Grosbras et al., 2012): left STS (-52, -50, 4; BA21) and left vPMC (-50, 8, 28; BA6). It should be noted that the left vPMC is embedded in the left MFG (BA6) in the current study.

Experiment for Time-Course Analysis

Methods

Twenty-four (12 male) neurologically normal participants (20–24 years old; 22.4 ± 2.93 years old) were paid to participate in the study. Two female participants were removed from further analysis because their level of performance on the memory task did not exceed the chance level. Two additional female participants were excluded from analysis due to a lack of time-locked fMRI signals (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006). Therefore, the final analysis was conducted on 20 participants.

Study procedures were identical to the experiment for defining ROIs session, except for the aspects described below. Critically, the exposure time of the memory array was fixed at 4,000 ms for loads 2 and 4, such that the exposure time was identical during the encoding phase between the two load conditions, enabling us to directly compare BOLD activities between load 2 and 4. Additionally, the delay between the memory array and the BM probe was extended to 8,000 ms, allowing the hemodynamic response to reach its peak (e.g., Xu and Chun, 2006), and the feedback was shortened to 500 ms. The fixation-baseline was constructed by replacing the BM task in the memory-task condition (16 s in total) with a 16-s fixation while the digit rehearsal task remained (including presentation, response, and feedback for the digits). Fixation-baseline trial hence had the same duration as the memory-task trial. Following pervious fMRI studies that employed time-course analysis (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006), we took the fixation-baseline trial as an condition, which resulted in three experimental conditions: Load 2, load 4, and fixation-baseline. There were three runs, each containing 18 pseudo-randomly presented trials (six trials each for load 2, load 4, and fixation-baseline), with a 2,000 ms blank interval between each trial. Each run lasted 6 min and 4 s. Before the formal experiment, participants completed a practice session (18 trials) outside the scanner to become familiar with the task.

For each participant, we used MarsBar ROI toolbox for SPM4 to extract fMRI signals from the 15 ROIs that were pre-defined in the experiment for defining ROIs. The signals from each ROI were segmented and averaged for each stimulus condition. These time courses then were converted to the percentage signal change for each condition by subtracting the corresponding value of the fixation-baseline and then dividing by that value (cf. Xu and Chun, 2006). Following previous ROI studies (e.g., Rypma and D’Esposito, 1999; Koshino et al., 2005; Xu and Chun, 2006), a paired t-test was conducted separately for the percentage signal change of each ROI with memory load as the within-subjects factor. Additionally, we also adopted Bayes factor (Jeffery–Zellner–Siow Prior Bayes factor5; Rouder et al., 2009) to estimate the likelihood of the null hypothesis over the alternative hypothesis.

Results

Behavioral Results

The overall accuracy of the digit task was relatively high (M = 0.97; SD = 0.03), suggesting that participants concentrated on covert digit rehearsal. For the accuracy of BM memory task, a paired t-test revealed that the accuracy for load 2 (M = 0.90; SD = 0.08) was significantly higher than that for load 4 [M = 0.83; SD = 0.10; t(19) = 2.458, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.831]. By using Cowan’s formula, we found that the participants could maintain 2–3 BM (Kmax = 2.71) in WM.

fMRI Results

Because the accuracy in each condition was relatively high, all trials were analyzed. Figure 3 shows the activations extracted from the pre-defined ROIs. Two peaks were observed, corresponding to the fMRI signals for the memory onset (8–10 s) and the probe onset (18–20 s), respectively. We selected and averaged the signals between 8 and 10 s for the encoding phase, and the signals between 14 and 16 s for the maintenance phase.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Activations during the encoding (E), maintenance (M), and retrieval (R) phases of WM task (gray bars). Two or four memory stimuli were presented 4000 ms. Green arrow shows the onset time of the memory array, while the purple arrow indicates the presentation of the probe.

Table 2 summarizes the results. For WM encoding, all ROIs exhibited larger activation for load 4 than for load 2. For WM maintenance, only the following nine ROIs exhibited larger activation for load 4 than for load 2: Left MFG (BA6), right MFG (BA6), left MFG (BA9), left MFG (BA46), right pIFG (BA47), left SPL (BA7), right SPL (BA7), left rIPL (BA40), and left vPMC (BA6).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. BM load effect in WM encoding and maintenance.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to directly investigate the neural substrates underlying BM processing in WM by focusing on the WM encoding and maintenance phases. To fully understand the neural substrates underlying BM storage in WM, the current study followed the procedures outlined in Xu and Chun (2006) by comparing activation during the encoding and maintenance phases to a fixation-baseline instead of a scrambled PLD. To directly compare to previous BM perception studies, we also included two other ROIs (STS and vPMC), which are suggested as the neural substrates underlying BM perception (Grosbras et al., 2012). We found that all activations in the pre-defined 15 ROIs were positively modulated by the memory load of BM in the WM encoding phase, suggesting that these areas were involved in the encoding of visual information conveyed by the BM. Moreover, most of these pre-defined ROIs were also contributed to the maintenance of BM information in WM, including the left MFG (BA6), right MFG (BA6), left MFG (BA9), left MFG (BA46), right pIFG (BA47), left SPL (BA7), right SPL (BA7), left rIPL (BA40), and left vPMC (BA6).

We found that the classic constituent regions of MNS, which have been revealed in previous BM perception studies, were involved in both WM encoding and WM maintenance of BM. These results are consistent with previous studies showing that the MNS is dedicated to BM processing (e.g., Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Saygin et al., 2004; Grosbras et al., 2012; Pavlova, 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012), and also extend the function of the MNS from BM perception to WM. The results of the current study are also congruent with the EEG findings in Gao et al. (2015), as the current study provides direct fMRI evidence that the MNS is indeed devoted to the rehearsal of BM information in WM. Therefore, both EEG and fMRI evidence suggest that we retain BM stimuli by simulating BM via our cortical motor system (Cook et al., 2014). The current study also offers the first fMRI evidence in support of the “theory of event coding,” (Hommel et al., 2001; see Prinz, 1997 for a similar claim) which predicts that the same mental representations are employed when memorizing an action as when executing that same action. Additionally, the current finding provides indirect evidence that the EEG Mu-suppression signal has a close relationship with the MNS (Kuhlman, 1978; Oberman and Ramachandran, 2007; Ulloa and Pineda, 2007; Perry et al., 2010; Singh et al., 2011; Hogeveen et al., 2015). Moreover, it is worthwhile to note that, except for the vPMC (BA6), other regions of the MFG were also activated. These extra regions of the MFG may be related to general visual processing involved in BM (see discussion below).

The STS is a key region in BM perception (Pavlova, 2012). We found that the memory load is significantly modulated by STS activation in WM encoding but not in WM maintenance. Previous neuroimaging studies have suggested that the STS integrates information derived from both the ventral ‘what’ and the dorsal ‘where’ visual pathways to construct a high-level representation of actions (for review, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007), which provide the motoric aspects of the action to be imitated by the MNS (for review, see Lacoboni and Dapretto, 2006). In the current WM encoding phase, BM perception must occur in order to construct integrated BM percepts; therefore, it is reasonable to find that STS activation was modulated by the memory load. Based on these findings, however, it remains unclear why STS activation was not observed during the WM maintenance phase. There are at least two explanations for this finding. First, it is possible that the role of STS in BM integration during the encoding phase negates the need for STS activation during the maintenance phase. Given that the MNS is employed to rehearse the integrated BM (cf. Gao et al., 2015; and the aforementioned MNS results), the STS may not be needed for reconstruction of the BM. This possibility is somewhat supported by our findings. In particular, we found that the FG and MOG exhibited a similar pattern to the STS such that there was a load effect on these regions during the encoding phase but not during the maintenance phase. Both the FG and MOG have been implicated in the processing of lower-level information prior to STS during BM perception (Grosbras et al., 2012; for review, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012). Second, it is possible that our design/analysis was not sensitive enough to reveal the involvement of the STS during the WM maintenance phase. Most previous neuroimaging studies investigating BM used scrambled or inverted PLDs as a baseline (for reviews, see Blake and Shiffrar, 2007; Pavlova, 2012; Thompson and Parasuraman, 2012; Troje, 2013), which are comprised of individual dot trajectories identical to those of intact PLDs but with global, spatiotemporal coherence to depict human action. This method is appropriate for locating specific brain regions specialized for processing the kinematics that define BM. In contrast, the current study used a fixation-baseline in order to observe all possible neural substrates underlying BM processing in WM. Although our method inevitably led to the identification of more activated regions, this does not necessarily mean that this method is more sensitive in revealing STS activation. Further studies are needed to elucidate this issue. If the STS indeed does not play a role in WM maintenance, it would shed an important light on understanding the function of STS and the rehearsal mechanisms of BM. Additionally, it should be noted that we did not find significant STS activations during the encoding phase in Experiment for defining ROIs. One possible reason is that the imperfect parameters adopted in that experiment (e.g., the unbalanced exposure time of the memory array between loads 2 and 4, and low power from the relatively few trials per subject) may have reduced the power to reveal STS activation. We found significant STS activation after controlling for these factors in our time-course analysis experiment, which is consistent with this possibility.

Additionally, due to the specific baseline we adopted, we found that the bilateral SPL and bilateral prefrontal cortex (i.e., BA9 and BA46) were significantly modulated by the memory load of BM in both the encoding and maintenance phase. These regions are routinely found to be involved in WM tasks. As to the SPL, previous WM studies using common visual stimuli found a strong load effect in this region, suggesting that the SPL plays a pivotal role in retaining visual information in WM (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2006; Gao et al., 2011). As to the prefrontal cortex, it plays a pivotal role in WM processing, including attention control and information manipulation (e.g., D’Esposito et al., 1995; Curtis and D’Esposito, 2003; Barbey et al., 2013). Therefore, we consider activation of the SPL and prefrontal cortex to reflect general visual processing of BM stimuli. From this perspective, this finding implies that perhaps there are two distinct rehearsal mechanisms for BM: One that is specific to BM information and employs the MNS system and another that employs common visual regions such as the SPL to retain general visual information. It will be interesting to verify this possibility and explore how these two distinct mechanisms interact in future studies.

Finally, the largely overlapping regions for WM encoding and maintenance in our current study adds to evidence that WM and perception recruit similar neural mechanisms. However, distinct from previous studies using static simple stimuli (e.g., Gabor patches; Ester et al., 2009; Harrison and Tong, 2009), the PLDs used are far more complex and convey rich social information. Therefore, it seems that the shared neural mechanism between perception and WM is a general mechanism in our brain. However, it is important to note that the current study also identified at least three regions dedicated to BM encoding, implying that there may be certain processing differences between the perception and WM (Offen et al., 2009).

Author Contributions

ZG, XL, JH, and MS initiated the conception and designed the study; XL, JH, YY, XW, and ZG, performed data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation; XL, JH, YY, MS, and ZG drafted the manuscript, XL, JH, YY, MS, WX, and ZG revised the manuscript together.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research is supported by NSFC (No.31271089, 31571119, and 61431015) and fundamental research funds for the central universities.

Footnotes

  1. ^ The retrieval phase was not analyzed in this study because activation during this phase are masked by the responses.
  2. ^ Digit rehearsal inevitably evoked certain brain activations that may confound the activations of BM. However, we required participants to rehearse the digits during the fixation-baseline in an attempt to minimize these brain activations when comparing to the results of the WM task.
  3. ^ www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
  4. ^ http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/
  5. ^ http://pcl.missouri.edu/bayesfactor

References

Allen, R. J., Hitch, G. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2009). Cross-modal binding and working memory. Vis. Cogn. 17, 83–102. doi: 10.1080/13506280802281386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, T., Puce, A., and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M., and Grafman, J. (2013). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 49, 1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, R., and Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 47–73. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics Toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., and Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 177–192. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, C. E., and D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., and Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 378, 279–281. doi: 10.1038/378279a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, X., Zhao, Y., Wu, F., Lu, X., Gao, Z., and Shen, M. (2015). Binding biological motion and visual features in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 850–865. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ester, E. F., Serences, J. T., and Awh, E. (2009). Spatially global representations in human primary visual cortex during working memory maintenance. J. Neurosci. 29, 15258–15265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J. P., Frith, C. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1994). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–610. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, T., Scholl, B. J., and McCarthy, G. (2012). Dissociating the detection of intentionality from animacy in the right posterior superior temporal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 32, 14276–14280. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0562-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Bentin, S., and Shen, M. (2015). Rehearsing biological motion in working memory: an EEG study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 198–209. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Li, J., Liang, J., Chen, H., Yin, J., and Shen, M. (2009). Storing fine detailed information in visual working memory–evidence from event-related potentials. J. Vis. 9, 1–12. doi: 10.1167/9.7.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Xu, X., Chen, Z., Yin, J., Shen, M., and Shui, R. (2011). Contralateral delay activity tracks object identity information in visual short term memory. Brain Res. 1406, 30–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Ye, T., Shen, M., and Perry, A. (2016). Working memory capacity of biological movements predicts empathy traits. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 468–475.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., and Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? Neuroimage 13, 775–785. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grosbras, M. H., Beaton, S., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Brain regions involved in human movement perception: a quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 431–454. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 711–720. doi: 10.1162/089892900562417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, E. D., Battelli, L., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Res. 45, 2847–2853.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Harrison, S. A., and Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature 458, 632–635. doi: 10.1038/nature07832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogeveen, J., Chartrand, T. L., and Obhi, S. S. (2015). Social mimicry enhances Mu-suppression during action observation. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2076–2082. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., and Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 849–878; discussion 878–937. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103

CrossRef Full Text

Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201–211. doi: 10.3758/BF03212378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keysers, C., and Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koshino, H., Carpenter, P. A., Minshew, N. J., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., and Just, M. A. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage 24, 810–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhlman, W. N. (1978). Functional topography of the human mu rhythm. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 44, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(78)90107-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacoboni, M., and Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 942–951. doi: 10.1038/nrn2024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. M., Gao, T., and McCarthy, G. (2014). Attributing intentions to random motion engages the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 81–87. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–281. doi: 10.1038/36846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, R., and Vogel, E. K. (2011). Shape and color conjunction stimuli are represented as bound objects in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 49, 1632–1639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 103–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman-Norlund, R. D., Ondobaka, S., van Schie, H. T., van Elswijk, G., and Bekkering, H. (2010). Virtual lesions of the IFG abolish response facilitation for biological and non-biological cues. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.005.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberman, L. M., Pineda, J. A., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The human mirror neuron system: a link between action observation and social skills. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 62–66. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberman, L. M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Bull. 133, 310–327. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Offen, S., Schluppeck, D., and Heeger, D. J. (2009). The role of early visual cortex in visual short-term memory and visual attention. Vision Res. 49, 1352–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlova, M. A. (2012). Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. Cereb. Cortex 22, 981–995. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, A., Troje, N. F., and Bentin, S. (2010). Exploring motor system contributions to the perception of social information: evidence from EEG activity in the mu/alpha frequency range. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 272–284. doi: 10.1080/17470910903395767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. Euro. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 129–154. doi: 10.1080/713752551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puce, A., and Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lon. B Biol. Sci. 358, 435–445. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670. doi: 10.1038/35090060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Morey, C. C., and Cowan, N. (2011). How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 324–330. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0055-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., and Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 225–237. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rypma, B., and D’Esposito, M. (1999). The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 6558–6563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.11.6558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. Brain 130, 2452–2461. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P., Wilson, S. M., Hagler, D. J., Bates, E., and Sereno, M. I. (2004). Point-light biological motion perception activates human premotor cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 6181–6188. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0504-04.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servos, P., Osu, R., Santi, A., and Kawato, M. (2002). The neural substrates of biological motion perception: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 12, 772–782. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.7.772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, M., Gao, Z., Ding, X., Zhou, B., and Huang, X. (2014). Holding biological motion information in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40, 1332–1345. doi: 10.1037/a0036839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, F., Pineda, J., and Cadenhead, K. S. (2011). Association of impaired EEG mu wave suppression, negative symptoms and social functioning in biological motion processing in first episode of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 130, 182–186. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, M. M., Pearson, N. A., and Pendleton, L. R. (1988). Movement and working memory: patterns and positions in space. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 40, 497–514. doi: 10.1080/02724988843000041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, M. M., and Pendleton, L. R. (1989). Working memory for movements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 41, 235–250. doi: 10.1080/14640748908402363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, M. M., and Pendleton, L. R. (1990). Space and movement in working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 42, 291–304. doi: 10.1080/14640749008401223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steel, K., Ellem, E., and Baxter, D. (2014). The application of biological motion research: biometrics, sport, and the military. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 78–87. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0659-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J., and Parasuraman, R. (2012). Attention, biological motion, and action recognition. Neuroimage 59, 4–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. C., Clarke, M., Stewart, T., and Puce, A. (2005). Configural processing of biological motion in human superior temporal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 25, 9059–9066. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2129-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, J. J., and Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428, 751–754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troje, N. F. (2013). “What is biological motion?: definition, stimuli and paradigms,” in Social Perception: Detection and Interpretation of Animacy, Agency, and Intention, eds M. D. Rutherford and V. A. Kuhlmeier (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 13–36.

Google Scholar

Ulloa, E. R., and Pineda, J. A. (2007). Recognition of point-light biological motion: mu rhythms and mirror neuron activity. Behav. Brain Res. 183, 188–194. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., and Belliveau, J. W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11656–11661. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191374198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., and Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 48, 564–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanrie, J., and Verfaillie, K. (2004). Perception of biological motion: a stimulus set of human point-light actions. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 625–629. doi: 10.3758/BF03206542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, E. K., and Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428, 748–751. doi: 10.1038/nature02447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., and Luck, S. J. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 92–114.

Google Scholar

Wheeler, M. E., and Treisman, A. M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 131, 48–64. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.48

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, J. N. (2007). Visual working memory for observed actions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 639–652. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, J. N. (2008). Visual memory for agents and their actions. Cognition 108, 522–532. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, J. N. (2011). A core knowledge architecture of visual working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 37, 633–633. doi: 10.1037/a0021935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y. (2002). Limitations of object-based feature encoding in visual short-term memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 458–468.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., and Chun, M. M. (2006). Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature 440, 91–95. doi: 10.1038/nature04262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., and Chun, M. M. (2007). Visual grouping in human parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 18766–18771. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705618104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: biological motion, working memory, point-light display, mirror neuron system, fMRI

Citation: Lu X, Huang J, Yi Y, Shen M, Weng X and Gao Z (2016) Holding Biological Motion in Working Memory: An fMRI Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:251. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00251

Received: 15 January 2016; Accepted: 16 May 2016;
Published: 01 June 2016.

Edited by:

Francesco Di Russo, University of Rome “Foro Italico”, Italy

Reviewed by:

Emily D. Grossman, University of California, Irvine, USA
Jie Yang, Macquarie University, Australia

Copyright © 2016 Lu, Huang, Yi, Shen, Weng and Gao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zaifeng Gao, zaifengg@gmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Download